Selective attention in cognitive and clinical research Jenny Yiend1, Andrew Mathews1 and Nelson Cowan2 1 MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK. 2 Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, USA. Address correspondence to: Jenny Yiend MRC-Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge, CB2 2EF, U.K. Fax: 01223 359062 Email: jenny.yiend@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk Look around and you can’t help but become aware that the external world contains far more information than can be taken in at any one instant. The concept of selective attention is the psychologist’s attempt to explain how our limited capacity processing system deals with this overload of information. As psychologists we assume that selective attention starts with competition between possible inputs (both internal and external) resulting in a selection of what is important together with a rejection of the remainder. The precise point at which this selection occurs is one topic that we discuss in the first of our companion chapters. The use of the phrase selective attention can however create the misleading impression that because there is one word there is also one process. On the contrary, when one source of information is to be selected at the expense of another, there are various processes that could be used to achieve this. For example one might maintain visual focus on a particular location; search through items looking for one and ignoring others; or elect to perform one entire task rather than another, more habitual, one. All require selection but may recruit different cognitive and neural processes. Together with most psychologists, we view performance in such situations as being a variable composite of bottom-up and top-down processes. Thus the capture of perceptual resources by a sensory event, such as the appearance of an object, may be influenced by higher control systems which can to some extent determine the outcome of competition arising from multiple inputs (e.g. Desimone & Duncan, 1995). An interesting point raised towards the end of our first chapter is that this capacity to effortfully screen out unwanted information varies across people. Individuals with high working memory capacity may be better able to inhibit unwanted information from capturing attention. Clearly this is also relevant to clinical research, where resources are often depleted, for example due to preoccupations with worrisome thoughts. Some of the findings of attention to emotional distracters may therefore represent inhibition failure due to reduced resources and impaired attentional control (Derryberry & Reed, 2002; Eysenck & Calvo, 1992) as well as the specific content of emotional distractors. The impetus to develop methods of assessing attention in clinical populations grew out of the intuition that people with particular emotional disorders deploy their attention in distinctive ways, and that this may be a factor in maintaining that disorder. Some clinical researchers (e.g. Beck, 1976) have long argued that emotional disorders are caused by maladaptive cognitive processes or schemata that influence how emotional information is encoded. Data showing that anxious patients, relative to control groups, attended more to threatening cues (e.g. Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1997) has been taken as support for this general view. Additional data suggesting that cues related to individual worries or concerns were particularly likely to capture attention was taken as further evidence that measures of attentional deployment may be useful for diagnostic purposes, or as a means of investigating etiological differences. In our first chapter we explore the range of methods most commonly used within cognitive psychology to assess selective attention. For clarity these are categorized into cueing, search, filtering, and multiple task paradigms. Cueing tasks describe methods in which a stimulus or event attracts attention to a particular location(s) and is followed by a target to be detected, with attention usually measured by the speed of participants’ response. This method also happens to be one that has been widely adopted by clinical researchers. Discussion of it thus forms a large part of our second chapter, which focuses on the application of selective attention tasks to clinical populations. In search tasks, participants must find and report on a particular target in an array of distractors. A specific example of such a task is the "face-in-the-crowd" method, again discussed at length in our clinical chapter, in which angry faces are easier to find than those with neutral expressions. Filtering tasks involve presenting targets and distractors together, testing participants’ ability to suppress or ignore the latter. The Stroop task is perhaps the most obvious example of this (the emotional Stroop when applied to clinical populations) but this is not discussed here as it is the topic of another chapter. Finally, in multiple tasks, people must allocate their limited processing capacity to meet more than one demand, as when having to report two sequential targets, in the ‘attentional blink’ method. Attending to one target takes time, meaning that another arriving too soon afterwards is often missed. We touch on this method only briefly in the clinical chapter as work on its application to emotional processing is only just beginning. In Table 1 we attempt to summarize the methods discussed in our two chapters into a single framework. It is hoped that this will provide the reader with both an overview of the chapters themselves and a guide for easy future reference. [INSERT TABLE ONE ABOUT HERE] We will end this introduction by emphasizing the point made in both chapters, that specific variations of method can often have powerful effects on results. These can only be understood from a sound knowledge of the many factors, theoretical and methodological, influencing attention and its interaction with emotion. We hope that together our chapters will assist the researcher in developing this understanding and will focus future work into domains of optimum theoretical and clinical relevance. Table 1. Framework describing selective attentional studies Category Method Single Cueing Location cueing Deployment of attention task (DOAT) Double Cueing Attentional (dot) probe Target among Distractors e.g. Face-inthe-crowd Search Eye movement monitoring Stroop Putative mechanisms Spatial shifting, engaging and disengaging Pre-emptive Recruitment of Attention by Significant Stimulus Spatial engagement, shifting, disengagement Key cognitive studies Serial/ parallel search; spatial engagement; distractor suppression Attention – Switching, Engagement, and Disengagement Spatial and non spatial interference; distractor suppression Shiffrin & Schneider (1977); Treisman & Gelade (1980) Rayner (1998) Filtering Dichotic Stimuli with Selective Listening Distractor suppression; auditory attention Flanker task Distractor suppression Posner (1980) Stroop (1935); Elliott, Cowan, & Valle-Inclan (1998); MacLeod (1991) Cherry (1953); Moray (1959); Conway, Cowan, and Bunting (2001) Eriksen and Eriksen (1974) Key clinical studies Fox et al. (2001); Yiend and Mathews (2001) Gotlib, McLachlan and Katz (1988); McCabe and Toman (2000) MacLeod, Mathews and Tata (1986); Mogg and Bradley (1999) Byrne and Eysenck (1995); Fox et al. (2000) Populations studied Analogue non clinical only Matthews and Antes (1992); Mogg, Millar and Bradley (2000) Mathews and MacLeod (1985); Williams, Mathews and MacLeod (1996) Mathews and MacLeod (1986) Analogue non clinical; GAD, spider phobics Mathews, May, Mogg and Eysenck (1990) Analogue non clinical; GAD Analogue non clinical; depressed, GAD Analogue non clinical; most clinical Unselected; analogue non clinical; social anxiety Analogue non clinical; most clinical GAD; PTSD Attentional blink Pre-emptive Recruitment of Attention by Significant Stimulus Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992; Reeves & Sperling, 1986 Dichotic Stimuli with Dual Monitoring Inattentional Blindness Need for Attentionswitching Broadbent (1957) Limit in Capacity of Attentional Focus Limit in Capacity of Visual Attention Cowan (2001); Rensink (2002) Simons (2000) Multiple tasks Multi-object Tracking Pylyshyn and Storm (1988) Barnard and Ramponi (personal communication); Mackintosh, Munafo and Barnard (personal communication) Analogue non clinical References Barnard, P., & Ramponi, C. (personal communication). Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York: International Universities Press. Broadbent, D.E. (1957). Immediate memory and simultaneous stimuli. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 1-11. Byrne, A., & Eysenck, M. W. (1995). Trait anxiety, anxious mood and threat detection. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 549-562. Cherry, E.C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25(5), 975-979. Conway, R.A., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M.F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 331-335. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87-185. Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225-236. Elliott, E.M., Cowan, N., & Valle-Inclan, F. (1998). The nature of cross-modal, color-word interference effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 761-767. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters on the identification of a target in a non search task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and peformance:The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409-434. Fox, E., Lester, V., Russo, R., Bowles, R. J., Pichler, A., & Dutton, K. (2000). Facial expressions of emotion: Are angry faces detected more efficiently? Cognition & Emotion, 14(1), 6192. Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 130(4), 681-700. Gotlib, I. H., McLachlan, A. L., & Katz, A. N. (1988). Biases in visual attention in depressed and nondepressed individuals. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 185-200. Mackintosh, B., Munafo, M., & Barnard, P. (personal communication). MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15-20. MacLeod, Colin M. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin. Vol 109(2) Mar 1991, 163-203. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 563-569. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1986). Discrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 131-138. Mathews, A., May, J., Mogg, K., & Eysenck, M. (1990). Attentional Bias in Anxiety Selective Search or Defective Filtering. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(2), 166-173. Matthews, G. R., & Antes, J. R. (1992). Visual attention and depression: Cognitive biases in the eye fixations of the dysphoric and the nondepressed. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 359-371. McCabe, S. B., & Toman, P. E. (2000). Stimulus exposure duration in a deployment-ofattention task: Effects on dysphoric, recently dysphoric, and nondysphoric individuals. Cognition & Emotion, 14(1), 125-142. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: a replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(6), 595-604. Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56-60. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25. Pylyshyn, Z.W., & Storm, R.W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3, 179-197. Raymond, J.E., Shapiro, K.L., & Arnell, K.M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849-860. Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372-422. Reeves, A., & Sperling, G. (1986). Attention gating in short term visual memory. Psychological Review, 93, 180-206. Rensink, R.A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 245-277. Shiffrin, R.M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127-190. Simons, D.J. (2000). Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 147 - 155. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662. Treisman, A.M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester: Wiley. Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section a-Human Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 665-681.