Iara Cury Anthropology of Development 28/04/2011 Culture of Poverty Is poverty destiny? With widespread poverty in developing countries, entrenched poverty in affluent countries and rising levels of inequality, the question is on the table. Possibly the worst nightmare of the development establishment, the suggestion that the poor might involuntarily forego the opportunity for education, employment, and social and political participation based on certain internalized “adaptive mechanisms” constitutes the concept of the “culture of poverty”. Originally articulated by Oscar Lewis, the idea has stirred much controversy, embedded as it is in the political dimension through a juxtaposition of anthropological, social, and policy problems. Despite its prolonged unpopularity as a topic of debate over the past few decades it has become clear that investigating the validity and implications of the culture of poverty remain imperative if the connection between culture, development and self-determination is to be better understood. On top of that, examining the concept of the culture of poverty places pressure on the continuing development of current social theories of agency, challenging us to better conceptualize the constraint of social and economic power structures on individuals. Through the publication of his ethnographic studies of poor families in Mexico and Puerto Rico, Oscar Lewis questioned the dichotomous and unrealistic view of the poor as “blessed, virtuous, upright, serene, independent, honest, kind, and happy” or “evil, mean, violent, sordid and criminal” (1994, p.270). Carried out in the 1950s and 60s, his fieldwork focused on the details of the life in poverty of “the great mass of peasants and urban dwellers of the underdeveloped countries who constitute almost eighty per cent of the world’s population” (1959, p.1). In particular, Lewis attempted to understand more about what poor people think and feel, how they react to external constraints and how they make decisions. What Lewis proposed was that poverty engendered a culture—that is, patterns of behavior—that transcended regional, rural-urban, and national differences. For him, these patterns were evidence of “similarities in family structure, interpersonal relations, time orientation, value systems, and spending patterns” (1994, p.270) of the poor. Ultimately, however, Lewis’ controversial contribution was to assert that this culture tended to perpetuate itself across generations. He stated that by the time poor children were “age six or seven they have usually absorbed the basic values and attitudes of their subculture and are not psychologically geared to take full advantage of changing conditions or increased opportunities” (p.271). Lewis’s work achieved prominence in a “heated political environment” (Small et. al, 2010, p.7), becoming entangled with the controversial effects of Daniel Patrick Moynihan’s 1965 report entitled “The Negro family: The case for national action”. These and other studies quickly attracted the indictment that they essentially exempted society from resolving social problems in that they blamed the poor for their poverty through their portrayed the culture of poverty as self-generating and self-perpetuating. Amid political battles over what entailed appropriate social policy, in 1968 Charles Valentine published the book, Culture and Poverty: Critique and Counter-Proposals, “remembered by many as the watershed “refutation of Lewis’s work” (Harvey and Reed, 1996, p.468). Valentine accused Lewis of promoting an ideology, through the concept of a culture of poverty, which amounted to “a lightly veiled Social Darwinism” (1969, p.181). For him, Lewis disproportionately focused on the disorganization and pathology of poor people rather than on severe structural problems, “assigning first priority to doing away with the ‘culture of poverty,’ not poverty itself” (idem). Following Valentine’s attack on the culture of poverty, many studies have been presented both in favor of and against the culture of poverty, particularly with respect to whether poor people have fundamentally different values from society. Valentine himself takes Elliot Liebow’s 1967 book, Tally’s Corner, as the prime example of how the poor do ascribe to middle class values. According to Valentine, Liebow discovers that the men he studied “experience their lives as devoid of success or satisfaction...precisely because they share the standards and criteria of the wider culture” (1968, p.97). Leo Howe’s 1998 article, “Where is the culture in the ‘culture of poverty’”, similarly makes the point that the poor do not ascribe to a different set of values by looking at the social and moral discourses surrounding the unemployed status of a group of Belfast men. He states, “the values and practices supposedly characteristic of the poor…are rather attributions imposed on them by other sections of society”; at the same time, “the category of the ‘deserving’, ‘reputable’ poor enables individuals to use these imposed cultural distinctions in their own personal interests” (1998, p.87). Along the same lines, Small et al., in a 2010 overview of the resurgence of the culture of poverty debate, cite Kathryn Edin and Maria Kefalas’ extensive study of low-income mothers. According to them, low-income women were apt to be single-mothers because “they held marriage in such high esteem that they were reluctant to marry until they believed that both they and their partners were emotionally and financially prepared” (2005, cited in Small et. al, 2010, p.12). In the same article, however, Small et al. make reference to Prudence Carter’s work on the experience of poor and working-class children at school. Her conclusion is that there exists a serious “mismatch between the cultural signals favored by middle-class institutions and those necessary for inclusion, identity and social support in poor urban communities (2005, cited in Small et al., 2010, p.19). Instead of arguing for the correspondence between lower-class and middle class values, Carter brings to fore the possibility that poor people might have to navigate through binary sets of values. Ulf Hannerz supports this position by proposing the usefulness of the “idea of a bicultural situation among the poor”, that is, the recognition that the poor are acutely aware of mainstream values, constantly comparing their own lives to that of their contemporaries in other segments of society (1969, p.186). As can be gathered from the diversity of cases and factors involved in discussions about the culture of poverty, thoroughly dealing with this controversial concept seems to be not relevant but rather essential to refining our understanding of both theoretical and pragmatic issues regarding poverty, social and cultural change, and issues of structure versus agency. Yet much has changed in the conceptualization of culture, poverty, and in anthropological theory in general since Oscar Lewis’s time. The assumption of social homogeneity, a static perspective of culture, and a lack of cultural reflexivity are some of the matters that anthropologists have attempted to amend, as will be considered below. One of the frequent points of attack from his critics was that Lewis essentialized the poor as well as the middle class, promoting a sense of homogeneity in values and attitudes where heterogeneity was the norm. Eleanor Leacock denounces the subtle degeneration of differences or variation to polar opposites (1971, p.25), questioning the validity of the clean and clear, yet thoroughly misleading, dichotomies constructed within the discipline to frame complex social problems. She manifests this by highlighting the many “variations according to the specific histories and circumstances of different regions, nationalities, religious groups, occupational groups, and residence styles (rural-urban)” present within and across cultures (1971, p.26), which do not fit a continuum or polarized model. In any case, the acceptance of the heterogeneity of social groups does not invalidate the investigation of patterns of responses to the structural constraints of poverty. If anything, the “significant variation in behavior, decision making, and outcomes among people living in seemingly identical structural conditions” (Small et al., 2010, p.9) provides rich ground for anthropological exploration. Small et al. also emphasize the extent to which anthropologists currently view culture as much more fluid, dynamic, interconnected, and heterogeneous than previous conceptualizations led us to imagine. Here, issues of the transmission and malleability of culture play their part. While Lewis argued that by the age of seven poor children had already irrevocably absorbed the culture of poverty of their parents, Leacock challenges this assessment on the grounds of new findings in developmental psychology (1971, p.13). For her, the “process of adaptation continues actively throughout an individual’s lifetime; individuals are not simply set in motion as children to respond automatically for the rest of their lives” (p.16). While Leacock’s statement might appear infinitely more attractive to believers in free will and individual agency, advances in cognitive science and developmental psychology do not yet (and might never) provide enough evidence to settle the score either way. Another blazing topic of criticism of the culture of poverty was that the “discourse of poverty, and the policies resulting from that discourse, are themselves cultural products, subject to the whims, predilections, prejudices, beliefs, attitudes, and orientations of policy elites” (Small et al, 2010, p.12). Granted that much of the global development establishment indeed suffers from ethnocentric, moralist, modernist and elite biases—a reality thoroughly chronicled by postmodern scholars and the post-development movement—it does not necessarily follow that we must subscribe to an academic nihilism. Even without an absolute and universal standard of objectivity, in fact, only after such a framework has been discarded, can we hope to begin to generate revised and revisable theories of the workings of power within the complex, specific contexts of world affairs. In fact, the question of perspective brings to fore the “problems” of rationality, one of which being the ever-present accusation or presumption that the poor act irrationally. Whereas to middle class spectators poor people’s attitudes and behavior might seem senseless, many of these practices, when seen within the context of people’s limited choices, make sense for survival (Goode, 1996, p.280). Indeed, these practices can be termed positive adaptive mechanisms inasmuch as they ease the pain of living poor, making the unbearable bearable step by step (Harvey and Reed, 1996, p.482). Yet allegations of the irrationality of “others” are not limited to lower class segments of Western society; anthropologists have extensively dealt with the ethnocentrism of standards of rationality, putting to rest the notion that non-Westerners think in different and deficient ways. Adding the social and cultural dimension to economic theories of the rational agent, scholars have revealed that human decision-making processes are always complex and multifaceted. One of the key problems of the last decades in anthropology has been how to theorize the imaginary triangle between society, culture and individual, and the debate on the culture of poverty seems to fit right in the domain of practice theories. Determining what is culture and social structure’s influence on the individual and how does the individual in turn influence these is one important side of the equation; understanding how power is enacted through social relations, practices and ideologies is the other side (Howe, 1998, p.87). Bourdieu’s concept of the habitus and Foucault’s treatment of discourses and the production of knowledge seem highly relevant for further research on the connection between poverty and culture. Overall, the classic questions of whether there is such a thing as the culture of poverty, how widespread is it and what are its implications remain valid points of inquiry alongside attempts at resolving the structural dimensions of poverty. Furthermore, anthropologists and other social scientists continue to focus on understanding the ways in which children are affected by poverty, particularly how permanent and incapacitating are these effects— physically, cognitively and emotionally. In fact, even with respect to adults certain difficult questions must be asked: is the culture of poverty a transitory, positive collective adaptive mechanism or does it represent a self-destructive cultural or psychological configuration? In light of the much wider context of development work over the past several decades, it has become clear that however one phrases it, culture cannot be divorced from social inequality and must be taken into account in the design and implementation of poverty relief and development programs. Interestingly enough, discussions about colonialism, globalization and the rise of a hegemonic Western economic and cultural system also have an underlying connection with Lewis’s formulation of the culture of poverty. According to him, “the culture of poverty develops when a stratified social and economic system is breaking down or is being replaced by another” (1996, p.271). Margaret Mead, in her foreword to Lewis’s Five Families, likewise speaks of a “malaise” originating from the replacement of “old, physically satisfactory, primitive existence” by an “unsatisfactory, impoverished existence” as people are drawn into the globalized world (1959, p.x). Can it be that by voluntarily or involuntarily contributing to the disintegration of local cultures Western powers might be escorting much of the population of the world into some sort of culture of poverty? Was Lewis right in equating a “poverty of culture” with the “culture of poverty”? Yet despite the profound significance and uncertainty of these questions, one thing is certain: we cannot but benefit from the belief that given their rightful share of opportunities poor people across the globe will surprise us with their response. Our part in the game is to cease to be oppressive through ethnocentric and elitist judgment and expectations. Bibliography Duvoux, N. 2010. The Culture of Poverty Reconsidered. La vie des idées.fr Goode, Judith (1996). "An Anthropological Critique of the Culture of Poverty". In G. Gmelch and W. Zenner. Urban Life. Long Grove: Waveland Press. Harvey, D. and M. Reed. 1996. The Culture of Poverty: An Ideological Analysis. Sociological Perspectives, Vol. 39, No. 4 pp.465-495. Howe, L. 1998. Where is the culture in the ‘culture of poverty’? Cambridge Anthropology 20 (1/2): 66-91). Leacock, E. (ed.) 1971. The culture of poverty: a critique. Simon & Schuster. Lewis, O. 1959. Five Families: Mexican case studies in the culture of poverty. Basic Books. Lewis, Oscar (1996 (1966)). "The Culture of Poverty". In G. Gmelch and W. Zenner, eds. Urban Life. Long Grove: Waveland Press. Reviews. 1969. Current Anthropology, Vol.10 No. 2/3, p.181-201. Small, M., D. Harding and M. Lamont. 2010. Reconsidering Culture and Poverty. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 2010 629:6. Valentine, C. 1968 Culture and Poverty: critique and counter-proposals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.