Sensitivity of Various PCC-Type Measures and Phoneme Acquisition Measures Ann Bosma Smit Kansas State University asmit@ksu.edu Klaire Mann Brumbaugh Bluebonnet Trails Early Childhood Intervention (Austin, TX) Barbara Weltsch Melanie Hilgers Kansas State University Kansas State University Presented at the Annual Convention of the Kansas Speech-Language-Hearing Association, September 25, 2015. Disclosure Statement: Dr. Smit, Ms. Brumbaugh, Ms. Weltsch, and Ms. Hilgers have no relevant financial or nonfinancial relationships to disclose. Introduction If a client is a preschooler with phonological disorder (PD), how much change can the SLP expect in such a child’s phonology in a given period of intervention? This is an important question, especially in the age of evidence-based practice. However, the answer is not readily apparent, and one possible reason is that there may be problems with the measures. Smit's (2014) review of PCC in conversation (PCCconv) and PCC in a word list (PCClist) found that studies that reported relatively high rates of improvement in PCC, for example, estimated gain in PCCconv of 1.0% per week of intervention or 1.5% per week in PCClist, did not maintain adequate measures against bias in transcription. In contrast, the studies that reported stringent precautions against bias on the part of transcribers reported minimal improvements over a period of intervention. What this meant as a practical matter was that in the studies where transcribers were completely “blinded,” PCClist and PCCconv were not sensitive to differences in outcomes between treatment groups and were possibly insensitive to changes over typical periods of therapy. The purpose of the study reported here was to examine the sensitivity of several measures of phonological improvement to changes during a relatively long period of intervention. This was a small feasibility study for randomized controlled trial in which two interventions were compared. The transcribers were completely blinded. Only the pre- and post-measures for the entire group of 13 children are reported here. Method Participants. The participants were 13 preschool-age children with phonological disorder who were referred by USD 383 for Speech Groups, a joint project of the school district and the Speech and Hearing Center. The project was approved by the K-State IRB and USD 383. The children were randomly assigned to one of two treatments. All the children passed a hearing screening. None required treatment for any concomitant disorder. Their mean age was 47.6 months (SD 6.79), their mean scaled score on the PLS-4 was 106.8 (SD 14.27), and their median on the SHAPE was at the 5th percentile (range 2nd – 23rd). The Assessments. Children were assessed at the beginning and end of 8 months of intervention while producing a 137-word list and engaging in spontaneous conversation during play. The samples were video-recorded using very high quality equipment. The measures were based on the video-recordings, and they were made from transcripts generated by 5 transcribers in external sites who were completely blinded to the children’s treatment group and to whether a sample was from the beginning or the end of a period of intervention. Transcription reliability was assessed in several ways and was considered adequate for these types of speech materials. Consonant Acquisition Measures. Using a consonant acquisition measure that we devised, we determined which consonants this group had not acquired at the beginning of treatment. We compared them to two other lists of “late” and “difficult” consonants and determined that /v θ ð s z ʃ ʧ ʤ l r/ can be considered Late and/or Difficult (L/D). In addition to the conventional PCC measures, we determined the L/D PCC for the word lists and the conversations, as well as a measure of how many of the L/D consonants had been acquired at the beginning and at the end of intervention. Results Table. Comparisons between the pre-treatment data and the post-treatment data for the whole group of 13 participants. For all data reported as percentages, an arcsine data transformation was used. Variable Pre-treatment mean (SD) Post-treatment mean (SD) t-value (related pairs) Probability Cohen’s (p) db PCClist 64.9% (10.89%) 74.4% (9.06%) 3.991 (df=12) .002 0.964 L/D PCClist 33.9% (12.64%) 48.9% (16.52%) 4.043 (df=12) .002 1.00 Acquired L/D consonants, list 1.5 (0.97) 3.1 (1.38) 4.170 (df=12) .001 1.290 PCCconv 67.3% (8.42%) 73.8% (8.97%) 2.640 (df=12a) .022 0.745 L/D PCCconv 39.1% (14.26%) 53.4% (15.17%) 2.832 (df=11a) .016 0.954 Acquired L/D consonants, conversation 2.9 (1.00) 4.2 (1.54) 3.218 (df=11a) .008 1.023 a Due to human error, no conversation was elicited from one of the children at Time 1. An estimated value was used for comparisons involving PCCconv, but the child’s data were not used in the other analyses of conversational data. b Calculated using the formula for repeated measures presented by Robey, 2004, p. 315. Findings and Discussion This is a very small data set, with only 13 children. However, three findings may be relevant for future research and practice: • First, intervention periods longer than 4-5 months may be necessary for conventional PCC to show changes. Over a period of 8 months, changes of 9.6-13.5% in conventional PCC can be expected. • Second, measures based on word lists and on Late/Difficult consonants appear to be more sensitive to change over a period of intervention than measures based on conversations or on the whole range of consonants. • Three, L/D acquisition measures are probably numerically too small to be useful with children who have moderate-to-severe PD due to floor effects. However, PCC for L/D consonants is likely to be free of ceiling and floor effects. Acknowledgements This project was supported by grants from Kansas State University and from the College of Human Ecology. We thank staff of USD 383 for their assistance and cooperation with this study. The parents and children who participated in the study have our deepest thanks, as do the dedicated student clinicians and the many students who helped to analyze the data. Many thanks to the 5 external transcribers whose contributions were so invaluable: Erica Bates, Julie Erb, Fanisha Esfield, Sarah Moll, and Carly Thibault. References Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gierut, J. A., Elbert, M., & Dinnsen, D. A. (1987). A functional analysis of phonological knowledge and generalization learning in misarticulating children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 462-479. Girolametto, L., Steig Pearce, P., & Weitzman, E. (1997). Effects of lexical intervention on the phonology of late talkers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 338-348. Robey, R. R. (2004). Reporting point and interval estimates of effect-size for planned contrasts: fixed within effect analyses of variance. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29, 307-341. Rvachew, S., Nowak, M., & Cloutier, G. (2004). Effect of phonemic perception training on the speech production and phonological awareness skills of children with expressive phonological delay. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 250-263. Shriberg, L. D. (1993). Four new speech and prosody-voice measures for genetics research and other studies in developmental phonological disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 105140. Shriberg, L. D., & Kwiatkowski, J. (1982). Phonological disorders III: A procedure for assessing severity of involvement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 256-270. Smit, A. B. (2014, November). PCC-type measures in phonological intervention, including a feasibility study. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Orlando, FL. Smit, A. B., & Hand, L. (1997). Smit-Hand Articulation and Phonology Evaluation (SHAPE). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool Language Scale (4th Ed.) (PLS-4). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.