Cognitive development in children with spastic forms of cerebral palsy. Louise Bøttcher, PhD Boettcher@dpu.dk (corresponding author) Department of Education, Aarhus University Tuborgvej 164 2400 Copenhagen NV Denmark 1 Cognitive development in children with spastic forms of cerebral palsy. Louise Bøttcher, Cand Psych, PhD Abstract The aim of the paper is to present a theoretical framework that relates the cognitive development of children with spastic cerebral palsy (CP) to their learning and participation over time. Results from selected recent studies of the development of general cognitive functioning and specific cognitive functions in children with CP disagree and highlight a need to address how the initial brain lesions associated with CP impacts on specific cognitive functions and their interaction over time. Through the inclusion of the neuroconstructionist paradigm, the initial brain lesion of children with spastic CP is reconceptualised as a neurobiological constraint on the child’s interactions with its environment. An early brain lesion, e.g. in the white matter, interferes with normal information processes that are the foundation of gradual modularisation and affect the brain and the development of both lower and higher cognitive functions in a wide-spread manner. Furthermore, even though cognitive development builds on neurological possibilities, the development of cognition arises from the child’s participation in organised learning activities over time. It will be argued that in order to understand the cognitive development of children with spastic CP, we need to include how their learning is supported through their participation in organised learning activities in school. A model will be proposed that frames how learning activities afford and develop particular cognitive activities and create developmental possibilities that feedback on the child’s individual cognitive activity and neural development. Introduction 2 The aim of this paper is to present a theoretical framework that relates the cognitive development of children with spastic cerebral palsy (CP) to their learning and participation over time. The paper will begin with a presentation of three recent studies of general intellectual development (IQ), followed by a presentation of four studies of the development of specific cognitive functions in children with (spastic) CP. The aim is to not to give a complete picture of studies of cognitive development in children with spastic CP, but to point to differences and contrasts within the material. What becomes apparent is that a developmental approach is necessary. Therefore, the remaining part of the paper will include concepts from two developmental approaches; the neuroconstructionist approach and the cultural historical approach in order to create a framework in which to connect knowledge of neurobiological development with studies of the participation of children with spastic CP. Studies of general cognitive functioning. Studies of general intellectual development (IQ) can only be used to point to overall trends in cognitive development. Three recent studies have been included (Table 1). Table 1: Selected recent studies of general intellectual development in children with CP All three studies follow a longitudinal design, although differences can be found in the follow-up. The participants are all children with unilateral spastic CP and their cognitive functioning has been measured with age-appropriate versions of the Wechsler scales. However, the three studies differ in their results. Muter et al. (1997) found that IQ remained stable. The main objective against the study is the restricted age range of the children participating in the study and the short follow-up interval. A significant part of cognitive development takes place after the age of 8 and the study is not 3 informative about this. In contrast, Levine et al. (2005) found that IQ declined with age. In their study, general cognitive functioning was measured at two data-points: one before the age of seven and one after. The cut-off at the age of seven was chosen because a previous cross-sectional study had found a decline in IQ beginning around this age. The time between the two data collections were quite wide-ranging, between 1½ year and 15. Comparing level of performance before and after the age of seven, a decline in IQ was seen with increasing age, especially for those children with smaller lesions and higher IQ early on. A decline was seen regardless of seizure status, lesion laterality and was similar for verbal and performance IQ. The authors suggest that early lesions in the larger range initially have a greater immediate impact on cognitive functioning whereas the impact of smaller lesions might not show until later on. Finally, the third study by Gonzales-Monge et al. (2009) found that the mean (full-scale) IQ was close to the normal reference score at all three times. No trend was found towards a change of full IQ with age. However, a decline in PIQ was found. The three studies of general cognitive functioning illustrate the disagreement about whether IQ is stable or declines in children with unilateral spastic CP. The difference between Muter et al. (1997) and Levine et al. (2005) can be explained by the difference in age of the participating children. The children participating in the study by Muter et al. (1997) might have been too young to show the decline found by Levine et al. (2005). However, this cannot explain the difference between the two last studies. Studies in specific cognitive functions Studies in general cognitive functioning can only point to general trends in cognitive development. The general IQ measure may cover up developmental increases or decreases in specific cognitive 4 functions that might be more informative about the developmental trajectories of cognitive development. To examine developmental trajectories of specific cognitive functions, four recent studies have been selected for further presentation (Table 2). All but one followed a longitudinal design, mainly with children with bilateral spastic CP, although a couple of studies also included children with other types of CP. Table 2: Selected recent studies of the development of specific cognitive functions in children with (spastic) CP White & Christ (2005) analysed the development of verbal learning and inhibition using a crosssectional design. Initial learning and retention of information were at the level of the control group when controlling for general verbal ability. However, learning over repeated trials, strategic processing (spontaneous use of semantic clustering) and inhibition (interference/intrusion errors of list B on the retention of list A) were below the performance of the control group. Analysis included the interaction between age and group and revealed that the difference between the CP group and the control group was more pronounced in the younger than the older children in the CP group. The study pointed to a developmental delay. Opposite to what was found in studies of general cognitive functioning, younger children showed greater impairment compared to same-aged peers and older children revealed a catch-up. In contrast, the two papers from Dahlgren Sandberg (2001; 2006), where the second represent a follow-up of the first one, point to a decline in the development of both literacy skills and working memory. Even though the children were behind same-aged peers at T1 and T2, they showed 5 progress in literacy skills. However, the follow-up study revealed that the progress in the first study was follow by a developmental arrest between T2 and T3. The last study in Table 2 by Jenks et al (2009) included a larger group of children, most of whom had bilateral spastic CP. In this study the development of arithmetic skills was followed through measurement at five data points. The support of executive functions and working memory on arithmetic skills was included by the measurement of different aspects of executive functions and working memory at one data point only, so the study is only informative about the development of arithmetic skills, not the development of executive functions and memory. Impairment of arithmetic skills was persistent across all data points and was found to be related to impairments in working memory and executive functions (updating, visuo-spatial sketchpad, shifting) (Jenk et al. 2009). The continuance of arithmetic problems might suggest persistent impairments in working memory and executive functions, at least in the age group in the study. Taken together, the four studies reveal different developmental trajectories of specific cognitive functions. In addition, several of the studies suggest that different patterns of interaction between specific cognitive functions might explain the cognitive developmental trajectories found in children with spastic CP. Previously, phonological awareness and literacy have been found to be related and the theoretical assumption has been that phonological awareness supports the development of literacy. However, this supportive relationship was not found in children with spastic CP (with accompanying motor speech problems) (Dahlgren Sandberg, 2009). The hypothesis is that impairments in speech, working memory or the use of strategies for spelling prevented the children from making use of their phonological abilities the way children usually do in order to learn how to spell. 6 Theoretical approaches to neurocognitive development Results from different studies paint a complex picture of the cognitive development and call for a need to address the question; how are we to make sense of the way the initial brain lesions affect the developmental trajectory of the child with CP, including cognitive development? It is clear that not one single trajectory exists. We need to include theories of neural development that embrace the interaction between low level and higher level processes, early and later development and the environment where the development of cognitive functions take place. As Annette Karmiloff-Smith has stated: “Development itself is the key to understand developmental disorders” (KarmiloffSmith, 1998). From this neuroconstructive point of view, neural development is conceptualised as a process of gradual modularisation where neural information processing over time straightens particular synapses and creates cognitive modules, which is then mirrored in more effective cognitive functioning. A central biological mechanism is synaptic pruning: the discarding of excess synapses. At the same time as particular synapses are straightened, excess synapses are discarded over a prolonged period of time. Both straightening of useful synapses and pruning of excess synapses builds on cognitive information processes to activate certain synapses and reveal others as redundant and dispensable (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998). White matter lesions, which are common in children with spastic CP, give rise to inefficient information processing and impact on the quality of neural messages between different brain regions (Anderson, 2007). White matter lesions might interfere with normal processes of brain development because of damage to normal information processing circuits in the periventricular regions of the brain, whose functionality is of central importance to the fine-tuning of brain functioning and the development of higher level cognitive functions. Thus early low level impairments might result in impairments in higher level cognitive functions later on. It has also been suggested that white matter lesions might alter normal patterns and timing of myelination (Anderson, 2007). 7 Taken together, the understanding of the cognitive development of children with CP needs to include mechanisms of neural development. A central point of neuroconstructionism is that the cognitive profile of a child is the systemic result of interaction between different brain regions and processes and the structuring effect of the environment (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998). The child actively seeks out stimuli in the environment and through its processing of stimuli sculpts its own neural system. The initial brain lesion functions as a biological constraint on the child’s interactions with the environment. This is readily apparent in children with CP, whose motor impairment and often additional perceptual and cognitive impairments impacts on their possibilities for interaction with the environment. Neuroconstructive development as a cultural-historically framed process in specific learning practices The systemic interaction between the child and its environment enables the child to change its way of thinking and acting towards more advanced levels of functioning. This process by which the child interacts with its environment and changes its cognitive functioning is at the same time a description of what in the cultural-historical theoretical perspective would be conceptualised as learning. Both learning and cognitive development denotes processes, where the child changes its way of thinking and acting towards higher and more advanced ways and do so not only in one instance, but across situations. According to this approach, learning is a socially and culturally structured process, where the instructions, support and other scaffolding processes enable the child to solve cognitive challenges it would not be able to solve on its own. This is what Vygotsky conceptualised as the zone of proximal development. 8 “The crucial characteristic of instruction is the fact that instruction creates the zone of proximal development, i.e. elicits in the child, promotes, and brings to movement a number of internal developmental processes, which at the present time are available for the child only in the sphere of relations with the people around and in joint action with peers, but which later, undergoing an internal course of development, become then the internal property of the child himself.” (Vygotsky, 1960, from Valsiner,1997 p. 149). Vygotsky anchored a child’s zone of proximal development in its current mental development. The instruction of the adult or the imitation by the child only lead to cognitive development if the child is able to understand the cognitive exercise it is instructed in or is imitating. From this perspective, learning is a distributed process by which the child’s cognitive functions are developed through interactions with others, especially more competent others (Vygotsky, 1998). The important point is that in order to understand the cognitive development of children with CP, we need to look at both the individual neurobiological and neuropsychological endowments of the child and how the learning and cognitive development of that child is supported by his or hers participation in school and other settings for learning. After all, learning is a cultural and social activity, often situated in schools, where it is organised by adults according to their goals and aims with the children. In order to connect the different aspects that are important to understand the cognitive development of children with spastic CP, the following model is proposed. The model suggests that cognitive development arises from developmental dynamics that include the children’s’ cerebral lesions, their individual cognitive functioning and the way their learning activities are organised. Figure 1: Cognitive development as processes between the brain, cognitive activity and child participation 9 Knowledge about the child’s brain lesion and its impact on the neural system and neural processes is indispensable in order to understand its cognitive development. The cognitive functions are supported by functional neural networks and through feedback processes, the use of neural networks in cognitive activity develop the neural system to the extent that is possible at the given time and circumstances. The brain lesion impacts on the left spiral in the model as neurobiological constraints on the ability of the neural systems and processes to serve the child’s cognitive processes and, through the feedback process, the possibilities for further development of the neural systems and processes in the brain. Child’s participation in settings affording learning and cognitive development covers that the single child’s cognition takes place within a cultural activity system, e.g. a school, in which the child’s cognitive processes are supported, transformed, enabled or constrained through the concrete organisation of learning activities. Different types of learning activities call for different cognitive activity of the child. Differences in amount of time spent on a subject, e.g. arithmetic, have been shown to have an impact on the child’s development of arithmetic abilities (Jenks et al. 2007). The relation between the child’s cognitive functioning and the child’s environment functions with a spiral-like dynamic, in which the child’s cognition is supported or constrained by the practice framework of cognition, and the child’s participation in different activities affords and develops particular cognitive activities and processes and possibly further impact on the development of neural systems and processes in the left spiral. In conclusion, what is proposed is that in order to understand the cognitive development of children with CP, it is necessary to consider how cognitive development arise from the interaction between two developmental spirals: one between neural structures and processes on the one hand and 10 cognition on the other; and another one between cognition and activities that afford different types of child (cognitive) activities. Because cognition figures in both spirals, the two developmental spirals are parts of the same developmental process. And in practice, they work in parallel by constantly constraining and enabling each other. Together neurobiological, individual and social processes create developmental possibilities or social constraints that feedback on the child’s individual cognitive activity and development. 11 References Anderson, V. (2007), “Childhood white matter injuries: What are the issues?” Dev neuropsychol 32(2), 619-623. Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (2001), “Reading and spelling, phonological awareness, and working memory in children with severe speech impairment: A longitudinal study”, Augment Altern Commun 17, 11-26. Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (2006), “Reading and spelling abilities in children with severe speech impairments and cerebral palsy at 6, 9, and 12 years of age in relation to cognitive development: A longitudinal study”, Dev Med Child Neurol 48, 629-634. Gonzalez-Monge, S., Boudia, B., Ritz, A., Abbas-Chorfa, F., Rabilloud, M., Iwaz, J., Bérand, C. (2009), ”A 7-year longitudinal follow-up of intellectual development in children with conginital hemiplegia”, Dev Med Child Neurol 51, 959-967. Jenks, K. , de Moor, J., van Lieshout, E., Maathuis, K., Keus, I., Gorter, J. (2007), ”The effect of cerebral palsy on arithmetic accuracy is mediated by working memory, intelligence, early numeracy, and instruction time”, Dev Neuropsychol 32, 861-879. Jenks, K., de Moor, J., van Lieshout, E. (2009), “Arithmetic difficulties in children with cerebral palsy are related to executive function and working memory”, J Child Psychol and Psychiatr 50, 824-833. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998), ”Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders”, Trends Cogn Sci, 2(19), 389-398. Levine, S., Kraus, R., Alexander, E., Suriyakham, L., Huttenlocher, P. (2005), “IQ decline following early unilateral brain injury: A longitudinal study”, Brain Cogn 59, 114-123. Muter, V., Taylor, A., Vargha-Khadem, F. (1997), “A longitudinal study of early intellectual development in hemiplegic children”, Neuropsychologia 35, 289-298. 12 Valsiner, J. (1997), Culture and the development of children’s action. A theory of human development, New York, John Wiley & sons. Vygotsky, L. (1998). The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Vol 5. Child Psychology. New York, Plenum Press. White, D., Christ, S. (2005), “Executive control of learning and memory in children with bilateral spastic cerebral palsy”, JINS 11, 920-924. 13 Table 1: Selected recent studies of general intellectual development in children with CP Paper Research design N Type of CP Age of children Findings in regard of developmental trajectories Muter et al., 1997 Longitudinal 38 Unilateral T1: 3-6 T2: 5-8 IQ remained stable. Levine et al., 2005 Longitudinal 15 Unilateral T1: 4-6 T2: 7-21 Decline in IQ with increasing age. Gonzalez-Monge et al., 2009 Longitudinal 32 Unilateral spastic T1: 4 T2: 7 T3: 14 FIQ stable, PIQ decline. Neg. effect of epilepsy and birth prematurity 14 Table 2: Selected recent studies of the development of specific cognitive functions in children with (spastic) CP Paper Cognitive domain N Type of CP Age of children Findings in regard of developmental trajectories White & Christ, 2005 Memory and executive function 16 Bilateral spastic CP T1: 6-16 (Crosssectional study) Dahlgren Sandberg, 2001 Reading and spelling, phonological awareness, working memory IQ, literacy skills, phonological abilities, working memory 7 Dystonia or spastic CP, all with severe speech impairments T1: 6 T2: 10 Impairment in verbal learning and inhibition more pronounced in younger children, suggesting a developmental delay in executive function. Slower development of reading and spelling. Auditory memory behind peers at T1 and T2. Almost normal developmental trajectory of phonological awareness. 6 Dystonia or spastic diplegia, all with severe speech impairments T1: 6 T2: 9 T3: 12 Arithmetic skills, executive function, working memory 57 Mainly unilateral or bilateral spastic CP (95 %), ataxic CP (5 %) T1: 7 T2: 7-8 T3:8 T4:8-9 T5: 9 Dahlgren Sandberg, 2006 Jenks et al., 2009 The development seen in Dahlgren Sandberg (2001) was followed by an arrest between T2 and T3. Phonological awareness not predictive of development in literacy as in normal development. Deficits in executive function and working memory at were related to arithmetic difficulties that were persistent over time. 15 Figure 1: Cognitive development as processes between the brain, cognitive activity and child participation Developmental time Develops… Develops... … Neural systems and processes Serves… Child cognitions used in... Child activity in settings affording distributed cognition 16