Running head: FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY The Uses and Functions of Formulaic Sequences in the Development of Speech Fluency in English as A Second Language (ESL) Jing Fang (Elise) The Pennsylvania State University APLNG 482Y 2013 Fall FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 2 The Uses and Functions of Formulaic Sequences in the Development of Speech Fluency in English as A Second Language (ESL) Introduction In the past few decades, linguistic scholarship about formulaic sequences has significantly increased. Educators and researchers interested in English language learning seem to be specifically focused on the relationship between formulaic language and the development of the fluency in second-language (L2) speech. As early as 1983, Pawley and Syder identified that formulaic sequences present the most challenging phase in language acquisition for L2 speakers. For advanced learners, the major problem can lie in the production of perfectly grammatical utterances that are simply not the preferred idiomatic sequences used by native speakers. In late 1900s,Wray indicated that a speech community has a ‘preferred way’ of saying something. For speakers within a particular community, certain word-strings are more likely to be selected as the default expression of a given idea for they are prioritized during processing. Other researchers such as Wood continued to investigate the link between the use of formulaic language and the development of speech fluency in both children and adults learners of L2. Wood (2006) conducted a longitudinal investigation that focuses on the role of formulaic sequences in L2 acquisition, particularly in the development of speech fluency. He concluded that formulaic sequences can facilitate fluency in speech by making pauses shorter and less frequent, and allowing longer runs of speech between FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 3 pauses. In this article, various definitions of formulaic sequence and speech fluency will be examined, and then the research evidence of the role of formulaic sequences in L2 acquisition and production will be identified, particularly the relationship between the use of formulaic sequences and the development of L2 speech fluency. Furthermore, teaching implications are investigated, with particular emphasis on providing more opportunities for input and fostering interaction to facilitate the acquisition of a repertoire of formulaic language. Speech fluency The existing literature on speech fluency strongly suggested that there has been a considerable agreement on determining the types of measurable temporal variables to be tracked in speech. These are rate of speech, measured as syllables uttered per minute, amount of pauses and the length of runs, measured as number of syllables uttered between pauses. It has provided reliable measures with which to determine fluency in both first-language (L1) and L2 speech production. Studies have shown that pause time in L1 speech is generally shorter than that in L2 speech, and L2 pause time and frequencies reduce over time. In studies of uses of formulaic sequences in the foundation of fluency (Pawley & Syder, 1983; Wood, 2006), and research associating with spoken fluency and its development (Wood, 2009), it has shown that native speakers or highly fluent L2 learners FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 4 most often have longer runs and shorter pause time, which are usually considered as key indicators of fluency. It is likely attributed to fluent speakers having a larger repertoire of formulaic sequences to help balance skills, attention, and planning during speech. Pawley and Syder (1983) also suggested that mastery of a body of lexicalized sentence stems plays a direct role on achieving fluency. Native speakers do not produce speech word-for-word, rather they focus on rhythm and variety, combine memorized chunks and produce creative connections of lexical strings as well as concepts. It seems likely that fluency is greatly enhanced by the control of large numbers of formulaic sequences. Formulaic sequences Draw upon many researchers, definitions of formulaic sequences considerably focus on the notion that they are multi-word units or strings of language, and they are stored in and retrieved from long-term memory as if they were single lexical units in spontaneous speech production. As early as 1983, Pawley and Syder asserted that only minority of spoken clauses is novel, and that memorized chunks form a high proportion of the speech of everyday conversation. They observed that people tend to take advantage of standard and predicable phrases to express a certain meaning, rather than make use of grammatically but communicatively unlikely ways of expressing the same meaning or function. In early 1990’s, Nattinger and DeCarrico reinforced the concept of formulaic sequences, and defined formulaic sequences as lexical phrases that pragmatically FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 5 specialized in discourse functions and served as a sort of bridge between lexis and grammar. They stated that formulaic sequences cover a large proportion of the types of utterances produced in a language. Furthermore, a great deal of variety and diversity in formulaic sequences are identified in literature. In 2000, Wray concluded two main explanations for formulaicity in language-- saving processing resources and achieving interactional functions. Afterwards, Wray and Perkins continued identifying three major types of socio-international function for formulaic sequences. These are manipulation of others, asserting separate identity and asserting group identity, all of them relate to aspects of how we want others to treat or view us. Researchers have also noted that formulaic sequences help learners cope with the complexity of various social situations, make orderly and clear communication, and develop a sense of group identity. Formulaic sequences in facilitating fluency in L2 speech Due to lack of the necessary sensitivity and adequate experience of the new language, L2 learners may have difficulty figuring out the most idiomatic ways of expressing a particular idea even though they can come up with various grammatically right lexical phrases. Gaining full command of a new language requires L2 learners to have a good awareness of the native speakers’ preferences for certain sequences of words in communication. As Nattinger and DeCarrico (1983) pointed out, the use of formulaic speech is not just a language learning strategy that no longer be used once language FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 6 ‘chunks’ have been learned, rather, it is an essential component of both fluent reception and production. In a research article by Wood (2002), he made a step further in exploring the relationship between speech fluency and the use of large numbers of lexical phrases and sentence stems. He found out that a large proportion of the most familiar concepts and speech acts can be expressed formulaically, and if a speaker can pull these readily from memory as wholes, fluency is enhanced. This can be attributed to reduced amount of planning, processing as well as encoding for it gives speakers time to pay attention to other tasks while speaking, such as generating specific lexical items, planning the next unit of discourse, syntactic processing of novel pieces, and so on. Furthermore, there is some empirical evidence that formulaic sequences have functions in facilitating the development in L2 speech fluency. Researchers such as Wood conducted a series of studies that mainly focus on this issue. In his longitudinal study that investigates the close link between use of formulaic sequences and L2 fluency in spontaneous spoken narrative retelling, Wood (2006) collected narrative speech samples monthly from a group of mixed L1 leaners and adopted native speaker judgment in analyzing data. The results showed that the use of formulaic sequences played a clear role in facilitating the development of speech fluency over time. In particular, the participants employed a range of uses and functions of the sequences to extend the length of runs between pauses- a key indicator of increased fluency. Afterwards, five categories of FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 7 fluency-enhancing uses of formulaic sequences were categorized and identified. These are repetition of a formula to extend a run of speech; use of multiple formulas to extend a run; reliance on repetition of one formula; use of self-talk and filler formulas; and use of formulas as rhetorical devices in spoken narrative. These categories of the use of formulaic sequences expand somewhat on those of Wray (2002, p.97), who determined four main functions of formulas in aiding speech production. They include enabling the manipulation of information; allowing a continuing flow of speech to occur while the conscious mind is focused elsewhere in communication; shortening the processing route of speech by bypassing the need for assemble of components or use of short-term memory; and signaling the organization of spoken discourse. Wray’s summary of the function of formulaic sequences in speech production mainly focuses on their roles in controlling the flow of information, allowing time for mental processing of other aspects of speech and signaling the organization of speech. Whereas, in Wood’s categories, the functions of self-talk and extending the length of runs of fluent speech are also emphasized. Wood (2009) continued exploring the relationship between uses of formulaic sequences and L2 speech performance. He conducted a study that mainly focused on the effects of focused instruction of formulaic sequences and fluency on the performance of a Japanese learner of English. In this study, spontaneous narratives of this leaner were collected in an intensive study abroad situation. The use of formulaic sequences before FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 8 and after a six-week fluency development workshop that included a focus on native speaker models were analyzed. Results indicate a strong increase in speech fluency of this learner after six weeks of focused instruction. It shows that the L2 learner were able to borrow formulaic sequences from the workshop models, and fit them into her own narrative quite effectively, and eventually stored them as her own repertoire. Teaching implications Formulaic sequences have great significance in accomplishing the production of fluent language, which encourages researchers to explore ways to incorporating formulaic sequences into classroom pedagogy. Repeated exposure to formulaic sequences over time would encourage learners to achieve a certain level of comfort with natural expression in English. Therefore, introducing classroom activity with special attention paid to the formulaic sequences being used will facilitate the development of speech fluency and achievement of particular pragmatic goals. Various researches introduce formulaic sequences into language teaching in both principled way and practical way. In Wray (2000), three attempts to introduce formulaic language into second language teaching were examined. The assumptions underlying the three syllabuses: Willis (1990), Nattinger and DeCarrico (1992) and Lewis (1993) are compared and evaluated. Willis (1990) discussed certain words figures and demonstrated the most frequent usage of certain words. Nattinger and DeCarrico (1992) were interested FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 9 in the interactional functions of common formulaic sequences and suggested to use formulaic sequences in teaching conversation. Lewis (1993) emphasized the uses and functions of formulaic sequences that can help learners build up a large vocabulary. This article constitutes a valuable introduction and calls for further research to the potential application of formulaic sequences to second-language pedagogy. The authors provide useful suggestions for incorporating formulaic sequences into curriculum both in terms of spoken discourse and written discourse. In a research article be Wood (2002), he introduced two practical types of tasks that would help to facilitate awareness of the role of formulaic sequences: shadowing and dictogloss. Shadowing involves learners in close imitation of how fluent or native speaker performs while listening to speech on tape. During shadowing, learners are required to repeat the phrases until they have mastered them. For classroom teaching, choosing texts rich in formulaic sequences of particular relevance to leaner needs for shadowing is a good way for awareness raising and production in real-time speech. Dictogloss is also an awareness-raising activity in which leaners listen to a short text with pauses of several seconds between sentences or phrases. Afterwards, learners are required to write down content words and then work in teams to reconstruct the entire text. This activity offers learners a chance to focus on how formulaic sequences are chunked and fit into the flow of discourse in real speech. Whereas, it seems difficult for L2 learners to have the sensitivity and automation of FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 10 using formulaic sequences in real-life experience by simply expose to input in classroom. In particular, dealing with the spoken language, interaction would seem to be key to facilitating acquisition of formulaic sequences. Therefore, interactive tasks such as small-group and pair activity are encouraged to enable learners to deliver more natural utterances and accomplish communicative goals together. Interaction with native speakers is another effective way to assist learners in gaining facility with formulaic sequences. Native speakers can offer feedback to L2 learners in terms of the appropriateness and effectiveness of their use of formulaic sequences in certain contexts, which allows them to be aware of how formulas are pieced together in discourse. Conclusion Studies of speech fluency show that formulaic sequences paly an essential in maintaining smoothness and speed of real-time speech. Learners can be exposed to models of most idiomatic ways of using formulaic sequences, and move towards the development of their L2 speech fluency in real-life situations. Given the significant role of formulaic language in acquisition and production in L2, it is considerably vital to each formulaic sequences and facilitate their acquisition more directly in the classroom. There is still a long way to go to consolidate the evidence presented thus to use the knowledge to further support classroom language pedagogy. More clear definition and criteria as well as classification of what constitute formulaic sequences should be discussed in further studies and research, especially formulaic sequences in L2 speech. FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 11 Reference Lewis, M. (1993). The lexical approach. Hove: Teacher Training Publications. Nattinger, J. R., & DeCarrico, J.S. (1992). Lexical phrases and language teaching. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Pawley, A., & Syder, F. H. (1983). Two puzzles for linguistic theory: Nativelike selection and nativelike fluency. In J.C. Richards & R.W. Schmidt (Eds.), Language and communication (pp. 191-226). New York: Longman. Willis, D. (1990). The lexical syllabus: A new approach to language teaching. London: HarperCollins. Wood, D. (2002). Formulaic Language in Acquisition and Production: Implications for Teaching. TESL Canada Journal, 20 (1), 1-15. Wood, D. (2006). Uses and functions of formulaic sequences in second language speech: An exploration of the foundations of fluency. Canadian Modern Language Review, 63, 13-33. Wood, D. (2009). Effects of focused instruction of formulaic sequences on fluent expression in second language narratives: A case study. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 12 (1), 39-57. Wray, A. (1999). Formulaic language in learners and native speakers. Language Teaching, 34 (4), 213-231. FORMULAIC SEQUENCES IN L2 SPEECH FLUENCY 12 Wray, A. (2000). Formulaic sequences in second language teaching: Principle and practice. Applied Linguistics 21 (4), 463-489. Wray, A. (2002). Formulaic language and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Wray, A, & Perkins, M.R (2000). The functions of formulaic language: An integrated model. Language and Communication, 20, 1-28.