Magical Causal Explanations 1 Running Head: Magical Causal Explanations Plausible Explanations: Magical Causal Explanation in Preschoolers Caitlin M. Clark Cornell University, College of Human Ecology Author Notes Caitlin M. Clark, Department of Human Ecology, Cornell University. This study was completed as a Senior Honor’s Thesis in the Department of Human Ecology. Funding was provided by the Marjorie A. Corwin Undergraduate Research Fellows Endowment for Faculty Excellence. This project would not have been possible without the guidance and advice given by Dr. Tamar Kushnir. Her enthusiasm in my project and faith in me were invaluable. Dr. Kushnir not only acted as a professor, but also as a friend and mentor, and for this I cannot even begin to say a proper thanks. Thank you to Dr. Marianella Casasola for her comments, edits and guidance throughout this process. I would also like to thank the Early Childhood Cognition lab for being there to record, transcribe and code when I was overwhelmed. Thank you to my Magical Causal Explanations 2 friends and family who listened to me endlessly talk about this project and still supported me throughout the year. Lastly, thanks to Walt Disney for teaching me that you’re never too old to believe in magic. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Caitlin M. Clark, Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853. Email: CaitlinClark11@gmail.com Magical Causal Explanations 3 Abstract The literature has repeatedly shown that children believe in magic and can distinguish between fantasy and reality (Browne & Woolley, 2004; Harris et al,1991; Phelps & Woolley, 1994; Sharon & Woolley, 2004; Subkotsky, 2001; Taylor & Carlson, 1997; Woolley et al, 2004; Woolley et al, 2006; Woolley & Van Reet, 2006). However, very little research has been done on the context of magical thinking, specifically how children use magic to reason causally. The present study explored magical causal explanations in 32 preschoolers, broken into 16 3 year-olds and 16 4 year-olds. Children saw blocks activate a special toy either working by contact or at a distance and were then asked to categorize whether the event was magic or electric. They were then asked to activate the toy themselves and then to correct the experimenter’s failed action. Children fell into one of three categories. They were either Inconsistent in their categorizations, Consistent One Responders, or Consistent Discriminators. Consistent Discriminators chose distance as magic and contact as electric. These children also discriminated in their action, using distance for magic. There was also an effect of age on children’s use of magic. Older children were more likely to invoke magic as an explanation. Magical Causal Explanations 4 Plausible Explanations: Magical Causal Explanation in Preschoolers There is no question that children are magical thinkers, as the research has repeatedly found (Browne & Woolley, 2004; Harris et al.,1991; Phelps & Woolley, 1994; Sharon & Woolley, 2004; Subkotsky, 2001; Taylor & Carlson, 1997; Woolley et al., 2004; Woolley et al, 2006; Woolley & Van Reet, 2006).Magic provides not just entertainment and wonder to a child; it also functions as one of the first means for learning and reasoning about the world around them. Children form beliefs and theories about culturally constructed fantastical beings (Woolley, Boerger, & Markman, 2004) and they are able to distinguish between fantasy and reality (Harris, Brown, Marriott, Whittall, & Harmer, 1991). Research has started to explore children’s use of magic as an explanatory tool. Phelps and Woolley (1994) proposed the idea that children might use magical explanations when they lack adequate physical explanation. The current study expands on this idea and attempts to assess whether children do in fact use magic as an explanation when they lack other knowledge. When presented with a concrete, mechanical event and a scientific or magic explanation, which will children choose? Magical Beliefs in Culture When most people think of children’s magical beliefs, they immediately think of the many fantasy characters that occur in culture. Most children believe in fantasy entities tied to holidays and special events. Children believe in many of these magical events and characters because these stories are perpetuated by adults. Every year parents explain the appearance of presents under the Magical Causal Explanations 5 Christmas tree as being put there by Santa Claus. Children are told that the money under their pillow came from the tooth fairy. Woolley et al. (2004) created the Candy Witch, a magical character that comes to children’s houses on Halloween night, taking some of their candy in exchange for a toy. Surprisingly, older children (mean age 4.9) who were visited by the Candy Witch had higher levels of belief than did younger children (mean age 3.9). Though initially it might seem that children got more credulous as they grew, the authors claim that believing in the Candy Witch involved a level of cognitive maturity because the older children had to take into account their previous ideas and work out the seemingly magic events of Halloween night. This belief in fantasy entities cannot be explained by childhood credulity. Children do not believe in these characters just because they are told they exist. They already have a belief in magic, and they then hear adults tell these stories. This interaction cements their beliefs. Children’s belief in this type of magic can vary greatly. Each kindergarten class has one student who doesn’t believe in Santa at all, and in every fourth grade class you can find that student who is still clinging to his belief in the jolly old elf. This huge individual variation shows that this belief in cultural magical entities is not universal and varies based on the child. Fantasy/Reality Distinction Though children are magical thinkers, they are not completely credulous. Young children are able to discriminate between reality and fantasy. Harris et al. (1991) explored the fantasy/reality distinction in children, by simply asking them Magical Causal Explanations 6 about real items, imagined items and supernatural imagined items. The children (4- and 6-year-olds) easily differentiated between the real and imagined entities. However, in a second experiment, children were shown a box and told to imagine that there was a monster in it. Children then showed apprehension and caution when approaching the box, even if they had earlier shown a clear distinction between fantasy and reality. Harris et al. (1991) proposed an “availability” and “transmigration” hypothesis. When children imagine a possibility, the idea is easily brought to mind and more mentally available. This availability makes the child start to believe that the event is more likely. Transmigration explains the gap in knowledge between adults and children. Though there is not a huge cognitive gap between children and adults, children lack certain biological and physical knowledge to certainly rule out that magic exists, or that a monster is in the box (Harris et al., 1991). Children use context as one way to discriminate between magic and reality. Woolley and Reet (2006) examined the effect of context on children’s decisions about reality versus fantasy. Children were read either a book about magic, science or everyday life. They were then exposed to a novel entity, with reference to a scientist, a dragon or a child (depending on what book they had read). Children used the story book context they had been exposed to when discriminating whether the novel entity was real or not. Those who had heard the science story said the novel entity was real, while those who were in the magic condition thought the entity was magic. Children can evaluate new information Magical Causal Explanations 7 and determine its reality status depending on context of the presentation (Woolley & Reet, 2006). In addition to context, children also use evidence to distinguish between fantasy and reality. Tullos and Woolley (in press) concluded that children can use physical evidence to discriminate between reality and fantasy and that this ability increases between the ages of 4 and 6. They presented evidence (such as left over feathers and twigs) about real and fantastic animals and then asked children to reason about their existence. Their study showed children gathering and using evidence to make judgments about reality. They also proposed that the acquisition of these skills was developmental, improving with age. Context can provide a very useful piece of evidence in the discrimination between fantasy and reality. Through the years, various researchers have created a measure called Fantasy Orientation, which they used to assess a child’s association with the fantasy world. How often a child participates in pretend and imaginary play and whether or not they have an imaginary friend are some of the things used to measure a child’s Fantasy Orientation. They also looked at children’s patterns of impersonation in play, and what toys children chose to play with. Woolley et al. (2004) found that a child’s measured Fantasy Orientation factored into their belief of the Candy Witch. Those with high Fantasy Orientations had higher beliefs in the Candy Witch than their peers with low fantasy orientation (Woolley et al., 2004). All children vary in their Fantasy Orientation, providing more evidence to the claim that magical beliefs in children are highly variable. Magical Causal Explanations 8 Does a high fantasy orientation lead to understanding of the fantasy/reality distinction or does it lead children to live more in a fantasy world with little distinction between fantasy and reality? Sharon and Woolley (2004) explored the fantasy/reality distinction in young children. Perhaps most interesting out of their findings was that children with a higher fantasy orientation were more accurate in labeling and categorizing real and fantastic figures. These children spent more time engaging in imagination and fantasy than their low fantasy oriented peers. Perhaps their familiarity with the fantasy world caused them to have more knowledge of its limits. The study also provided a sketch of the development of the reality/fantasy distinction. Very young children may be unsure about the properties of real and fantastic entities. As they grow and gain experience they lose this uncertainty. Children begin to treat real and fantasy figures differently, attributing different qualities to the two groups and categorizing them by these qualities. For example, older children and adults attributed human-like social properties to real figures but not to fantasy figures (Sharon & Woolley, 2004). Up to now, all the mentioned literature has just shown that children discriminate between reality and fantasy. They do this through the use of culture, context and evidence. Theory of Mind is critical in the fantasy/reality distinction. Children need to understand that their thoughts are separate from real life in order to distinguish between reality and the fantasies they believe in their own mind. Magic as an Explanation Magical Causal Explanations 9 As children are beginning to distinguish between reality and fantasy and employ magic beliefs in their thinking, they are also exploring the world and are eager to find explanations for the things they see around them. Magic now provides an explanation that many children are willing to accept. Phelps and Woolley (1994) proposed that children of all ages use magical explanations when they lacked adequate physical explanations. However, as they get older, their use of magical beliefs decrease as they learn more about the physical world and are provided with new explanations. It is at this point that children’s magical beliefs begin to take on a sense of causality. After grasping the distinction between reality and fantasy they are able to invoke magic in their explanations. Children can use magic to explain violations of different laws. In their study, Browne and Woolley explored children’s thoughts about violations of physical (walking on the ceiling) mental (reading someone’s mind), and social (a boy wearing a dress) laws. When children were presented with situations violating certain laws and then asked if they required magic, children consistently responded that physical violations require magic more than the social violations. Older children (older 3-year-olds and 4-year-olds) also responded that mental violations need magic more than social violations would. This result shows that magic is not just used to explain physical violations. Children also use magic to explain different types of violations (social, mental), for which they cannot provide their own explanations. Some researchers have been curious not just about the development of magical thinking in children, but in the development of magical thinking Magical Causal Explanations 10 throughout life. Subbotsky (2001) used an unusual event (a box which destroys items put in it) to question children and adults about their magical causal reasoning. The prevailing thought was that older children and adults would show that they understood the distinction between a scientific and magic event through their verbalizations and their actions. However, it seemed that throughout the study, older children’s (9-year-olds) and adult’s actions seemed to show that they accepted both magical and scientific explanations. The 9-year-olds actually showed greater credulity towards a magic spell than a physical device explanation, showing that even at this age, the children still held some belief in magical explanations. The study showed that even adults retain some belief in magical explanations. Causal judgments do not develop in a replacement fashion. Magical thinking is not completely replaced by scientific reasoning. Instead, these two types of causal reasoning coexist in the minds of children and adults (Subbotsky, 2001). Causal Reasoning Preschool children possess a great deal of causal knowledge. Research has proved over the last 30 years that even young children possess a sizeable amount of causal knowledge about the world around them, and they use this knowledge to reason about the physical, psychological and biological world. (Bullock, Gelman, & Baillargeon, 1982; Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1995; Gelman & Welman, 1991; Gopnik and Wellman, 1994; Inagaki & Hatano, 1993; Kalish, 1996; Keil, 1995; Leslie & Keeble, 1987; Oakes & Cohen, 1990; Perner, 1991; Spelke, Breinlinger, Macomber, & Jacobson, 1992; Wellman, 1990). Magical Causal Explanations 11 Spatial cues (for example, contact and at-a-distance) are very important to a child’s perception of causal relations (Cohen & Oakes, 1993; Leslie & Keeble, 1987, Oakes & Cohen,1990, 1993). However, spatial cues are misleading. Many causal events that children see in everyday life do not follow contiguity (Kushnir & Gopnik, 2007). Sometimes, children have to reason about physical events that are unusual, and may even seem magical to them. If given valid information about causal mechanisms in a situation in which spatial contiguity is violated, children are sometimes able to causally reason without using contiguity (Bullock, 1985; Bullock et al, 1982; Schlottmann, 1999; Schlottmann & Surian, 1999; Shultz, 1982). Without contiguity information, children are also able to explain a causal event if they receive implicit instructions (Bullock et al, 1982). Kushnir and Gopnik (2005) found that 4 year old children use probability when inferring causal strength. They presented children with a “detector” and two different toys that make the “detector” go. One of the blocks worked on the “detector” and made it go more than the other, with a 2/3 success rate. Children consistently chose that block as the one that would make the toy go again. Children were shown another two toys and told that some have special stuff inside that make the “detector” go. Again, children chose the block that worked 2/3 of the time when they were asked to make the “detector” go again. Children also tended to favor toys that gave results when the child intervened, showing that intervention affected their judgment of the causal mechanism. They preferred their own intervention as opposed to the intervention of the adult experimenter because their intervention was less likely to be confounded. Magical Causal Explanations 12 Kushnir and Gopnik (2007) further explored causal reasoning in 3- and 4year-olds through the use of an action-at-a-distance design. In this study, the detector was used in many different ways. Sometimes it was activated deterministically (all the time) and sometimes probabilistically (more than half). Action-at-a-distance referred to the conditions when the detector was activated when a toy was simply held above it (not making any contact). Children were able to take into account new evidence in the four tasks. When something worked before, but a new condition such as action-at-a-distance was introduced, they were able to change their prior beliefs about the causality. The study showed that causal learning is not rigid and domain specific, but rather flexible. Children can revise their causal reasoning when they are presented with new evidence. As children’s causal mechanism knowledge grew, their use of magical explanations decreases (Phelps & Woolley, 1994). Causal mechanisms are extremely important in the development of magical thinking and the fantasy/reality distinction. A solid grasp of the fantasy/reality distinction is established concurrently as children’s conception of causal links between their mind and reality improve. Woolley, Browne and Boerger (2006) found that in children, magic followed the causal rules that (1) cause precedes effect and (2) causes are exclusive. They showed children a wishing box with 10 smaller trick boxes containing everyday items inside. Children watched the experimenter wish for certain objects and were then asked to determine if the outcomes were caused by wishing or something else. The many outcomes violated causal rules Magical Causal Explanations 13 such as priority, consistency and exclusivity. They repeated this experiment twice, but instead of using a wishing machine, they once used a regular machine with buttons and then again used a toy with blocks and marbles to demonstrate a simple causal action. They found that children’s causal thinking about wishing was similar to the way they causally reasoned about the working of a machine and a toy. Hypothesis Most of the research on children’s magical thinking has used abstract stories and situations. We are interested in exploring children’s magical thinking in ordinary situations that children have no explanation for, through the use of a mechanical device. How does real world causal knowledge interact with children’s magical thinking? What kind of explanations will they use to explain the activation of a “detector”? When presented with either science or magic, which will they choose? Woolley et al. (2006) mention “Many of the machines with which children interact may seem quite magical to them.” However, they do not expand on this very interesting concept. Today’s child interacts with many electronic devices that may seem magical on a daily basis. Remote controls, DVD players, and stereos are all objects that children often encounter, yet children do not understand how they work. How do children reason about these seemingly magical devices? Past research has shown that children use context to help in their discrimination between fantasy and reality (Woolley & Reet, 2006). Context may Magical Causal Explanations 14 be important in children’s use of magic as a causal explanation. What happens in a real-world, mechanical context? Browne and Woolley (2004) studied children’s thoughts about various kinds of violations, and found that children consistently responded that magic is needed more in a physical violation than in other kinds of violations (i.e., social). When children see a hard-to-believe mechanical, physical event, will they be more likely to invoke magic as an explanation than when they see an easier to believe event? Similar to Phelps and Woolley (1994), we believe that in such a situation, in which children lack physical explanations, they will use magical explanations. We believe that when X causes Y, but children do not understand the causal mechanism, they will invoke magic. Woolley et al. (2004) found that older children (mean age 4.9 years) were stronger believers in the Candy Witch than the younger children (mean age 3.9 years). When studying children’s use of evidence to discriminate between fantasy and reality, Tullos and Woolley (in press) proposed that abilities required in this discrimination were developmental. Similarly, we expect to find a developmental trend, in which older children will invoke magic in their explanations of a mechanical event more than the younger children. We expect the ability to use magic as a causal explanation to improve with age. Many view magic as an explanation children use in place of an “I don’t know” answer. They believe children use magic as a naïve excuse explanation. Harris et al. (1991) proposed that children used magic only when they lack all other knowledge. To many, it is an explanation to fall back on when all else fails. Magical Causal Explanations 15 However, it is our belief that children use magic as a legitimate causal tool. To children, it is a sophisticated explanation that requires them to think and theorize about causal mechanism. Many children believe in magic even when they have knowledge (such as biological and physical knowledge) about reality. Method Participants Participants were 52 preschool aged children, consisting of 26 3 year-olds (average age 41.82 months) and 26 4 year-olds (average age 55.32 months). Children were recruited from local preschools and day care centers in Ithaca, NY. Materials The detector used is identical to that used by Kushnir and Gopnik (2005), and is a wood box with a plastic Lucite top. The box can be activated to light up and play music when the experimenter hits a hidden switch. The objects used with the detector in the categorization and action tasks are 12 wooden, cube blocks, painted blue. There are also two boxes, identical in shape in size. One has a picture of a lightning bolt, signifying electricity on it. The other has a picture of a wand on it, signifying magic. Each picture is the same size and is placed on the same spot of each box. Procedure Task 1 – Categorization The experimenter introduces the child to the blicket detector, or “toy” and says “Here is my new toy. I just got this toy and I brought it to show you. Let’s see how it works.” The experimenter then takes out the blue blocks, and tells the child they are “things.” The child is also told “Some of these things are electric Magical Causal Explanations 16 and work because they have electricity inside them. Some of these things are magic and work because they have magic stuff inside them.” The order of presenting electricity and magic is counterbalanced. Then the experimenter presents each of the boxes, pointing out the picture on the box and explaining which one is for magic things and which one is for electric things. The child is then told that they were going to watch the things and pick which box they go in. The experimenter shows 8 demonstrations with the “things.” Four times, the thing makes contact with the blicket detector and activates it. The other four times, it works at a distance. The experimenter holds the “thing” over the blicket detector and it is activated. The order of these 8 demonstrations is counterbalanced. After each demonstration, the experimenter holds the “thing” out to the child and asks, “Which box does this go in?” The child then places it in one of the two boxes. Task 2 – Action The experimenter tells the child that they are going to get a chance to try with two new blue “things.” The child is told that one has magic in it, and is asked to “Make it go!” Then the child is presented with another “thing”, but this time they are told that it is an electric one. The order here is counterbalanced. The same number of children were presented with an electric “thing” first. Children attempt to make the blicket toy go by either making contact or working at-a-distance. After they try two “things” themselves, the experimenter says “Let me try again.” The experimenter then makes contact with the blicket detector, but it is not activated. The experimenter looks confused, gives the “thing” to the child and asks if they can make it go. This is repeated, but this time, the experimenter fails Magical Causal Explanations 17 with action at-a-distance. The order of contact and at-a-distance is counterbalanced. Results The results of the categorization task are presented first, followed by the results of the Action tasks. Categorization Task Children’s categorizations were coded into one of five response categories. The percent of the time (out of the four distance and four contact demonstrations) the child chose distance as magic and the percent of the time the child chose contact as distance was calculated. Children who categorized 50% of the contact events as magic and 50% of the distance events as magic were coded as “Inconsistent.” Children who put the majority of the things (contact and distance) in the magic box were coded as “All Magic.” On the other hand, children who put the majority of the things (contact and distance) in the electric box were coded “No Magic.” Children who put the 75% or more of the distance things in the magic box, and 25% or less of the contact things in the magic box were coded “Distance is Magic.” Children who did the reverse, and put 75% or more of the contact things in the magic box and 25% or more of the distance things in the electric box were coded “Contact is Magic.” The categorization task investigated how children would categorize different physical events by observing when children use magic and electricity as an explanation. Age in years and the percent of the time children categorized distance as magic were correlated, r(50) = .302, p = .029. Magical Causal Explanations 18 Fig. 1 presents the number of children in each response category (explained previously). Looking at the responses, it became apparent that there were three groups of children. Fig. 2 shows the results split into these groups. The first group was the Inconsistent children. There were 17 children in this group (11 3-year-olds and 6 4-year-olds). The “No Magic” group and the “All Magic” response categories made up the second group of “Consistent One Response.” This group consisted of 20 children (9 3-year-olds and 11 4-year-olds). Within this group there was a marginal age difference between the 3- and 4-year-olds and whether these children categorized everything as magic, 2(1, N = 20) =3.104, p = .078. The 4year-olds were more likely to categorize everything as magic while the 3-yearolds were more likely to categorize everything as electric (no magic). The last group of children was “Consistent Discrimination.” This group was made up of 15 children (6 3-year-olds and 9 4-year-olds). These were children that discriminated between contact and distance, and consistently categorized one action as magic and the other as electric (not magic). There was a significant difference between children choosing distance as magic and contact as magic, 2(1, N = 15) = 5.4, p = .020. Children were more likely to categorize distance as magic and contact as electric. Action Task Children’s responses to the action task were coded. Children were coded as either “all contact” “all distance” “distance=magic” or “contact=magic.” “All contact children made contact with the blicket detector when handed each thing Magical Causal Explanations 19 (both magic and electric). “All distance” children used distance when handed both “things.” “Distance=magic” children used distance when handed a magic thing and contact when handed an electric thing. “Contact=magic” children used contact when handed a magic thing and distance when handed an electric thing. The child’s action task was analyzed using the groups created in the categorization task (Inconsistent, Consistent one response and Consistent Discrimination). There was no difference in the response patterns of the Inconsistent group and the One Response group, 2(1, N = 37) = .091, p = .7633. Combining these two groups, there was a significant effect of children choosing “all contact,” 2(1, N = 37) = 7.811, p = .005. The Consistent Discrimination group was different from the other two groups, and they discriminated between magic and distance in their action responses more than the One Response group, 2 (1, N = 35) = 7.758, p = .005. The Inconsistent, Consistent One Response and Consistent Discrimination groups were used again when analyzing the failed action task. Amongst these groups, children’s responses were coded into “all contact,” “all distance, “corrected,” and “imitate.” “All contact” and “all distance” meant the child activated the blicket detector exclusively by contact or distance after the experimenter failed both times. “Corrected” meant the children used contact when the experimenter failed with distance, and distance when the experimenter failed with contact. Children who coded “imitate” used the same action they had seen the experimenter use. Magical Causal Explanations 20 Again, there was no difference in the response patterns of the Inconsistent and Consistent One Response group, 2(1, N = 37) = .854, p = .356. The children in these two groups also significantly used only contact to activate the detector, 2(1, N = 37) = 10.242, p = .001. Amongst the Consistent One Response group, there was no difference in using only contact between the children who categorized “all magic” and “no magic” in the first task, 2(1, N = 20) = 2.074, p = .150. When compared to the Consistent One Response group, the Consistent Discrimination group was more likely to discriminate in their actions, and use both contact and distance to activate the detector, 2(1, N = 36) = 4.415, p = .036. Discussion The present study examined children’s use of magic as an explanation in a physical, real life context. It was hypothesized that children would invoke magic in their explanations after seeing a harder to believe physical event (in this study, seeing a detector activated by hovering something above it). The children fell into three different response groups. One of these groups, the Consistent Discrimination gives evidence to our hypothesis that children will categorize a harder to believe event as magic. These children were more likely to categorize at-a-distance activation as magic, and contact activation as electric. This same group of children was also more likely to discriminate between at-a-distance and contact in their own action and activation of the detector. This discriminatory pattern suggests that these children might be using a theory, in which they are showing some understanding of the underlying causal mechanism involved. Magical Causal Explanations 21 The two other groups, the Inconsistent children and the Consistent One Response children did not discriminate in their categorization or their own actions. When they were able to activate the detector themselves, they showed a contact bias, activating the detector by contact every time. Though the Consistent One Response group did categorize according to a specific pattern or method (choosing either magic or electricity and staying with that explanation throughout the categorization) they did not continue to use this method when they were able to use their own actions. The children in the Consistent Discrimination group understood that contact and distance work for different reasons. The experimenter showed them different reasons and explanations for the events they saw. They were then able to think about these explanations and discriminate between the physical events, designating a different explanation to each of the two different events (contact or at-a-distance). These children were also able to use both actions and discriminate in their own actions and activation of the detector. To these children, both explanations had value and were equally plausible explanations. If children are provided with an explanation, there will be a group of children (just like the group of Consistent Discrimination in the present study) willing to accept and think about provided explanations and use them to discriminate in their actions. While the discriminating children understood the experimenter’s description of the magic and electric “things” and the proceeding demonstrations as explanations, the children in the Consistent One Response group saw this as evidence instead of explanation. They took the evidence they had seen for one Magical Causal Explanations 22 action and consistently used only that action. These children are not yet using explanations to discriminate. Instead, they are being safe, taking in the evidence they saw in the activation of the detector and continuing to use that method. Though the categorization task might suggest that these children are using smallest inkling of theory when categorizing all the events into either magic or electric, their consistent of use of contact in the action task shows that they don’t have a theory. Instead, they are sticking to what evidence has already shown them works. This study found that as children increased in age, so did the amount of times they categorized at-a-distance as magic. Amongst the Consistent One Responders in the categorization task, there was an age difference. 4-year-olds more often responded that everything was magic, whereas the 3-year-olds were more skeptical and responded that everything was electric (in other words, there was no magic). Both of these findings provide evidence to our hypothesis that invoking magic as an explanation was developmental and increased with age. Phelps and Woolley (1994) claimed that as children get older and learn more about the physical world and gain new explanations, their magical beliefs decrease. In the present study however, younger children are more skeptical and the older children are the one using magic as an explanation. This is easily seen in the Consistent One Response group, but can also be seen in the Consistent Discrimination group. The older children in this group accept and use magic as a plausible explanation. Magical Causal Explanations 23 The results in the present study are more consistent with the findings of Woolley et al. (2004), in which older children had stronger beliefs in a magical character (the Candy Witch). The authors believed that believing in this kind of magical character involved a higher level of cognitive functioning than children who dismissed it. Children who believed had to take conflicting evidence such as their own ideas and what they heard from those around them and form a theory about the Candy Witch. In the present study a similar thing might be happening. Perhaps magic, just as any kind of physical or scientific explanation, requires knowledge and a level of somewhat mature cognitive functioning in order for a child to use it as an explanation. Magic is not a naïve explanation, and children in the study use it as a legitimate causal tool. As Subbotsky (2001) claimed, scientific reasoning does not replace magical thinking. Magical thinking still exists to an extent in adults. The two types of causal reasoning can coexist, as seen in the Consistent Discrimination group. They are able to invoke both magic (a magical explanation) and electricity (a scientific explanation). As children get older, their knowledge of other causal explanations grows, but this does not mean magical thinking is immediately replaced. Sharon and Woolley (1994) explained that as children get older, they lose uncertainty about the properties of real and magical entities. As these children grow and gain experience, they treat real and magical figures differently, attributing different qualities to reality and magic, and categorizing things by Magical Causal Explanations 24 these qualities. The present study found something similar. The children in the Consistent Discrimination group were able to take in the experimenter’s reasons and explanation and attribute different qualities to the different events. Using these new qualifications for magic and electric, they categorized the events they saw. Other groups did not show such discrimination patterns, perhaps because they are still in the stage of uncertainty about real and magical properties, and therefore they cannot yet discriminate between the magical and electric events. The present study’s methodology was not perfect. It is hard to tell if the contact bias in the action task exists because children were being reinforced by the first action they saw. This is possible, though half of the children saw distance as the first action, and they still used contact all the time. It can also be claimed that the order of the tasks should not have been the same for each child. Children may have learned from the repetition of the categorization task, and this could have affected their action. Perhaps the study can be replicated, but with a randomized task order. Future research would benefit from considering the effect of Fantasy Orientation on a child’s use of magic as a causal explanation. Replicating the current study, but adding on a measure of each child’s Fantasy Orientation would be useful. This would make it possible to see if children with higher Fantasy Orientations are more likely to invoke magic in their explanations. A longitudinal study would also be valuable in exploring how a child’s use of magic as a causal explanation changes, as they get older. At what age will the use of magic begin to decrease? The current study, in its unique real life, mechanical context Magical Causal Explanations 25 provides the possibility of many future studies. Future research and knowledge will make it possible to delve deeper into children’s cognition and study how they reason and explain the world around them. Magical Causal Explanations 26 References Bullock, M. (1985). Causal reasoning and developmental change over the preschool years. Human Development, 28, 169-191. Bullock, M., Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1982). The development of causal reasoning. In W. Friedman (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time (pp. 209-254). New York: Academic Press. Browne, C.A., & Woolley, J.D. (2004). Preschoolers’ magical explanations for violations of physical, social and mental laws. Journal of Cognition and Development. 5, 239-260. Cohen, L.B., & Oakes, L.M. (1993). How infants perceive a simple causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421-433. Flavell, J.H., Green, F.L., & Flavell, E.R. (1995). Young children’s knowledge about thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(1, Series No. 243). Gelman, S.A., & Wellman, H.M. (1991). Insides and essence: Early understanding of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213-244. Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H.M. (1994). The theory theory. In L. Hirschfield & S.A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 257-293). New York: Cambridge University Press. Harris, P.L., Brown, E., Marriott, C., Whittall, S., & Harmer, S. (1991). Monsters, ghosts and witches: Testing the limits of the fantasy-reality distinction in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 9, 105-123. Ingaki, K., & Hatano, G. (1993). Young children’s understanding of the mind-body distinction. Child Development, 64, 1534-1549. Magical Causal Explanations 27 Kalish, C. (1996). Causes and symptoms in preschoolers’ conceptions of illness. Child Development, 67, 1647-1670. Kushnir, T., & Gopnik, A. (2007). Conditional probability versus spatial contiguity in causal learning: Preschoolers use new contingency evidence to overcome prior spatial assumptions. Developmental Psychology, 44, 186-196. Kushnir, T., & Gopnik, A. (2004). Young children infer causal strength from probabilities and interventions. Psychological Science, 16, 678-683. Leslie, A.M., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265-288. Oakes, L.M., & Cohen, L.B. (1990). Infant perception of a causal event. Cognitive Development, 5, 193-207. Perner, J. (2000). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Phelps, K.E., & Woolley, J. (1994). The form and function of young children’s magical beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 385-394. Schlottman, A. (1999). Seeing it happen and knowing how it works: How children understand the relation between perceptual causality and underlying mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 35, 303-317. Schlottman, A., & Surian, L. (1999). Do 9-month-olds perceive causationat-a-distance? Perception, 28, 1105-1113. Sharon, T., & Woolley, J. (2004). Do monsters dream? Young children’s understanding of the fantasy/reality distinction. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 293-310. Magical Causal Explanations 28 Schultz, T.R. (1982). Rules of causal attribution. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 47(1, Serial No. 194). Spelke, E.S., Breinlinger, K., Mcaomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605-632. Subbotsky, E. (2001). Causal explanations of events by children and adults: Can alternative causal modes coexist in one mind? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 23-46. Subbotsky, E. (2004). Magical thinking in judgments of causation: Can anomalous phenomena affect ontological causal beliefs in children and adults? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 123-152. Taylor, M. & Carlson, S.M. (1997). The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 436-455. Tullos, A., & Woolley, J. (in press) Evidence evaluation and reality status inferences. Child Development. Wellman, H.M. (1990). The Child’s Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Woolley, J.D., Boerger, E.A., & Markman, A.B. (2004). A visit from the Candy Witch: Factors influencing young children’s belief in a novel fantastical being. Developmental Science, 7, 456-468. Woolley, J.D., Browne, C.A., & Boerger, E.A. (2006). Constraints on children’s judgments of magical causality. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 253-277. Magical Causal Explanations 29 Woolley, J. D., & Van Reet, J. (2006). Effects of context on judgment concerning the reality status of novel entities. Child Development, 77, 1778-1793 Magical Causal Explanations 30 Figure 1 Categorization Responses Number of Children in Response Category 12 10 8 6 3 Year Olds 4 Year Olds 4 2 0 Irregular No Magic All Magic Distance is Magic Contact is Magic Magical Causal Explanations 31 Figure 2 Categorization Responses Number of Children in Response Category 12 Inconsistent 10 Consistent One Response 8 Consistant Discrimination 6 3 Year Olds 4 Year Olds 4 2 0 Irregular No Magic All Magic Distance is Magic Contact is Magic Magical Causal Explanations 32 Figure Captions Figure 1. Frequency of categorization responses Figure 2. Frequency of categorization responses split into three new groups, Inconsistsent, Consistent One Response and Consistent Discrimination