“Unbundling” Difficult Content with Cognitive Load Theory Lana L. Becker, Accountancy, East Tennessee State University Abstract In most college courses regardless of discipline, certain concepts or skills emerge as being the “most difficult” for students and thus create significant challenges for educators as well as learners. As experts in our disciplines, it is sometimes difficult to understand why students struggle with certain content. Cognitive load theory provides a valuable lens through which educators can begin to unravel some of the mystery associated with student difficulties. Cognitive overload is especially threatening to novice learners who are faced with tasks which are inherently complex. Complexity of tasks often occurs due to the presence of underlying elements or sub-skills which must be processed simultaneously. The term “unbundling” is used to describe the important process of identifying these underlying skills which must be individually mastered in order to successfully complete the more complex task. A classroom example will be presented to illustrate how this “unbundling” process was used to develop an instructional tool which addresses the intrinsic load associated with complexity and also makes learning more “visible.” Participants in the session will engage in activities designed to help them practice the “unbundling process” so that they can begin to “unbundle” the difficult content in their own courses. Objectives Raise awareness of the effect of cognitive overload on learning, especially for novice learners who lack schema Explain what creates complexity of tasks, how complexity creates cognitive overload, and how cognitive overload impacts learning Demonstrate how “difficult” content can be “unbundled” to address complexity and drive instructional design Demonstrate the value of making learning “visible” in the classroom Engage participants in activities to reveal the complexity of tasks within their own classrooms Provide ideas for addressing the intrinsic load often associated with complex course content Literature Review According to Paas and Ayres (2014), the fundamental objective of cognitive load theory is to “optimize learning of complex tasks by efficiently using the relation between the limited working memory and unlimited long-term memory” (p. 192). Cognitive load theory is especially applicable to novice learners who lack the highly interrelated knowledge structures present in experts known as schemas. According to Sweller, Van Merriёnboer, and Paas (1998), the development of schemas is the fundamental purpose of education and creates what is known as “germane” load. Because working memory is limited, the building of knowledge structures creates cognitive load which must compete with extraneous and intrinsic cognitive load. Although extraneous load (e.g. confusing course materials) is considered to be controllable by the instructor, intrinsic load is driven by the complexity or the degree of interactivity between the underlying sub-skills of a complex task. The simultaneous processing of these interactive elements creates a cognitive burden for the novice learner, leaving little cognitive capacity for the schema construction that makes full understanding possible. An understanding of this phenomenon, described in the literature as the “paradox of learning,” can help educators begin to unravel the mystery of why some content is so difficult for novice learners (Pollock, Chandler, and Sweller, 2002). Research efforts have been devoted to determining instructional strategies that can effectively address cognitive complexity including scaffolding and sequencing techniques such as “simple-to-complex” and “progressive” sequencing (Van Merriënboer, Kirschner, and Kester, 2003; Sweller and Van Merriënboer, 2005). Description The literature related to cognitive load theory will be used to anchor a presentation focused on understanding why novice learners encounter significant difficulties with certain course content. The presenter will emphasize how identification of the sources of complexity through a process referred to as “unbundling” is a critical first step to designing effective instruction for difficult content. An instructional tool designed for a complex skill in an introductory accounting course will be presented as an example. Discussion Using small groups, participants will complete an exercise designed to “unbundle” a familiar task into its interrelated and underlying sub-skills. Using “pair and share,” participants will be asked to identify a “particularly challenging” concept or skill they teach in a course and “unbundle” its complexity by identifying the sub-skills that must be mastered and processed simultaneously. References Artino Jr, A. R. (2008). Cognitive load theory and the role of learner experience: An abbreviated review for educational practitioners. Aace Journal, 16(4), 425-439. Bannert, M. (2002). Managing cognitive load—recent trends in cognitive load theory. Learning and instruction, 12(1), 139-146. Blayney, P., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2010). Interactions between the isolated–interactive elements effect and levels of learner expertise: Experimental evidence from an accountancy class. Instructional Science,38 (3), 277-287. Davis, E. A., & Miyake, N. (2004). Explorations of scaffolding in complex classroom systems. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(3), 265-272. Lovett, M. C., & Greenhouse, J. B. (2000). Applying cognitive theory to statistics instruction. The American Statistician, 54(3), 196-206. Paas, F., & Ayres, P. (2014). Cognitive load theory: A broader view on the role of memory in learning and education. Educational Psychology Review, 26(2), 191-195. Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and instruction, 12(1), 6186. Sweller, J., Van Merriёnboer, J. J., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational psychology review, 10(3), 251-296. Van Merriënboer, J. J., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the load off a learner's mind: Instructional design for complex learning. Educational psychologist, 38(1), 5-13. Van Merriёnboer, J. J., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational psychology review, 17(2), 147-177.