Running head: MATH STEREOTYPES Math Stereotypes: Their Impact on Gender Performance on a Math Test Charlesa L. Verdell Valdosta State University 1 MATH STEREOTYPES 2 Abstract The purpose of this study was to examine how math stereotypes impact how well male and female participants perform on a math test. Participants’ consisted of 40 males and 40 females who were given a passage to read on math, that either mentioned the gender stereotype of math being a male subject or math and its involvement with writing, science, and sports. The participants were then, asked to take a five question math test and rate their level of agreement with the passage as well as how they believed they performed on the test. The results in the present study did not support the hypothesis and showed that in the presence of the math stereotype female participants scored lower then male participants. MATH STEREOTYPES 3 Math Stereotypes: Their Impact on Gender Performance on a Math Test Throughout the years of American cultural there is been the stereotype of math being more of a male subject then a subject for females. Studies shown that these stereotypes take root in our children as early as the second grade (Cvencek, Meltzoff, & Greenwald, 2011); this research has lead other researchers to study this stereotype and its effect throughout the years. Previous research on the math stereotype and its impacts on females for example Tomasetto, Alperton, and Cadinu (2011) have shown that the stereotype does impact the female’s performance in the subject of math. In their study they provided participants with an age appropriate math test and issued a stereotype awareness task. The results not only showed that girls were aware of the stereotype and that awareness impacted their math ablilty, but it also show cased that mother’s endorsed the stereotype more than fathers by the parenting questions on the stereotype task. Another study done on college students issued a challenging math test to both male and female students either showcasing the math threat or no threat at all (Lesko, and Corpus, 2006). Their study showed as previous studies did that in the presence of the math stereotype female participants scored lower than their male counterparts, but when there was no threat present they actually scored higher than the male participants. Studies have continued to showcase the trend of female participants underscoring at math test in relation to male participants when in the presence of the math stereotype of math being more of a male subject. The purpose of the present study was to showcase that times have changed and that when facing the math stereotype female participants would excel on the math test and would be motivated to go against the stereotype. Participants would either have the stereotype present or not and take a quick math test. MATH STEREOTYPES 4 Method Research Design This experiment used a 2 x 2 between-subjects factorial design with the independent variables being the sex of the participants (male or female) and the influence of math gender stereotypes (absence of or presence of). The participants’ performance on the basic math test and their motivation level based on the questionnaire were the dependent variables. Participants The participants consisted of 30 male and 30 female undergraduate students haphazardly selected from Valdosta, Georgia. Participation was on a volunteer basis and there was no compensation. The student classification of the sample was 10% freshman, 35% sophomore, 25% junior, and 30% senior. The ethnic background of the sample consisted of 34% Caucasian, 40% African-American, 5% Pacific Islander, 20% Asian and 5% Other. Male participants were randomly assigned to read an article that was male oriented and gender bias (n = 20) or had no gender implications (n = 20). Female participants were randomly assigned to an article that was male oriented and gender bias (n = 20) or had no gender implications (n = 20). Materials Participants will be given a packet that contained a short passage on math, a math test and a small questionnaire about the passage. The passage either mentioned math stereotypes and how males identify better with math or only talked about as a subject with no gender implications. While the math test consisted of five questions that focused on multiplication, division, and more, for example one question was solve for y: 4y-2= 33-3y. The questionnaire consisted of four statements that the participants rated they either agreed or disagreed with to assess their motivation based on the passage an example statement was “I agree with the MATH STEREOTYPES 5 information in the passage. The passage was very informative.” They were also asked to fill out demographic information such as their race, and age. Procedure The study was completed individually. I approached a student and asked if he or she would like to participant in a research study about mathematics by taking five to ten minutes of their time to read a passage and take a quick test and questionnaire. If a student agreed they were given a packet that contained a short passage, a math test, a questionnaire, and asked to follow the instructions given. If the student refused, then he or she was thanked for their time and not bothered any further. Each participant was asked to read the passage then to answer the five math questions that followed. Participants were instructed not to use a calculator of any sort and that there was space on the paper for them to work out the problems if needed. Following completion of the test participants were then asked to answer the questionnaire on the last page. Even though participants were advised that this would take five to ten minutes of their time they were not given a time limit. Once the participants completed the packet and turned it in, they were told the true purpose of the experiment and were given a chance to ask any questions that they may have about the study. They also were thanked for their time. Results Math Test Scores A 2 (sex of participant) x 2 (presence or lack of stereotype) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was calculated on participants performance on a math test. There was a significant main effect for participants sex, F(1, 76) = 8.94, p = .004. Male participants who read the passage that contained the stereotype scored higher (M = 92.00, SD = 10.05) than the female MATH STEREOTYPES 6 participants who also read the passage with the stereotype threat (M = 72.00, SD = 16.41). While male participants who did not have the threat (M = 88.00, SD = 10.05) scored lower than their female counterparts (M = 92.00, SD= 10.05). There was also a significant main effect due to the presence or lack of the stereotype threat F(1, 76) = 8.94, p = .004. The participants’ performance on the test depends on the presence of the stereotype threat. Agreement with the passage A 2 x 2 ANOVA was calculated on participants’ motivation to do well on the test based on their agreement with the passage. There were no significant main effects for agreement with the passage, F(1, 76) = 1.31, p = .256 or the participants’ sex, F(1, 76) = 1.31, p = .256. Discussion The results from the present study did not support the hypothesis that when faced with the stereotype threat, female participants would be highly motivated and would perform higher on the math test then the male participants. In general female participants exposed to the threat did poorer than females who were not expressed. By facing the threat female participants had no real desire to do well on the math test with an average of 72% on the grading scale. While male participants exposed to the threat performed higher than females exposed to the threat but lower then females who weren’t. The presence of the stereotype enhanced their desire to perform well with an average of 96% on the grading scale. The present results are consistent with a body of research showing that males performed better on a math test under the presence of a stereotype threat, while the female participants did poorly when faced with threat (Johnson, Barnard-Brak, Saxon, and M. Johnson, 2012). MATH STEREOTYPES 7 References University of Washington (2011, March 14). Gender stereotypes about math develop as early as second grade. ScienceDaily. http://www.sciencedaily.com- /releases/2011/03/110314091642.htm Dario Cvencek, Andrew N. Meltzoff, Anthony G. Greenwald. Math-Gender Stereotypes in Elementary School Children. Child Development, 2011; MATH STEREOTYPES 8 Table 1 Participants Mean of Test Score on the Presence of Stereotypes and Participant Sex Math Stereotype Participant Sex Threat or No Threat Male n 20 20 M 92 88 SD 10 10 n 20 20 M 72 92 SD 16.4 10 Female MATH STEREOTYPES 9 MATH STEREOTYPES 10