Legends for Supplementary Figures Supplementary Figure 1. Active lever presses during the five days of 2 h cue extinction training for rats that had previously been given aCSF or PEPA into the RMTg during the modified (15 min) non-cue extinction sessions. Note that no rats received microinjections during the 2 h cued extinction sessions. A, Active lever presses during the standard (2 h) cue extinction sessions for the cocaine experiment for rats that had previously received PEPA or aCSF during the modified non-cue extinction sessions. There was no effect of prior treatment on the lever pressing during cue extinction. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA found no effect of group (F(1,10) = 0.5440, p > 0.05), an effect of time (F(4,40) = 3.368, p < 0.01), and no interaction (F(4,40) = 0.3649, p > 0.05). B, Active lever presses during the standard (2 h) cue extinction sessions for the food experiment for rats that had previously received PEPA or aCSF during the modified non-cue extinction sessions. There was no effect of prior treatment on the lever pressing during the cue extinction. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA found no effect of group (F(1,15) = 0.1382, p > 0.05), no effect of time (F(4,60) = 1.379, p > 0.05), and no interaction (F(4,60) = 1.262, p > 0.05). The large variability on day 1 for the aCSF group appears to be due to an outlier in the aCSF group with a large number of lever presses (489). Supplementary Figure 2. Extinction learning for cocaine-seeking in rats with microinjections outside the RMTg. To determine the degree of site specificity for the microinjections, a separate analysis of the cocaine-seeking data from animals with misplaced cannulae was conducted. A, Active lever presses (mean ± SEM) during extinction sessions for those rats receiving pre- and post-training microinjections of PEPA or aCSF for all rats with misplaced cannulae. Days 1-5 were modified (15 min) non-cue extinction sessions with microinjections given immediately before or after each session, whereas days 6-12 were 2 h in length with no microinjections. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA found no significant effect of group (F(1,13) = 0.4157, p > 0.05), a significant effect of time (F(4,52) = 14.12, p < 0.01), and no significant interaction (F(4,52) = 0.8895, p > 0.05). For the standard (2 h) non-cue extinction sessions on days 6-12, a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no significant effect of group (F(1,13) = 0.7112, p > 0.05), a significant effect of time (F(6,78) = 3.418, p < 0.01), and no significant interaction (F(6,78) = 0.8126, p > 0.05).There was no difference in rate of extinction learning on days 1-5 or retention of extinction learning on days 6-12. B, Because there were a sufficient number of rats with posterior misplacements for analysis, we also analyzed that separately and, again, found no effect of misplacement. Panel B shows the active lever presses (mean ± SEM) for those rats with injection sites posterior to the RMTg Days 1-5 were 15 min in length with microinjections given immediately before or after each session, whereas days 6-12 were 2 h in length with no microinjections. There was no effect of misplaced PEPA microinjections on the extinction learning or retention for cocaine-seeking. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA found no significant effect of group (F(1,5) = 0.2208, p > 0.05), a significant effect of time (F(4,20) = 7.261, p < 0.01), and no significant interaction (F(4,20) = 2.120, p > 0.05). For the 2 h extinction sessions on days 6-12, a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no significant effect of group (F(1,5) = 0.03545, p > 0.05), no significant effect of time (F(6,30) = 2.211, p > 0.05), and no significant interaction (F(6,30) = 1.031, p > 0.05). Supplementary Figure 3. Active and inactive lever pressing during cue-induced reinstatement tests in rats with BM microinjections outside the RMTg. The analysis was conducted for all misplacement combined (panels A and B). In addition, due to the within-subjects design, we also examined the data for each “direction” of misplacement (all remaining panels). BM microinjections increased lever pressing regardless of the direction of misplacement, relative to the RMTg, for the microinjection. A and B, Combined active and inactive lever presses (mean ± SEM), respectively, during cue-induced reinstatement tests for those rats receiving intra-RMTg microinjections of aCSF or BM regardless of direction of placement error. One-way ANOVAs of the active and inactive lever presses revealed significant effects (F(2, 39) = 20.47, p < 0.01; and F(2, 39) = 13.98, p < 0.01, respectively). Post-hoc tests in both cases revealed that those rats that had received BM microinjections had significantly more lever presses during the cueinduced reinstatement test compared to the extinction baseline and those rats that had received aCSF (p < 0.01 in all cases). C and D, Active and inactive lever presses, respectively, for the rat with an injection site anterior to the RMTg. E and F, Active and inactive lever presses (mean ± SEM), respectively, for those rats with injection sites posterior to the RMTg. One-way ANOVAs of the active and inactive lever presses revealed significant effects (F(2, 15) = 11.06, p < 0.01; and F(2, 15) = 31.90, p < 0.01, respectively). Post-hoc tests in both cases revealed that those rats that had received BM microinjections had significantly more lever presses during the cueinduced reinstatement test compared to the extinction baseline and those rats that had received aCSF (p < 0.01 in all cases). G and H, Active and inactive lever presses (mean ± SEM) for those rats with injection sites with a medial-lateral shift. A one-way ANOVA of the active lever presses revealed a significant effect (F(2, 3) = 72.10, p < 0.01). Post-hoc tests revealed that those rats that had received BM microinjections had significantly more active lever presses during the cue-induced reinstatement test compared to the extinction baseline and those rats that had received aCSF (p < 0.01 in both cases). A one-way ANOVA of the inactive lever presses was not significant (F(2, 3) = 0.5309, p > 0.05). I and J, Active and inactive lever presses (mean ± SEM), respectively, for those rats with injection sites dorsal to the RMTg. A one-way ANOVA of the active lever presses revealed a significant effect (F(2, 9) = 5.738, p < 0.05). Post-hoc tests revealed that those rats that had received BM microinjections had significantly more lever presses during the cue-induced reinstatement test compared to the extinction baseline (p < 0.05). A one-way ANOVA of the inactive lever presses showed a trend toward a significant effect (F(2, 9) 3.744, p = 0.065). K and L, Active and inactive lever presses for the rat with an injection site ventral to the RMTg. * p < 0.01 compared to extinction baseline and the aCSF group. # p < 0.05 compared to extinction baseline. Supplementary Figure 4. Diagrams showing estimated microinjection termination sites for incorrect placements, broken down according to the relative direction of missed placement used in Figure S3. Figures adapted from Paxinos and Watson (2007) and A/P coordinates (in mm) are given relative to Bregma. It is critical to note that these are estimates and that there is less confidence regarding the certainty of these termination sites vis-à-vis those of rats whose termination sites were found to be within the RMTg. In addition, termination sites were categorized, as in Supplementary Figure 3, based on the primary dimension by the sites were incorrect. A, Termination site found to be too anterior (n = 1). B, Termination sites found to be too posterior (n = 6). C, Termination sites found to be shifted too far to the right (n = 2). D, Termination sites found to be too dorsal (n = 4). E, Termination site found to be too ventral (n = 1). Supplementary Figure 5. A, Active lever presses for rats grouped based on whether their missed placements were estimated to be ~0.5 mm or less (proximal) from the border of the RMTg or greater than 0.5 mm (distal) from the border of the RMTg. A one-way repeatedmeasures ANOVA for the active lever presses for “proximal” rats found a significant effect (F(2, 14) = 15.14, p < 0.001). Post hoc analysis revealed that those rats receiving BM had significantly more active lever presses compared to their extinction baseline and aCSF-control group (p < 0.05). A one-way repeated-measures ANOVA for the active lever presses for “distal” rats revealed a significant effect (F(2, 10) = 5.574, p < 0.05). Post hoc analysis revealed that those rats receiving BM had significantly more active lever presses compared to the extinction baseline (p < 0.05) and a trend toward more active lever presses compared to the aCSF control group (p < 0.09). B, Inactive lever presses for rats grouped based on whether their missed placements were proximal or distal from the border of the RMTg. A one-way repeated-measures ANOVA for the inactive lever presses for “proximal” rats found a significant effect (F(2, 14) = 34.39, p < 0.001). Post hoc analysis revealed that those rats receiving BM had significantly more inactive lever presses compared to their extinction baseline and aCSF-control group (p < 0.05). A one-way repeated-measures ANOVA for the inactive lever presses for “distal” rats revealed a trend toward a significant effect (F(2, 10) = 34.39, p < 0.07). Post hoc analysis of the distal rats revealed no significant effects or trends (p > 0.1 in all cases). C and D, Diagrams showing estimated microinjection termination sites for incorrect placements, broken down according to the relative missed placements in panels A and B. Figures adapted from Paxinos and Watson (2007) and A/P coordinates (in mm) are given relative to Bregma. * p < 0.01 compared to extinction baseline and the aCSF group. # p < 0.05 compared to extinction baseline. @ p < 0.09 compared to aCSF group.