“Prime-time Television's Portrayal of Women and the World of Work: A Demographic Profile.” By: Berg, Leah R. Vande; Streckfuss, Diane. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, Spring 92, 36. 2, p195+ PRIME-TIME TELEVISION'S PORTRAYAL OF WOMEN AND THE WORLD OF WORK: A DEMOGRAPHIC PROFILE Content analyses of the attributes and actions of foreground characters in organizational settings were performed. The analyses indicate that although there has been some increase in the representation of women and some slight increase in the heterogeneity of their occupational portrayals, women generally remain underrepresented and limited in their depictions in prime-time organizational settings. Women were portrayed as performing more interpersonal actions than similar male cohorts and fewer decisional, political, and operational actions. Although research on occupational role portrayals on prime-time television was conducted as early as 1951, it was not until after the women's movement gained strength in the late 1960s and early 1970s that the portrayal of occupational roles of women became a focus rather than an afterthought in television content studies. Several primary objectives of the women's movement -- the improvement of women's visibility, role diversity, and equality in the mass media -- stimulated a number of studies on the depiction of women in popular culture (e.g., Bate & Self, 1983; Dominick, 1979; Franzwa, 1974; Turow, 1974). However, few subsequent studies extended these analyses into the 1980s. The present study does so by adding a fourth decade of analysis into the portrayal of gender and occupation on prime-time television. Additionally, this study extends previous ones by examining the portrayal of prime-time male and female-characters and, most importantly and uniquely, the organizational actions they perform in prime-time television's world of work. Previous Research on Working Women in Prime-Time Television Research on the portrayal of the world of work on prime-time television has tended to focus on three general areas: representation, employment status, and sex-typed behavioral and psychological traits. In the following section, we briefly note key content analytic research in each of these areas over the past three decades. Representation. Beginning with Smythe's (1954) study four decades ago, researchers have consistently found that women are underrepresented on television (e.g., DeFleur, 1964; Seggar & Wheeler, 1973). Virtually no change occurred in the 3 to 1 proportion of male to female characters on prime-time television over the 30-year period from the 1950s through the 1970s (e.g., Butler & Paisley, 1980; Dominick, 1979; Greenberg, Simmons, Hogan, & Atkin, 1980; Lemon, 1974; Tedesco, 1974; U.S. Commission on Civil Rights, 1977, 1979). Signorielli (1989) concluded that by 1986 women were still underrepresented. Employment status. Most researchers who have studied the portrayal of occupational roles on television use the U.S. Census Bureau schema to compare the occupational role distributions on television with those of the U.S. labor force. Because each researcher has adapted this schema somewhat differently to suit his/her research purpose, it is impossible to compare directly these findings on specific occupational categories. However, these studies consistently have found that women on prime-time television are portrayed in a much narrower range of roles -- primarily as wives and parents -- than are men, and that men are more often portrayed as being employed and as holding higher status occupations compared to women (e.g., DeFleur, 1964; Downing, 1974; Gentile & Miller, 1961; Long & Simon, 1974; Manes & Melnyk, 1974; Seggar & Wheeler, 1973; Signorielli, 1982; Smythe, 1954; Tedesco, 1974; Turow, 1974; U.S. Commission on Civil Rights, 1977, 1979). Sex-typed behavioral and psychological traits. Turow (1974) examined "advising and ordering" in both prime-time and daytime television and found that men gave directives in 70% (women in 30%) of the episodes. Other content analytic studies of sex-typed behaviors on prime-time television have found that women were more likely than men to be portrayed as emotional and in need of emotional support (Greenberg, Richards, & Henderson, 1980); as sympathetic, nurturing, and reinforcing (Downs & Gowan, 1980; Harvey, Sprafkin, & Rubenstein, 1979); and as lacking interpersonal and occupational power (Lemon, 1977; Turow, 1974). None of the studies reviewed above has examined differences in types of organizational actions performed by prime-time working women and men. The concept of organizational action is an important contextual variable, because it reflects the type of behavior characters perform in the workplace. Vande Berg and Trujillo (1989) regard organizational actions as being composed of five categories of behavior: interpersonal function (development and cultivation of interpersonal activities in the organization, which includes counseling, motivating, and general sociabilities); informational function (disseminating information to or receiving information from organizational insiders and outsiders); decisional function (problem solving and conflict resolution); political function (display, development, or use of power to accomplish individual or group self-interests); and the operational function (directly resulting in manufacturing products, delivering services, or everyday work tasks being done). This study examines the ways female and male characters are portrayed on primetime television. The analysis will focus on equality of representation across industries, occupational roles, hierarchical position, depictions, genre, and dramatic tone. But this study will not be limited to comparing the presence of males and females or these contextual variables. An additional set of comparisons will be conducted to reflect the degree of activity of males and females. Thus, the patterns of presence can be compared with the patterns of activity. Methods The sample for this study included 116 prime-time television program episodes that covered two weeks of programming for each of the three major U.S. commercial networks (CBS, NBC, and ABC). One sample week was from the spring of 1986 and the other from the spring of 1987. The sample was restricted to regularly scheduled prime-time network programs, excluding all specials, movies, sports, and news programs. When regularly scheduled programs were unavailable for videotaping during the scheduled week, makeup episodes were videotaped at later dates. In the few instances where this was not possible, a comparable program (i.e., regularly scheduled, of the same length, belonging to the same genre, and appearing on the same network) was videotaped. Of the 116 episodes in the sample, 115 contained at least one character with an identifiable occupation and who performed an organizational action in at least one scene. A total of 115 prime-time program episodes containing 1,944 characters and 7,601 actions were coded. This article reports only the findings for the 986 foreground characters. A foreground character was defined as a character (major regular and minor regular series character as well as major and minor episode character) whose speaking and action role served an important plot function. Two parallel but separate sets of analyses were performed on this data in order to examine not only the demography of organizational characters but also the types and patterns of organizational actions performed in television's world of work. The first set of analyses focused on the 986 foreground characters. The second set of analyses looked at the 6,087 organizational actions performed by these characters. For both sets of analyses, six contextual variables were used as points of comparisons. These contextual variables were industry, occupational role, hierarchical position, depiction, genre, and dramatic tone. The five-category schema developed by Vande Berg and Trujillo (1989) was used to provide a single overall organizational function code for each foreground character and to code each organizational action in each scene. An action was defined as a verbal or behavioral work-related activity performed in a single scene by a character in an organizational context. Each action in each scene that served one of the five organizational functions was coded as a separate action. A scene was operationally defined as a sequence of related shots in which there was no change of setting or break in the continuity of time. A change in scene was deemed to occur, however, when the setting remained constant but the characters changed (e.g., when a new character entered or a present character departed). This coding is consistent with the procedures of other media researchers who have studied prime-time actions. Contextual Variables When a foreground character was identified, a coding was made on each of the following contextual variables: industry, occupational role, hierarchical position, depiction, genre, and dramatic tone. Also these six contextual variables were coded for each organizational action of each foreground character. Industry. Defined as the work setting of the characters, industry was coded into one of the 11 major categories of the U.S. Department of Labor's Standard Industrial Classification schema. Transportation and communications industry categories were collapsed into one industry category and an additional "other" category was added to account for illegal and/or unidentifiable industry settings (see Table 1). Also, several infrequently appearing categories (agriculture, mining, construction, manufacturing, and wholesale trade) were collapsed into a single category. Occupational role. The U.S. Bureau of the Census classification schema was adapted to describe character occupations not recognized by the government coding system (e.g., student, lawbreaker, and customer/patient). The resulting 17 categories were collapsed into 10 in this analysis (see Table 2). Hierarchical position. This was defined as the character's position within an organization's formal power hierarchy. This variable was created by combining the exclusively managerial typology of some management scholars (e.g., Hellriegel & Slocum, 1985) with broader typologies of other organizational scholars (e.g., England, 1967) in order to account for such hierarchical differences as those between resident physicians or detectives (upper level professional staff) and nurses or patrol officers (lower level professional staff). Thus, following Vande Berg and Trujillo (1989), 11 hierarchical categories were used to code characters as indicated in Table 3. Depiction. Each character and all organizational actions were coded according to their plot function (positive, negative, or neutral). Positive depictions were defined as those that benefitted the organization and its members or the broader society of which the individual or organization was a part. The four positive levels included charitable/philanthropic, sympathetic/helpful, socially or economically productive, and friendly. Neutral depictions were defined as those displaying mere civility or general politeness with no discernible positive or negative plot function. Negative depictions were defined as those which hurt the organization, its members, clients, and/or relevant outsiders, or which harmed the broader society. The five negative variable levels included unfriendly, greedy/selfish, foolish, malevolent, and illegal. Specific operational definitions of each depiction category can be found in Vande Berg and Trujillo (1989,pp. 75-76). Genre. TV Guide genre classifications were collapsed into four genre categories: comedy, drama, action-adventure, and other (including science fiction and the recent dramedies). Dramatic tone. Dramatic tone was defined as the literary backdrop against which the attitude of the "author(s)" toward the characters and their actions was presented. Three categories of this variable were comedic, serious, and combined serious and comedic (including irony and dramedy). Reliability Two independent coders trained for three hours in the use of coding protocols independently coded the character and action data for the contextual variables. One original coder and one new coder coded all data for sex. After viewing an entire episode and completing the scene-by-scene coding of organizational actions (hereafter referred to as the action analysis), coders gave each character a single overall summary coding judgment for each of the variables (hereafter referred to as the character analysis). On the sex coding of foreground characters and their actions, there was 100% intercoder agreement (using Scott's pi based on a 10% subsample of the data coded by two independent coders). Intercoder reliability estimates for contextual variables were: .87 and .84, respectively, for the character and action in the industry analyses; .$4 and .82 for the occupational role; .81 and .79 for hierarchical position; .74 and .71 for depiction; .96 and .93 for genre; .96 and .93 for dramatic tone; and .76 and .79 for organizational function. Results Overall representation by sex. Male characters were found to outnumber female characters by a factor of about 2 to 1. Of the 986 foreground characters identified, 640 (64.9%) were male and 346 (35.1%) were female (see Table 1). When the gender comparison is made on occupational actions, the proportion of males is even higher. Of the 6,087 occupational actions of foreground characters, 4,12 3 (67.7%) were attributed to males.. By industry. Overall, there were statistically significant differences between male and female foreground characters in our sample in terms of the industries in which they worked. Twice as many male (29%) as female characters (15%) worked in public administration industries (law enforcement, courts, national security), and men outnumbered women in transportation, communication, and manufacturing industries. Female and male characters were almost equally as likely to be seen in agriculture, wholesale, and finance industries, while women were proportionately more likely than men to be seen in the construction and retail trade industries. We also found statistically significant differences between male and female actions in terms of their industrial contexts (p < .001). Compared to male activity, female activity was proportionally higher in the categories of service, retail trade, and no industry. When the findings of the overall character analysis and the scene-by-scene action analyses were compared, we noted several differences. For example, while more men than women worked in the transportation/communications industry, men and women performed proportionately nearly equal percentages of the organizational actions in this industry. By occupational role. These findings confirm that the differential sex-based pattern of the past three decades has continued into the late 1980s. Only professional and technical workers were proportionately the same between men and women characters. Men appeared more often than women in the occupations of manager, service, military, and lawbreakers. Females were seen far more frequently than males in household occupations and as students. They were also far more likely than males to be portrayed without an organizational occupational role as part of their characterization. These same differences resulted in the analysis by actions. By hierarchical position. This study found that male characters who were professional were more likely to hold higher status organizational positions than females (see Table 3). However, as managers males were more likely to be lower level compared to females, female characters proportionately were less likely than males to hold upper level professional staff positions (e.g., staff physicians and detectives), were far more likely than males to be found in the lower level professional staff positions (e.g., nurses and beat officers), and were nearly as likely as males to hold worker/laborer positions. Significant differences were also found between the organizational actions of men and women at the same level in organizational hierarchies. Male managers and lower level professional staff members performed a significantly greater number of organizational actions than did females at those same organizational levels, while proportionally more organizational actions were performed by female workers, female general staff members, and female upper level professional staff members than by males at those same organizational levels. Also notable was the finding that females performed proportionately more customer and client actions than did males. By depiction. The overall character analysis found statistically significant differences between the ways males and females in organizational contexts are depicted. Working women were proportionally less likely than men to be depicted as negative characters, somewhat more likely than men to be depicted as positive characters, and equally likely to be portrayed neutrally (see Table 4). When the analysis was conducted on actions, the same pattern resulted with the exception that females were depicted a bit less neutral than were males. By genre. A significant difference was found between male and female characters in terms of the genre in which each was most likely to appear (X[2] = 12.6, df = 3, p < .005). Females were more likely than males to appear in comedies and less likely to appear in dramas, while twice as many males as females appeared in actionadventure programs. The scene-by-scene analysis of the actions of organizational members revealed similar findings. Females performed significantly more actions in comedies and the "other" program category, while males performed significantly more of the actions in dramas and in action-adventure genre programs (X2 = 34.66, df = 2, p < .001). By tone. There were no statistically significant differences in the overall character analysis in terms of the dramatic tone with which male and female characters were portrayed. However, the scene-by-scene action analysis did reveal statistically significant differences (X2 = 337.35, df = 9, p < .001) between the tone with which the actions of female and male organizational members were portrayed. The organizational actions of women were more likely to be presented comedically, less likely to be portrayed seriously, and slightly more likely to be presented in a mixed serious-comedic tone than were male characters' actions. By organizational function. Proportionately, female characters were far more likely than male characters to be portrayed performing interpersonal functions in organizations, while male characters were far more likely to be seen fulfilling informational, decisional, political, and operational functions in organizations. This pattern was the same when analyzing actions. Discussion One major contribution of this study is the discovery of the utility of the dual analyses used. As was noted in the findings section above, several significant differences appeared in the scene-by-scene analysis of the actions of organizational characters that did not appear in the single overall character analysis. This suggests that future content analytical studies might gain additional insights by utilizing similar two-pronged approaches in the analysis of prime-time portrayals. A second major research contribution of this study concerns the findings. Although nearly 57% of the U.S. civilian work force in 1988 were women (Sapiro, 1990, p. 23, quoting the Office for Economic Cooperation and Development), only 35% of this composite six-week sample of 1986 and 1987 prime-time's organizational members were females. This figure confirms what DeFleur (1964) noted some 25 years ago: "Overall, the world of work [on television] is a man's world." In other areas, television's world of work remains a "man's world" as well. Occupationally, this study confirmed that relatively little has changed over 40 years of prime-time television in terms of the portrayal of working women. Thus, although women on prime-time television in the late 1980s were nearly as likely as men to be employed as professionals, women were still far more likely to hold lower positions than males in their organization's status hierarchy (i.e., nurses rather than physicians). Additionally, even though women were found on each rung of the organizational hierarchy from Board Member/CEO to worker/ laborer, they tended to be underrepresented in most managerial positions. When women did hold upper level management positions such as CEO/ President, most often these positions were inherited from their spouses or relatives (for example, Owner/Publisher of the Los Angeles Tribune, Mrs. Margaret Pynchon, and the late owner of the St. Gregory hotel, Laura Trent, both inherited their CEO positions). Such findings suggest that television continues to present working women as lacking the competitively achieved occupational hierarchical power and status of male workers. The findings that female characters perform proportionately more interpersonal/relational actions (motivating, socializing, counseling, and other actions which develop worker relationships) and fewer decisional, political, and operational actions than do male characters suggest that in organizational as well as domestic life on television, the overall image of women continues to be one in which women are portrayed and defined primarily through stereotypically domestic "expressive and socio-emotional roles" (see Heibrun, 1976; Hire, 1976; Long & Simon, 1974; Pearson, 1985). Television's working women continue to be portrayed significantly less often than working men as decision-makers, as assertive corporate politicians, and as socially and economically productive working persons. From the dominant American corporate ethic (the aggressive, individualistic, competitive capitalist ideology), such portrayals illustrate "why women still don't hit the top" very often (Fierman, 1990). However, from an alternative humanist/feminist perspective, findings that television's working women engage in a large number of interpersonal actions such as counseling, motivating, and interacting in caring ways with other workers can be interpreted as succeeding. As Ehrenreich and others noted in the 1990 Time special issue, "Women: The road ahead," the goal of the women's movement of the 1970s was not to "exchange places with men" but to create "better places, in a kinder, gentler, less rigidly gendered world" (p. 15). From a feminist/humanist perspective, these images of working women and men are not disturbing because working women are depicted as enacting a humane, interpersonally-focused, cooperative, concerned, information-sharing style of working and managing. Rather, the portrayals of the world of work are disturbing because of the lack of change in the depiction of television's working men, who are often presented as tough, self-centered, and aggressively competitive. Research indicates both that children are active constructors of sex-role typed attitudes and that television plays a role in the occupational socialization of young viewers (e.g., Baxter & Kaplan, 1983; Durkin 1985; Morgan, 1982, 1987; Rothschild, 1984). For example, Morgan's (1982) panel study of 6th-8th graders concluded that television cultivates (especially in girls) such notions as "men are born with more drive to be ambitious and successful than women." The present study's findings that working men are seen more often performing operational, decision-making, and political actions, while women were seen more often performing interpersonal (relational) actions may help explain Morgan's adolescent audience findings and his pointed observation: "The stable assumptions and images about the norms of the adult world, as presented on television, may provide hidden yet vivid and powerful constraints on their (adolescents') notions about the places women should take in that world" (p. 954). This study is grounded in cultivation theory. Like cultivation research, it "begins with identifying and assessing the most recurrent and stable patterns in television content, emphasizing the consistent images, portrayals, and values that cut across programming genres" as an initial step in our "attempts to document and analyze the contributions of television to viewers' conceptions of social reality" (Signorielli & Morgan, 1990, p. 16). This study has added a fourth decade of analysis to the study of patterns in television's portrayal of occupational roles and has extended previous research by analyzing prime-time television's portrayal of the organizational actions of working women and men. However, because television is such a tireless and pervasive purveyor of images, there remains a need for ongoing analysis of the patterns in television portrayals of the world of work. Table 1 Industry of Characters and Actions Performed by Sex Legend for Chart: A B C D E F - Industry N (Characters[1]) %Male (Characters[1]) %Female (Characters[1]) N (Action[2]) %Male (Action[2]) G - %Female (Action[2]) A B E C F D G 457 2872 44% 46% 51% 50% 234 1862 29 34 15 23 Retail Trade 45 342 4 5 6 7 Finance 45 129 4 2 5 2 27 213 4 3 1 3 No Industry 141 502 12 7 18 11 Other[3] 37 161 3 2 4 4 986 6087 100% 100% 100% 100% Service Public Administration Transportation & Communication Totals 1 x2 = 40.92, df = 6, p < .001 2 x2 = 109.52, df= 6, p < .001 3 Includes agriculture, construction, manufacturing, and wholesale trade. Table 2 Occupation of Characters and Actions Performed by Sex Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G - Occupation N (Characters[1]) %Male (Characters[1]) %Female (Characters[1]) N (Action[2]) %Male (Action[2]) %Female (Action[2]) A Professionals Managers Service B E C F D G 195 1209 20% 20% 20% 20% 135 702 16 14 9 7 191 1765 21 31 17 26 Household 31 89 1 1 6 3 Military 30 269 4 6 1 2 Others 47 245 4 2 7 7 Student 77 291 7 4 9 6 Customer/Patient 98 595 8 8 14 14 No Occupation 130 628 12 9 16 13 Lawbreaker 52 294 7 5 1 2 986 6087 100% 100% 100% 100% Totals 1 X2 = 60.57, df = 9, p < .001 2 X2 = 347.35, df = 9, p < .001 3 Includes clerical, sales, crafts, operatives, and workers/laborers Table 3 Hierarchical Position of Characters and Actions Performed by Sex Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G - Position N (Characters[1]) %Male (Characters[1]) %Female (Characters[1]) N (Action[2]) %Male (Action[2]) %Female (Action[2]) A B E C F D G 49 194 4% 3% 6% 4% 53 318 7 7 3 2 Middle Manager 56 375 8 8 1 2 First Line Man. 65 364 8 7 4 5 CEO/Board Executive/ Top Manager Upper Level Prof. Staff 160 1477 18 25 14 22 27 179 2 2 4 6 Staff 98 438 8 6 14 10 Workers/Laborers 77 334 8 5 7 6 Customer/Patient 121 773 10 11 16 16 93 829 10 14 9 13 187 806 17 12 22 15 986 6087 100% 100% 100% 100% Lower Level Prof. Staff Small Business Owner Other[3] Totals 1 X2 = 53.58, df = 10, p < .001 2 X2 = 315.43, df = 10, p < .001 3 Position where organizational hierarchy was not identifiable Table 4 Depiction of Characters and Actions Performed by Sex Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G - Depiction N (Characters[1]) %Male (Characters[1]) %Female (Characters[1]) N (Action[2]) %Male (Action[2]) %Female (Action[2]) A B E C F D G 9 35 1% .5% 1% .3% 176 682 16 10 22 13 174 665 18 11 16 10 205 1444 20 22 23 28 Positive Charitable Helpful Soc./Econ. Productive Friendly Neutral 180 2268 18 39 20 35 48 119 6 2 2 1 Malevolent 24 109 3 2 1 1 Foolish 41 114 3 2 5 2 Selfish 83 275 9 4 8 5 Unfriendly 44 376 6 7 2 5 986 6087 100% 100% 100% 100% Negative Illegal Totals Note. Ferguson (1976) explains that with the use of chl square, error introduced by small expected frequencies is not of serious consequence with two degrees of freedom since "an expectation of not less than two in each cell will permit the estimation of roughly approximately probabilities" (p. 202). 1 X2 = 23.34, df= 9, p < .005 2 X2 = 80.86, df = 9, p < .001 Table 5 Organizational Functions of Characters and Actions Performed by Sex Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G - Organizational Function N (Characters[1]) %Male (Characters[1]) %Female (Characters[1]) N (Action[2]) %Male (Action[2]) %Female (Action[2]) A B E C F D G Interpersonal 339 2006 31% 30% 41% 39% Informational 373 1740 38 42 37 41 103 387 12 9 8 7 49 5 4 Decisional Political Operational Totals 145 4 3 122 617 14 15 10 10 986 6087 100% 100% 100% 100% 1X2=13.5, df = 4, p < .01 2X2 = 69.19, df = 4, p < .001 References Bate, B., & Self, L. (1983). The rhetoric of career success books for women. Journal of Communication, 33(2), 149-165. Baxter, L, & Kaplan, S. (1983). Context factors in analysis of prosocial and antisocial behavior on prime time television. Journal of Broadcasting, 27, 25-36. Butler, M., & Paisley, W. (1980). Women and the mass media: Sourcebook for research and action. New York: Human Sciences Press. DeFleur, M. L. (1964). Occupational roles as portrayed on television. Public Opinion Quarterly, 28, 57-74. Dominick, J. (1979). The portrayal of women in prime time, 1953-1979. Sex Roles, 5, 405-411. Downing, M. (1974). Heroine of the daytime serial. Journal of Communication, 24(2), 130-137. Downs, A. C., & Gowan, D.C. (1980). Sex differences in reinforcement and punishment on prime-time television. Sex Roles, 6, 683-694. Durkin, K. (1985). Television and sex-role acquisition 2: Effects. British Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 191-210. Ehrenreich, B. (1990,Fall). Sorry, sisters, this is not the revolution. Time (fall 1990 special issue, Women: The road ahead), p. 15. Fierman, J. (1990,July 30). Why women still don't hit the top. Fortune, pp. 40, 42, 46, 50, 54, 58, 62. Franzwa, H. (1974). Women in fact and fiction. Journal of Communication, 24(2), 104-109. Gentile, F., & Miller, S. M. (1961). Television and social class. Journal of Sociology and Social Research, 25, 259-264. Greenberg, B., Richards, M., & Henderson, L. (1980). Trends in sex-role portrayals on television. In B. S. Greenberg (Ed.), Life on television: Content analyses of U.S. TV drama (pp. 65-88). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Greenberg, B., Simmons, K., Hogan, L., & Atkin, C. (1980). The demography of fictional television characters. In B. S. Greenberg (Ed.), Life on television: Content analyses of U.S. TV drama (pp. 35-46). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Harvey, S. E., Sprafkin, J. N., & Rubenstein, E. (1979). Prime-time television: A profile of aggressive and prosocial behaviors. Journal of Broadcasting, 23, 179-189. Heilbrun, A. (1976). Measurement of masculine and feminine sex role identities as independent dimensions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 183190. Hellriegel, D., & Slocum, J. W. (1985). Management (4th ed.). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Hite, S. (1976). The Hire report. New York: Macmillan. Lemon, J. (1977). Women and blacks on prime-time television. Journal of Communication, 27(4), 70-79. Long, M., & Simon, R. J. (1974). The roles and statuses of women on children and family TV programs. Journalism Quarterly, 51, 107-110. Manes, A. L., & Melnyk, P. (1974). Televised models of female achievement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 4(4), 365-374. Morgan, M. (1982). Television and adolescents' sex role stereotypes: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 947-955. Morgan, M. (1987). Television, sex-role attitudes, and sex-role behavior. Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 269-282. Pearson, J. C. (1985). Gender and communication. Dubuque, IA: Willam C. Brown. Rothschild, N. (1984). Small group affiliation as a mediating factor in the cultivation process. In G. Melischek, K. E. Rosengren, & J. Stappets (Eds.), Cultural indicators: An international symposium. Vienna: Osterreichischen Akademie der Wissenschafien. Sapiro, V. (1990). Women in American society (2nd ed.). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield. Seggar, J. F., & Wheeler, P. (1973). World of work on TV: Ethnic and sex representation in TV drama. Journal of Broadcasting, 17, 201-214. Signorielli, N. (1982). Marital status in television drama: A case of reduced options. Journal of Broadcasting, 26, 585-597. Signorielli, N. (1989). Television and conceptions about sex roles: Maintaining conventionality and the status quo. Sex Roles, 21,341-360. Signorielli, N., & Morgan, M. (Eds.). (1990). Cultivation analysis: New directions in media effects research. Newbury Park: Sage. Smythe, D. W. (1954). Reality as presented by television. Public Opinion Quarterly, 18, 143-156. Tedesco, N. (1974). Patterns in prime time. Journal of Communication, 24(2), 119124. Turow, J. (1974). Advising and ordering: Daytime, prime time. Journal of Communication, 24(2), 138-141. U.S. Commission on Civil Rights. (1977). Window dressing on the set: Women and minorities in television. (Doc# CR1.2: W72). U.S. Commission on Civil Rights. (1979). Window dressing on the set: An update. (Doc# CR1.2: W72 / Update). Vande Berg, L., & Trujillo, N. (1989). Organizational life on television. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. ~~~~~~~~ Leah R. Vande Berg and Diane Streckfuss Leah R. Vande Berg (Ph.D., University of Iowa, 1981) is an Associate Professor of communication studies at California State University, Sacramento. Diane Streckfuss (M.A., Southern Methodist University, 1990) is a client service representative at the Arbitron Company. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Southern States Communication Association Convention in Louisville, Kentucky, 1989. This manuscript was accepted for publication December, 1991.