Appendix: detailed methods of group manipulation and data analysis Collection & Laboratory Maintenance Social spiders of genus Stegodyphus live together in a colony containing 1-2000 closely related individuals [1]. Like all social spiders, colony members cooperate in shared web maintenance, prey capture, and alloparental care [2]. Their webs are composed of two structures: a twodimensional capture web and a dense three-dimensional retreat composed of a series of silken tunnels. Spiders reside within their retreat for the majority of the day, and only emerge in response to prey or at night in order to repair their capture web. Fifteen colonies of S. dumicola were collected along fences in the Kalahari Desert between Groblerschloop and Uppington in the Northern Cape, South Africa. Colonies were hand collected, placed in a cloth pillowcase for transport, and transported to our laboratory at the University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA (Collection/Export Permit: FAUNA 189/2013— #0056 AAA041-00016, Research Permit: FAUNA 1060/2012— #16634). Colonies were sorted by hand in laboratory and their group sizes were determined by counting the number of females therein (25-691 individuals). Six of the largest colonies were selected for use in our experiment. We haphazardly selected 84 females for inclusion in our studies from each of these colonies. Assignment of females to different treatment groups was determined randomly using a random number generator in Excel (Microsoft). Prior to assignment to a given treatment, females were housed individually in translucent 2oz deli cups that contained a ball of poultry wiring to permit web construction. We maintained spiders in isolation for six weeks prior to the start of the experiments. Isolated spiders were maintained on an ad libitum diet of size-matched two-weekold crickets. Group manipulation Assignment of females to different treatment groups was determined randomly using a random number generator in Excel (Microsoft). Prior to assignment to a given treatment, females were housed individually in translucent 2oz deli cups that contained a ball of poultry wiring to permit web construction. We maintained spiders in isolation for six weeks prior to the start of the experiments. Isolated spiders were maintained on an ad libitum diet of size-matched two-weekold crickets. Altogether, the group identity experiment ran for eight weeks, and colonies were disturbed after either one, two, three, four, five, six, or seven weeks. So, for example, colonies that experienced a social disturbance after one week were then left undisturbed for seven weeks and would have more repeated interactions with each other than colonies that were disturbed after five weeks, and so on. Our social disturbance involved all of the members being removed from the colony and then being placed into a new container with all the same individuals (control treatment) or with a completely new and unfamiliar set of individuals (mixed treatment). Colonies in the mixed treatment were reassembled using individuals taken from the same source colony but that had not interacted. Therefore, individuals in the control and mixed colonies both experienced the same disturbance (i.e., their old web being destroyed and a new web being constructed), but the identity of the individuals in the mixed colonies was altered. Thus, any differences between the control and mixed colonies could be attributed to the effects of repeated interactions among the same members and not the effect of the social disturbance per se. We used a split design where each of our six source colonies was used to establish one replicate of each of our fourteen treatment combinations (mixed and control colonies at one, two, three, four, five, six, or seven weeks since social disturbance). Our design also ensured that all source colonies equally contributed to all treatment groups and that relatedness among individuals was at naturally-occurring levels, which is known to influence social spider behaviour [3, 4]. Therefore, if relatedness among individuals was more influential on behaviour than group identity, we would expect to see no differences between the control and mixed colonies at any treatment. All experimental colonies were housed in 1.5L clear plastic deli cups that contained a dome of poultry wiring for web construction. Lids were covered with a 1 × 1 mm screen, which allowed ample airflow and low humidity levels, consistent with conditions in the Kalahari. Colonies were kept on a maintenance diet of ad libitum six-week-old crickets once weekly. To ensure successful prey capture, crickets were immobilized prior to being placed in colonies’ capture webs. After the social disturbance, all individuals were re-housed within a new 1.5L container with chicken wire. All colonies resumed normal feeding behaviour within 3 days of the social disturbance. At the end of our eight-week experiment, all colonies were disassembled, individuals were isolated back into 2 oz deli cups and their personality (i.e., boldness) was repeatedly assayed daily for the next 5 days. Boldness Assay This assay was designed to measure how quickly an individual recovered from a potential predator attack. Five boldness assays (one per day) were completed on each spider beginning 24 h after the end of the group identity treatments. Boldness assays were initiated by placing a single individual within a rectangular enclosure (13.5×13×3.5 cm). Spiders were acclimated for 60s before applying two jets of air to the dorsal, anterior part of the animal using an ear-cleaning bulb. This universally elicited a huddle response from S. dumicola. As our measure of boldness, we recorded the individual's latency to resume movement following the huddle response. References 1. Seibt U., Wickler W. 1988 Bionomics and social structure of'Family Spiders' of the genus Stegodyphus, with special reference to the African species S. dumicola and S. mimosarum (Araneida, Eresidae). Verhandlungen des Naturwissenschaftlichen Vereins in Hamburg 30, 255304. 2. Lubin Y., Bilde T. 2007 The evolution of sociality in spiders. Advances in the study of Behavior 37, 83-145. 3. Ruch J., Heinrich L., Bilde T., Schneider J.M. 2009 Relatedness facilitates cooperation in the subsocial spider, Stegodyphus tentoriicola. BMC Evolutionary Biology 9(1), 257. 4. Schneider J.M., Bilde T. 2008 Benefits of cooperation with genetic kin in a subsocial spider. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(31), 10843-10846.