Context dependent learning using aversive stimuli in Eastern and Western Hognose snakes (Heterodon platyrhinos and Heterodon nasicus) Seana McGuffey Summer Honors Research 2003 Introduction Little is known about the learning capacity of reptiles. Though much research has been done involving learning in mammals and birds, learning in snakes is a topic that has not been extensively explored. This project was proposed in the interest of advancing knowledge regarding learning in reptiles, specifically snakes, and in understanding the ways in which they learn using aversive stimuli to come to understand the anti-predator behaviors of these organisms. Research done by Burghardt and Greene (1977) using neonate hognose snakes has indicated that these snakes habituate to a specific stimulus (predator). Burghardt and Greene have also found variation between Heterodon platyrhinos and Heterodon nasicus in response waning. Yet no research has been cited testing contextual cues with these snakes to test learning. This project seeks to test the variability of response waning within the new context, as well as measure learning in the snakes. The anti-predator responses of Heterodon platyrhinos and Heterodon nasicus have been explored minimally. The particular behaviors that comprise the responses of feigned attack and feigned death are specific to these snakes. The number of snakes used allows a limited comparison of individual variability, interspecies comparison and a control group for variability in response to predators in the grass context compared with the asphalt context. There has been much speculation about the intelligence of reptiles, more about specifically snakes because they are difficult organisms to study using Pavlovian Conditioning, and the task of finding an effective stimulus is incredibly difficult. In beginning to conduct studies that test capacity for learning in snakes, the ways snakes learn can begin to be investigated. Understanding the circumstances from which snakes take "learned behaviors" and how this information is remembered and processed allows humans to begin to understand the snakes themselves. The aspect of this project dealing with the defense mechanisms of the hognose snake and the circumstances that elicit these responses allows an understanding of what the organism perceives to be a threat. Work with these snakes will hopefully trigger interest in other snakes and how their learning processes and defense mechanisms work and add information about a new organism to a vast body of research concerning animal learning and behavior. One goal in this particular study was to gather information about the little investigated Western hognose snake in order to compare its behaviors to those of the Eastern Hognose snake. An ethogram for the Eastern Hognose snake was developed by Burghardt (Cognitive Ethology, 1991), but not for the Western Hognose. The ethogram developed in this study for the Western Hognose snake consists of four stages; flight, bluff, immobility and recovery. The flight stage includes the time during which the snake gathers information about its surroundings, and attempts flight from a perceived threat. The bluff occurs when the snake begins to puff the head and neck area, starts to hiss and strike, which can be open or close mouthed, and the snake's body will begin coiling. The immobility stage includes a complete coiling of the body, tucking the head under the body, and a maintained puffed posture, which does not indicate any sign of breathing. The final stage, recovery begins when the snake resumes normal breathing, lifts its head, begins flicking its tongue, starts to uncoil and slithers away. This project undertakes the problem of learning in snakes, specifically the Eastern Hognose (Heterodon platyrhinos) and Western Hognose (Heterodon nasicus). Pavlovian conditioning was the method by which learning in the snakes was tested for. Pavlovian conditioning is a process in which a biologically irrelevant, arbitrary event, or conditioned stimulus is paired with a biologically relevant event, or unconditioned stimulus. The unconditioned stimulus elicits a biological response or unconditioned response. When conditioning has occurred, the conditioned stimulus elicits a conditioned response, which often resembles the unconditioned response. For Pavlov's dog, the conditioned stimulus, or arbitrary event was a metronome, and the biologically relevant event, or unconditioned stimulus was food powder. The unconditioned, natural response of the dog was to salivate in the presence of the food powder. When conditioning had occurred and an association had been formed between the sound of the metronome and the presentation of the food powder, the salivation response was elicited by the metronome even in the absence of food presentation. This was the conditioned response. Contextual conditioning uses a new context as the conditioned stimulus in which a biologically relevant event is presented in order that an organism forms an association between the context and the event. When conditioning has occurred, an organism will begin to show responsiveness as soon as it is placed in the new test context, even in the absence of the stimulus. Research has been done investigating the fertility of female Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) based upon the context in which access to a copulation partner was provided (Gibbons and Cusato, 2003). Females were divided into a paired group in which the access to the male was provided in the context of the test chamber, and an unpaired group in which access to the male was provided in the home cage. Females in the paired group showed increased sexual receptivity in the form of squats and produced more fertile eggs than did the unpaired group by the conclusion of the study. Aversive conditioning uses an unpleasant stimulus, such as a burst of air to the face whereas appetitive conditioning uses a positive one such as access to food. Operant conditioning trials often use electric shock with rats to condition the behavior of the rats in attempting to access food. This aversive stimulus is paired with an attempt to access food when a colored light is not on. When the light is on and the rat attempts to access food, the food is given to the rat- this is an appetitive stimulus. The electric shock encourages the rat not to attempt to access the food until the light is on, the light serves as a cue to the rat to attempt to access the food. The shock is an aversive stimulus, and the food is an appetitive one. Background Little research has addressed the issue of learning in reptiles, more specifically snakes. Burghardt asserts that studies testing for learning in reptiles are difficult to conduct using appetitive conditioning, as appetitive conditioning usually includes food as reinforcement for certain behaviors. Most reptiles need to eat only weekly and some even monthly, hindering the use of food as reinforcement. Some studies have been conducted with the use of heat lamps connected to a lever as positive reinforcement for reptiles, but this poses a problem in studies using snakes as they have no limbs to press a lever and activate the heat lamp (Burghardt, 1977). A 1988 study conducted by Burghardt and Greene using neonate Eastern Hognose snakes, age thirteen days, was conducted using aversive stimuli. Snakes were placed in a four-foot by four-foot sand filled arena. An experimenter gazed at the snake from a distance of 25 centimeters, after ten seconds the snake was lightly touched on the head or neck, after another ten seconds the snakeís body was lightly stroked, and after another ten seconds elapsed, the subject was lifted and lightly shaken. The recovery was monitored in the presence of a stuffed owl 20 centimeters in height placed one meter away from the snake at a height of 80 centimeters. This experiment was then repeated the next day and the latency of the immobility was measured either in the presence of a human gazing directly at the snakes, in the presence of a human gazing away from the snakes, and in the presence of a human crouched out of view of the snakes. The snakes whose recovery was monitored under the direct gaze of the human and in the presence of the owl had significantly longer recovery times than those that were monitored in the presence of a human gazing away or crouched out of view. This study established a direct correlation between the gaze of a predator and the recovery time of the snakes from their immobility response. Methods Subjects Subjects included four wild-born Eastern Hognose snakes (Heterodon platyrhinos) that had been in captivity obtained from a commercial dealer, and two captive-born Western Hognose snakes (Heterodon nasicus) also obtained from a commercial dealer. Subjects were housed individually in ten gallon aquaria with a log ìhiding areaî and a water dish, and each subject was identified by a letter corresponding to the species and a randomly assigned number. Subject numbers were; E1, E2, E3, E4 (for Eastern Hognoses), W1 and W2 (for Western Hognoses). Within species groups, each subject was randomly assigned to a paired or unpaired condition resulting in the following pairing; E2, E4, W1 as paired snakes and E1, E3, W2 as unpaired snakes. Length measurements for each subject are: E1 62.7 centimeters; E2 77.5 centimeters; E3 73 centimeters; E4 52.5 centimeters; W1 36 centimeters; W2 31.2 centimeters. Snakes E2 and E4 were in poor condition upon arrival, appearing to suffer from malnourishment and died soon after the start of the experiment. Apparatus All trials were run outdoors. An outside context was used because in a series of pre-trials snakes were unresponsive to the tactile stimulus in an artificial, laboratory setting. A pretrial conducted outdoors indicated that context was indeed a factor in the behavior of the snakes. The snakes' habituation to humans was overcome by a change of context from indoors to outdoors. An area of grass marked by stakes and yellow twine corresponding to the field of view of the video camera used was the arena for the test context. An area of asphalt identical in size, shape and orientation was indicated by duct tape. A stopwatch was used to monitor the amount of time each subject spent in each context, as well as to monitor the duration of the stimulus presentation and duration of the response elicited. A human tactile stimulus was used which included light touching, light tapping, lifting and light shaking of the subjects. Procedure Dependent variables manipulated were the species of the snake (Heterodon platyrhinos and Heterodon nasicus) and the context in which the stimulus was presented- the grass context or the asphalt context as well as pairing for the test context- paired snakes were presented with the stimulus in the test context and unpaired snakes were presented with the stimulus in a different context than the paired snakes. Independent variables to be measured are: tongue flicks, immobility, latency to immobility, immobility duration, hissing, puffing, striking, avert or empty of cloaca and head and neck flattening. During trials, paired subjects were removed from the home cages and placed in a new context; either a grass or asphalt context. Four stakes in the grass and yellow string marked the test area in the grass and duct tape on the asphalt. In these new contexts, a human tactile stimulus was presented during which direct gaze was maintained. Learning was also to be measured: do the snakes begin to respond to the context in the absence of the predator? The experiment ran for thirteen consecutive days, with a total of three test-trial days and ten conditioning trial days. All trials were videotaped. Days one, seven, and thirteen were designated test trial days in which each subject was placed in the grass context for five minutes without the presentation of the stimulus because it was the grass context that was being tested for a response in the paired snakes. Test trials were used to measure the snakes' responsiveness to the context in the absence of the stimulus over the course of the project. Subjects were transported from home cages to the outside contexts on their ìhiding logsî and were lightly shaken off the log into the test area. Contact with the experimenter was limited to the time for which the tactile stimulus was presented. Conditioning trials ran for five consecutive days between test trial days one and seven, and between test trial days seven and thirteen. Subjects were on a three day rotation schedule so that no subject was run at the same time for any two consecutive days to prevent temporal anticipation of the conditioning trials. Conditioning trials for the paired subjects consisted of a presentation of the conditioned stimulus- the grass context- for one minute before the presentation of the human tactile stimulus. The stimulus was presented until the death-feign or immobility response was elicited, and trials were deemed over when subjects recovered normal or full mobility. After the CS-US pairing was presented, the paired snakes were placed in the asphalt context for eleven minutes. In the unpaired condition, trials consisted of a presentation of the grass context for eleven minutes, an elapse of at least forty-five minutes to prevent the grass context from serving as a cue to the unpaired snakes, and the presentation of the human tactile stimulus immediately as subjects were placed in the asphalt context. Paired snakes were given access to the asphalt context and unpaired to the grass context in order to ensure that individuals were given equal exposure to each context and to the stimulus. The unpaired group served as a control in order to compare the response to the new grass context to that of the paired group, it was necessary to ensure that both groups were treated the same. The different contexts were used in order to ensure that it was an association between the context and the stimulus that elicited the defense responses of the subjects and that the defense behaviors of the subjects were not in response to the new context itself. The stimulus was presented until the death-feign or immobility response was elicited or for twenty minutes to ensure equal exposure to the stimulus across pairings. Trials in which the response was elicited were taped until the subject resumed full mobility. Results The data presented is from the second day of conditioning trials compared with the data from the last day of conditioning trials for each subject that completed the study. Eastern Hognose 1, Eastern Hognose 3 and Western Hognose 2 were in the unpaired group. Western Hognose 1 was the only remaining subject in the paired group at the end of the study. The figures presented show the latency to immobility Fig 1, and immobility duration Fig 2, for each subject on the specified days. From day 3 to day 12 the duration of the presentation of the human tactile stimulus decreased from ninety seconds to twenty six seconds in Western Hognose 1, the only remaining paired snake. In the unpaired group, where there was an immobility response, the duration of the presentation of the human tactile stimulus was increased suggesting that learning had occurred in the paired snake and had not in the unpaired snake. The response was elicited in the context much faster in the paired condition than it was for the unpaired. The duration of the immobility response, where the response was elicited, was longer for the paired snake than for the unpaired group. Discussion The use of an aversive stimulus in conditioning trials allowed not only the measure of learning in the organisms, but also an observation of the defense mechanisms of the snakes. Though preliminary data collection indicates that learning has occurred in the paired group of snakes, a more thorough study should be conducted using a larger sample size in order to account for individual variability. Given the opportunity to expand this study, it would be best to focus only on one species in data collection- to conduct the study using only Heterodn platyrhinos or Heterodon nasics. Ideally, neonates would be used as Burghardt and Greene have used in their studies in order to prevent adult habituation to human experimenters- this problem was overcome by a change in context in this study. Though there are many similarities across the two species, there are differences from which an experimenter would benefit by investigating each species individually. A more thorough study of the Western Hognost or Heterodon nasicus individually would provide for a better comparison of the species with the Eastern Hognose based upon the information already gathered. The best approach to further study would be to use individuals from two different clutches of neonate Eastern Hognose snakes in order to account for genetic similarity to prevent that a response not be due to the genetic origination of the subjects but in large enough numbers that individual variability to find similarities across all individuals in behavior is accounted for as well. Though the snakes used in this project were experimentally naive, they had habituated to humans and artificial environment. In using neonates, the study could be conducted in a laboratory setting without losing the responsiveness of the subjects. Also beneficial to a study of this nature would be a lengthier conditioning period and a posttest trial to determine the degree to which each organism retained the behaviors they had learned throughout the course of the study. The goal of beginning to develop an ethogram that maps the behaviors of the Western Hognose snake was accomplished in this study, however no conclusive results were reached on the question of learning in snakes. From the data available with this sample, it appears that learning in snakes does indeed occur and has occurred in this study, but more thorough investigation is necessary using more individuals in order that an effective comparison can be made. References Burghardt, G.M. 1977. Learning processes in reptiles. In: Biology of the Reptilia. Vol. 7 (Ed. By C. Gans & D. Tinkle), pp. 555-681. New York: Academic Press. Burghardt, G.M. 1991. Cognitive ethology and critical anthropomorphism: A snake with two heads and hognose snakes that play dead. In: Cognitive Ethology The Minds of other animals. (Ed. By C.A. Ristau), pp53-90. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers. Burghardt, G.M. & Greene, H.W. 1988. Predator simulation and duration of death feigning in neonate hognose snakes. Animal Behavior. 36, 1842-1843. Domjan M. 1996. The essentials of conditioning & learning. pp. 23-52; 116-129. California: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Ford, N.B. & Burghardt, G.M. 1993. Perceptual mechanisms and the behavioral ecology of snakes. In: Snakes: Ecology and behavior. (Ed. By Seigel, R.A. & Collins, J.T.), pp117-164. Gibbons, K., & Cusato, B. (2003, April). Contextual conditioning of sexual behavior in female Japanese quail. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Southwestern Comparative Psychological Association, New Orleans, LA. Greene, H.W. 1988. Antipredator mechanisms in reptiles. In: Biology of the Reptilia. Vol. 16 (Ed. By C. Gans & R.B. Huey), pp 1-152. New York: Alan Liss.