Running head: MEASUREMENT OF NEGATIVITY BIAS Measurement of Negativity Bias in Personal Narratives Using Corpus-Based Emotion Dictionaries Shuki J. Cohen New York University, Department of Psychology Corresponding Author: Shuki J. Cohen Psychology Department John Jay College of Criminal Justice 445 W 59th St rm# 2402 New York, NY 10019 Ph: (646) 557-4627 Fax: (212) 237-8930 e-mail: shcohen@jjay.cuny.edu Yale University School of Medicine Department of Psychiatry 34 Park St. Suite #B-38 New Haven, CT 06519 1 Measurement of Negativity Bias 2 Abstract This study presents a novel methodology for the measurement of negativity bias using positive and negative dictionaries of emotion words applied to autobiographical narratives. At odds with the cognitive theory of mood dysregulation, previous text-analytical studies have failed to find significant correlation between emotion dictionaries and negative affectivity or dysphoria. In the present study, an a priori list dictionary of emotion words was refined based on the actual use of these words in personal narratives collected from close to 500 college students. Half of the corpus was used to construct, via concordance analysis, the grammatical structures associated with the words in their emotional sense. The second half of the corpus served as a validation corpus. The resulting dictionary ignores words that are not used in their intended emotional sense, including negated emotions, homophones, frozen idioms etc. Correlations of the resulting corpus-based negative and positive emotion dictionaries with self-report measures of negative affectivity were in the expected direction, and were statistically significant, with medium effect size. The potential use of these dictionaries as implicit measures of negativity bias and in the analysis of psychotherapy transcripts is discussed. Keywords: Text-Analysis, Dysphoria, Negativity Bias, Personal Narratives, Corpus Linguistics Footnotes: The author wishes to thank Patrick E. Shrout and the late Carol Feldman and Joan Welkowitz for their advice in conducting this research. Also, many thanks to the numerous research assistants who assisted with various parts of this research, especially to: Julia Betensky, Venessa Bikhazi ,Candis Conover, Heather Holahan, Melanie Fox, Tyson Fur, Jonathan Kirschner, Jen Leeder, Julie McCarroll, Joanna Murstein, Jonathan Shaffer and Sam Zun. Measurement of Negativity Bias 3 Measurement of Negativity Bias in Personal Narratives Using Corpus-Based Emotion Dictionaries Introduction Bias in the processing of emotional information has been widely established as one of the most fundamental features of psychological disorders as of individual personality. Extensive research has repeatedly demonstrated that people high on neuroticism or negative affectivity selectively perceive, attend to, interpret and recall negatively charged or ambiguous events more negatively than normal controls (Watson & Clark, 1984; Clark et al., 1999; Rusting, 1999; Abramson et al., 1989; Ingram, 1984; Rector et al., 1998, Rude et al., 2003). In cognitive psychology, as well as in its psychotherapeutic applications, the cognitive overrepresentation of negative information is conceptualized within the framework of depressive self-schema (e.g. Beck, 1976; Markus, 1977; Segal & Ingram, 1994; Nasby & Kihlstrom, 1986). Evidence for the existence of depressogenic self-schema among both dysphoric subjects as well as clinically depressed patients has been demonstrated in experiments involving attention, recall, lexical decision, mood priming, and other cognitive capacities (for reviews see Isen, 1984; Mathews & MacLeod, 1994; Rector et al., 1998; Rusting, 1998; Scher et al., 2004). Negativity bias in the self-schema has been mostly studied using self-report questionnaires (Shaw & Dobson, 1981; Dobson & Breiter, 1983; Oliver & Baumgart, 1985; Beck et al., 1988; Clark, 1988; Haaga et al., 1991; Ingram et al., 1995; Chien & Dunner, 1996; Glass & Arnoff, 1997; Alloy et al., 1999; Winters et al., 2002). Self-reports have mostly demonstrated excellent psychometric properties and are straightforward, practical and inexpensive to administer. Further, self-reports have demonstrated theory-consistent Measurement of Negativity Bias 4 correspondence with cognitive content and schematic organization of emotional material. However, self-report questionnaires are also frequently criticized due to their susceptibility to the introspective limits of the respondent and to response bias, including impression management and self-deception or defensiveness (e.g. Winters & Neale, 1985; Polaino & Senra, 1991; Shedler et al., 1993; Gotlib et al., 1995; Lindeman & Verkasalo, 1995; Paulhus & John, 1998; Stone et al., 2000; Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000; Rude et al., 2001; Nosek, 2005). Moreover, cognitive theory regards the depressogenic schema as a stable cognitive style that is minimally affected by states of mood or depressive symptomatology, but numerous self-reports have failed to detect negativity bias in remitted patients under normal mood conditions (e.g. Persons & Miranda, 1992; Segal & Ingram, 1994; Haeffel et al., 2005; Fresco et al., 2006). One vehicle that was suggested to circumvent the high face validity of self-reports is the analysis of verbal behavior. Studies that have used autobiographic speech samples to assess the negative emotional bias of the speaker rest on the premise that a) the ability of the speaker to control word choice is limited due to the rapidity of fluent speech and b) that speech may afford an almost instantaneous account of the cognitive content of the mind as it reaches awareness, thus making it the fastest method of introspection. Negativity bias in the emotional content of spontaneous speech has shown consistent associations with indices of mental health. For example, Gottschalk and Gleser (1969 p. 106) found significant correlations between their measure of verbal anxiety and the emotional instability subscale of the 16PF, and Gottschalk (1997) found significant correlations between the same scale and self-reported anxiety. Other studies that used manual-based content analysis of spontaneous speech have also reported significant correlations with the cognitive negativity bias of the speaker (e.g. Viney, 1983; Measurement of Negativity Bias 5 Watkins & Rush, 1983; Peterson, 1992; Cacioppo et al., 1997; Davison et al., 1997; Hurlburt, 1997; Hartman-Hall & Haaga, 1999) Manual-based content analysis of spontaneous speech has been criticized for being timeconsuming, training-intensive and susceptible to rater drift and other rater-based biases (e.g. Hurlburt, 1997; Pennebaker et al., 2003). In contrast, computerized text-analysis can process large amounts of transcribed speech in considerably shorter time (Gottschalk & Bechtel, 1982; Pennebaker et al., 2003; Alpers et al., 2005). Moreover, several studies that used computerized text-analysis of multiple categories of words have demonstrated superiority of this technique over human raters in assessing mental health (Rosenberg et al., 1990; Pennebaker et al., 1997; Pennebaker & King, 1999). This study reports the construction and initial validation of an index of negativity bias based on context-based dictionaries for positive and negative emotional language. These dictionaries can be plugged into text-analytical software to obtain a metric representing the positive and negative emotional content of the speech sample. The construction process is described briefly, while a more detailed account is now under review. The advantage of these dictionaries, as well as the importance of contextualized dictionary entries for the computerized detection of emotional language are then discussed. Method General Overview: The construction of both positive and negative dictionaries followed several phases. In Phase 1, three research assistants independently detected candidates for emotion words in Measurement of Negativity Bias 6 transcripts of 240 narratives obtained from undergraduate students using standard elicitation technique. The resulting list was consolidated and then contrasted and completed using other text-analytical dictionaries In Phase 2, the context in which each of the candidate emotion words occurred in the narrative was examined using concordances specific to each word. This phase detected word combinations that are reliably associated with the use of the word in its intended emotional meaning. Conversely, this phase also detected idioms or word combinations in which the candidate words were not used in their emotional sense. To avoid the ‘dilution’ of the proposed dictionary, this list was subtracted from the final metric. Overall, each positive or negative dictionary is comprised of a list of words, word stems or idioms that constitute the emotionality metric, and an ‘anti-dictionary’ of word combinations that should be subtracted from the dictionary to ensure its purported emotional meaning. Finally, to test the concurrent validity of the proposed dictionaries, their association with two common self-report measures of psychological distress and depression were examined. The Global Severity of symptom Index (GSI) of the Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90) was used as a measure of general psychological distress, and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) was used as a measure of dysphoria.. Participants: Participants were 483 undergraduate students (345 female and 138 male) from a large private urban northeastern university who received partial course credit for their participation. All participants were self-identified as native speakers of North American English. The average age of the subjects was 19.8 years. Measurement of Negativity Bias 7 The participants were recorded telling a story concerning a recent disagreement they had with a significant other, and then filled out a battery of self-report questionnaires covering various aspects of their mental well-being and symptoms. Personal narratives: For the elicitation of speech samples, research assistants approached the participant with the following request: “I would like for you to tell me in your own words the details of a recent disagreement you had with somebody who is close to you emotionally. It may be a small or big disagreement, as long as you feel comfortable sharing it and it is with somebody who is or was close to you at the time. You have five minutes to tell your story, and it will be recorded on the machine in front of you. Five minutes is a long time for a story, so you may want to go into details. Also, I will not be able to help you along or lead you with questions. I will stop the machine when the five minutes are up. Do you have any questions? (subjects questions were answered at this point as succinctly as possible, although almost invariably the subject replied negatively to this question). Ok, let me know when I can start recording” (subjects to the experimenter when they were ready to proceed). The elicitation requests were delivered verbatim to avoid variability in the demand characteristics of the task. The lack of verbal interaction with the subject was also designed to standardize the task and preserve the unique narrative structure of each speaker. The stories were Measurement of Negativity Bias 8 then transcribed according to a common standard in the field (Mergenthaler & Stinson, 1992), using a 3-stage process to ensure quality control. Computerized Text-analysis: The text-analysis software was written by the author, and was comprised of a set of computerized routines, or modules – each allowing the manipulation and analysis of the text in a manner tailored to the task at hand. Like existing text-analytical packages, the software couls search for whole words, word combinations, stems of words and word suffixes. The software could also construct a concordance showing the immediate context of a given word, and presented the results as both counts and frequency of the searched items (For a solid linguistics and methodological background, including previous studies utilizing this technique see Sinclair, 1991). The software was designed as a programmer’s tool, rather than a user-ready package, to ensure its accuracy and avoid the blind suppression of error messages – a common practice in commercial text-analytical packages. The Development of the Dictionary of Positive and Negative Emotional Language: Phase 1: A group of 3 research assistants-- all native speakers of US English-- inspected independently the narratives of the first 240 subjects for positive and negative emotion language. The definition of emotion used in this stage was broad, to foster maximal inclusion. The words could include behaviors associated with emotions (e.g. yelling, crying, fighting, break* up, slam Measurement of Negativity Bias 9 (door), hang up (phone) etc), emotional states (e.g. happy, upset, awkward, surprised, and cognitive words frequently associated with emotional states (e.g. discouraging, appreciate, agree, understand etc.) Words, word stems or word combinations that were selected by two or more research assistants were retained. This list was then merged with a comprehensive list of emotion words from other, mostly text-analytical, dictionaries (Zuckerman et al., 1965; Stone et al., 1966; Anderson, 1968; Dahl & Stengel, 1978; Sweeney & Whissell, 1984; Whissell et al., 1986; Marchitelli, 1983; Marchitelli & Levenson, 1992; Pennebaker et al., 2001). The resulting masterlist comprised of 1,613 positive words and 1,927 negative words. After eliminating duplicate words, words that have not been uttered by at least two speakers in the training corpus (corresponding to frequency of below 1/10,000 words), and valenced words that connote emotions but don’t represent them (e.g. noise, anniversary etc.), the final list of word candidates contained 211 negative words and 161 positive words. Phase 2: In this phase, a concordance was constructed for each word in the list described in the previous phase. The concordance presented the rater with the context in which the word appeared, approximately two sentences before and after the word was uttered in the training corpus. Based on this concordance a set of exclusion or inclusion criteria was crafted for the use of each word in its intended emotional sense. In cases where no consistent grammatical structures involving the word could be established, and no inclusion or exclusion criteria could be discerned, the word was deemed ambiguous in its emotional sense and was eliminated from the dictionary. For Measurement of Negativity Bias 10 example, whereas words like pretty, like or kind, were included in the positive emotion dictionary of Phase 1, Phase 2 showed that their usage is too variable and is rarely consistently emotional to merit inclusion. Almost invariably, the word pretty was used as an intensifier (e.g. “pretty big”), the word like was used mostly as a conversational filler (e.g. “and I was, like, totally shocked”), and the word kind was used as a qualifier or a hedge (e.g. “it was kind of surprising to see him there”). The concordance for these words showed that they were used to convey emotional meaning in less than 2% of the cases. Acknowledging the vast ‘dilution’ that could result from including these words in the positive emotional dictionary, they were deleted at this stage. However, conjugations of these words that reliably and consistently denoted emotional meaning (e.g. liked, prettier etc.) were retained. Similarly, negations of emotion words were added to the opposite-valence dictionary. For example, out of the 47 occurrences of the word happy in the training corpus, 17 (36%) were negated, and of the 16 occurrences of the word bother in the training corpus 11 (69%) were negated (e.g. “it didn’t bother me”). Including these words in isolation could have resulted in considerable ‘noise’ in assessing the emotionality of the narrative. This phase represents a methodological hybrid between traditional computerized textanalysis and the more nuanced corpus-based concordance analysis. As such, the process described above has the potential of being more sensitive to local context and word sense. General Severity Index (GSI): The Symptom Checklist -90- Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis et al., 1976; Derogatis & Savitz, 2000) is one of the most commonly used and extensively studied instruments for the Measurement of Negativity Bias 11 assessment of psychological distress. The SCL-90-R evaluates the degree of perceived emotional distress caused by 90 common psychologically-relevant symptoms. Despite its theoretical multidimensionality, the relatively low internal consistency of the scale has popularized the use of its overall score, the Global Severity Index (GSI) as a measure of general psychological distress (e.g. Cyr et al., 1985; Derogatis & Savitz, 2000). Beck Depression Inventory (BDI): The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1961) is arguably the most commonlyused instrument for the assessment of dysphoria and screening for depression (e.g. Ritterband & Spielberger, 1996). The subject is asked to rate the severity level of 21 common symptoms of depression. The instrument has demonstrated adequate psychometric properties in numerous settings and populations (for review see Beck et al., 1988). However, while the instrument was designed as a measure of depression, a growing body of research has established its low specificity, as evidenced by its moderate to high associations with measures of general psychological symptomatology (e.g. Endler et al., 1998; Hill et al., 1986). This compromised discriminant validity may be particularly pertinent for college population (see review in Gotlib, 1984). Results Split-half reliability of the emotional dictionaries Measurement of Negativity Bias 12 Both Phase 1 and Phase 2 were based solely on the first 240 narratives. Therefore, it was important to establish the split-half reliability of the corpus by comparing the emotional tone of these narratives (i.e. the training corpus) with the rest of the stories. To accomplish that, the two parts of the narrative corpus were compared using t-tests applied to the proportion of positive and negative emotion words identified through the dictionaries. The proportion of emotion words was defined as the number of emotion markers identified by the dictionary divided by the number of words in the story. The training and the validation corpora did not differ in the proportion of emotion words (Positive Emotions: t(239)=-0.59, p=0.555; Negative Emotions t(239)=1.56, p=0.121, using 2-tail t-test under the heteroscedasticity assumption), thus establishing the homogeneity of the corpus. Concurrent Validity Checks for the Dictionary of Positive and Negative Emotional Language: The association between the proportion of emotion words captured by the dictionaries and self-report measures of dysphoria and psychological distress is shown in Table 1. Table 1 also shows the associations of dysphoria and distress with the proportion of emotion words as captured by two leading text-analytical software packages – the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (Pennebaker & Francis, 1996; Pennebaker et al., 2001) and the General Inquirer (Stone et al., 1966). For the General Inquirer scales, the positive and negative emotion dictionaries proposed here were compared to the “negativ” and “positiv” scales from the Harvard-IV Psychosociological Dictionary, which are comprised of 2,291 and 1,915 words of negative and positive outlook, respectively. The corresponding dictionaries of the LIWC were the positive and Measurement of Negativity Bias 13 negative emotion dictionaries. As Table 1 shows, the correlations between the corpus-based emotion dictionaries and the GI and LIWC dictionaries are significantly different from zero, thus conferring a modest degree of concurrent validity to the proposed corpus-based dictionaries. However, the magnitude of the correlations ranges between small to moderate, with no statistically significant correlation between the corpus-based positive emotion dictionary and the LIWC positive emotion dictionary. In contrast, the corpus-based positive emotion dictionary exhibits statistically significant correlation with the respective dictionary in the GI program. However, the effect size of this association is small, and the dictionaries share only 6% of their variance. Construct Validity Checks for the Dictionary of Positive and Negative Emotional Language: A crucial test for the construct validity of the proposed corpus-based dictionary is its correlation with measures of psychological distress or dysphoria. As mentioned above, the construct of psychological distress, with its related manifestations, namely depression or dysphoria, is associated with preferential processing of negative information (for reviews see Clark et al., 1999; Blaney, 1986; Segal & Ingram, 1994; Rusting, 1999; Rector et al., 1998; Martin, 1985; Teasdale, 1999; Beck, 2002; Scher et al., 2004) has not been demonstrated reliably in most previous computerized text-analytical studies. Table 1 shows the correlations between the two measures of psychological distress (BDI and GSI) and the proportion of positive and negative emotion words based on the corpus-based dictionary. As an adjunct to the analysis of the concurrent validity of the corpus-based emotion Measurement of Negativity Bias 14 dictionaries, Table 1 also shows the correlations of the proposed dictionaries with measures of distress as compared with the two other text-analytical programs mentioned above. As shown in Table 1, the corpus-based negative and positive emotions correlated significantly, and in the expected direction, with measures of distress and dysphoria, while the two other programs did not exhibit such a correlation. The exception to this general finding is the LIWC’s negative emotion scale, which correlates significantly with the BDI. To assess the statistical significance of the difference between the correlations of the self-reports with the 3 text-analytical systems, a procedure developed by Olkin & Finn (1990, 1995) for testing the equality of dependent correlations was used. This technique was preferable to the common methods of comparing correlation magnitude based on Fisher-test, since the correlations to be compared were derived using the same sample, hence violating the independence assumption inherent in the Fisher test. Table 2 the 95% Confidence intervals for the differences between the correlations of the text-analytical scales and the outcome measures, as calculated by Olkin & Finn (1990,1995) procedure. The analysis has shown that the difference between the correlations is statistically significant. Therefore, the frequency of emotion words detected by the corpusbased dictionary was significantly more associated with measures of dysphoria and distress than the two other text-analytical programs. Discussion This study examined a novel approach to the assessment of negativity bias, based on computerized text-analysis of personal narratives that considers context information. Incorporating concordance-based contextual information into the automatic identification of Measurement of Negativity Bias 15 emotion words is an attractive alternative to extant text-analytical methodology, which currently relies almost solely on decontextualized word lists. In lieu of amassing a large number of emotion words or their synonyms, and tallying their occurrence in personal narratives, this study showed that including simple usage-based rules to preferentially tally only words that are used in their emotional sense robustly augmented the validity of the text-analytical dictionaries. The construct validity for the proposed dictionaries was tested by examining their correlations with common measures of general psychological distress (GSI) and dysphoria (BDI) on normal undergraduate population. Both corpus-based positive and negative emotion dictionaries demonstrated improved construct validity as compared to the two most studied text-analytical programs, namely the LIWC and the General Inquirer. Several methodological steps were taken to maximize the ecological validity of the corpus-based emotional dictionaries. Firstly, the personal narratives in the corpus used to construct the dictionaries concerned a recent disagreement with a significant other. This type of narrative is highly frequent in psychotherapy, as well as general social settings (Horowitz, 1979; Biber et al., 1998; McAdams, 2001; Bohanek et al., 2005). Further, Ample research has shown that negative bias in processing emotional information is most consistently demonstrated when the information pertains to the self or a significant other, but not as consistently when it concerns generic person (Martin et al., 1983; Kuiper & Higgins, 1985; Bargh & Tota, 1988; Kuiper et al., 1988; Mangan & Hookway, 1988; Baldwin, 1992; Cacioppo et al., 1997; Collins & Feeney, 2004). Secondly, the relatively passive role of the experimenter in the elicitation of the narrative contributed to narratives that are similar in structure and content to those that are usually told to a generic stranger. These narratives also look remarkably similar to those found in transcripts of first sessions of psychodynamic psychotherapy or psychodiagnostic interviews. From the Measurement of Negativity Bias 16 psycholinguistic and socio-linguistic point of view, this level of interaction minimize the effects of familiarity, speech accommodation, politeness and other external influence on the subject’s speech (e.g. Bell, 1984; Hudson, 1996; Shepard et al., 2001; Giles, 2001). Thirdly, the speakers’ unawareness of our interest in emotional language was crucial to the elicitation of emotional narratives while minimizing the demand characteristics of the situation. To that aim, nowhere in the description of the experiment to the subject was there any mention of emotions. Akin to a clinical discourse, the request to talk about a disagreement with a significant other (rather than a topic of the speaker’s choice) was designed to minimize simultaneously both the social desirability demand to share with strangers harmlessly positive stories and the demand to mention or elaborate on emotions per se. Further, avoiding a direct request to speak about negative emotions arguably contributed to the validity of the corpus, as speakers could choose the emotional elaboration level with which they felt most comfortable with a stranger. Indeed, the corpus demonstrated a wide range of emotional elaboration levels, including a wide variety of psycholinguistic devices to minimize emotionality: narrating the factual chain of events, minimizing the emotional impact of the event, or even reframing it as a positive experience. The results of this study underline the usefulness of theory-driven emotion dictionaries for text-analysis of speech. The substantial correlations of the dictionaries with measures of psychological distress make them a natural tool for exploring the link between psychopathology and negative information processing bias using naturalistic speech as a medium. As mentioned above, previous attempts to link text-analytical indices of negativity bias with emotional or clinical presentation yielded mixed results. In general, theory-guided computerized textanalytical scales failed so far to demonstrate convincing correlations with either self-report questionnaires or clinical interviews (Bohanek et al., 2005; Pennebaker & King, 1999; Williams Measurement of Negativity Bias 17 et al., 2003; Stirman & Pennebaker, 2001), especially considering the robust correlations between human ratings of speech emotionality and mental health indices (e.g. (Davison et al., 1997; Lee & Peterson, 1997; Gottschalk & Gleser, 1969; Viney, 1983; Ruiz-Caballero & Bermudez, 1995; Rusting, 1999; Klein et al., 1986; Cacioppo et al., 1997; Hurlburt, 1980; Weintraub, 1989; Demorest et al., 1999; Luborsky & Crits-Christoph, 1990; Hermans, 1995; Pennebaker et al., 2003). In contrast, those computerized text-analytical systems that have achieved adequate construct validity when compared with clinically-validated measures are not theoretically guided, but rather based on empirical, criterion-keyed or bottom-up principles (Bucci & Freedman, 1981; Campbell & Pennebaker, 2003; Fertuck et al., 2004; Mergenthaler & Bucci, 1999; Oxman et al., 1988; Pennebaker et al., 2003; Rosenberg et al., 1990; Rosenberg et al., 1994; Segal et al., 1993; Spencer & Spencer, 1993; Stone, 1997). The dictionaries reported here may be the only theory-based measures to demonstrate robust correlations with indices of dysphoria and general psychological distress, comparable to those of human ratings of speech emotionality. The results of this study also underline the crucial contribution that contextual information can make to the validity of text-analytical indices of emotionality. Unlike previous text-analytical systems, this study identified contextual constraints on the text-analytical dictionaries using concordance analysis of emotional markers that was applied to the corpus of narratives (For an important previous attempt, albeit more limited and syntax-bound rather than usage-based, to disambiguate emotional sense of words in computerized text-analysis see Kelly & Stone, 1975). Concordance analysis revealed substantial sources of “noise” in the decontextualized word-lists used currently in text-analytical programs. For example, while the words pretty, like and kind are listed in most text-analysis dictionaries as positive emotion Measurement of Negativity Bias 18 words, concordance analysis showed that over 97% of their occurrence is not consistent with their intended positive emotional meaning. Rather, the word pretty is used as a quantifier, and like and kind (especially as the phrase “kind of”, which constitutes about 1425/1435 or 99.3% of all occurrences of kind in our sample), are used as fillers, filled pauses or hedges. Concordance analysis also found that although speakers tend to prefer the affirmative sense of emotion words rather than their negation, it is not rare to find up to 75% of the word occurrences in its negated sense – which may introduce an unacceptable amount of sense misidentification to the textanalytical endeavor. In fact, analysis of the current corpus has found that the negation of words like accept, agree, trust, comfortable and ready is substantial enough to add to the reliability of the dictionary of negative emotions, while the negation of words like bother contributes similarly to the reliability of the positive emotion dictionary. The results of this study may shed a new light on findings made with traditional textanalytical systems. For example, using the LIWC text-analytical program, low to no correlations were found between measures of emotional dysregulation and the frequency of emotion words used by the informants. Thus, Pennebaker & King (1999) found that in a sample of 841 undergraduate students, self-report measures of Neuroticism correlated 0.16 with negative emotion words and -0.13 with positive emotion words in their personal essays. Both correlations were statistically significant and both exhibited the expected direction, but their effect size was not consistent with the overwhelming array of models that identify negativity bias as the core bias underlying Neuroticism or emotional dysregulation. Similar meager magnitude of correlation was found between negative emotion words in narratives concerning the September 11 terrorism attack and self-reported neuroticism of the speakers, while no statistically Measurement of Negativity Bias 19 significant correlation was established between speakers’ Neuroticism and positive emotion word frequency (Williams et al., 2003). Consistent with these results, in comparing poetry samples from poets who committed suicide to those who did not, Stirman & Pennebaker (2001) found no statistically significant effect of negative or positive emotion word frequency in the poetry excerpts and the suicidality of the poet. Somewhat surprisingly, the above-mentioned studies, among numerous others, found associations between measures of emotional distress and personal pronouns that were equal or even higher in their magnitude than those between emotional distress and negativity bias in the use of emotion words. For example, Pennebaker & King (1999) found a correlation of 0.13 between Neuroticism and first person pronoun, while Stirman & Pennebaker (2001), using ANOVA methodology, found that elevated first person markers and lower first person plural markers are associated with risk for suicide in poetry excerpts. In a similarly naturalistic speech samples, Pennebaker and Lay (2002) found that New York City’s Mayor Rudy Guilani’s use of first-person singular, and not of negative emotion words, was consistently greater during times of personal distress. Several other studies using the LIWC found that pronouns were also meaningfully related to recovery from traumatic events, while emotion words were either unpredictive of grief response following trauma or exhibiting surprising patterns. Thus, Boals & Klein (2005) found that pronoun use in narratives of romantic breakup was highly associated with a self-report measure of grief, unlike the use of negative or positive emotions, and in a textanalytical study of the evolvement of chat-rooms conversations concerning princess Diana shortly after her death, negative emotions had not changed, while positive emotions were used in significantly higher level than control chats. In contrast, first person pronouns have changed steadily and predictably, with grieving writers using collective speech (i.e. high level of plural Measurement of Negativity Bias 20 pronouns and low levels of singular pronouns) to individualized speech (Stone & Pennebaker, 2002). The superior stability, consistency and relative robustness of the association between indices of emotion dysregulation and pronouns as compared to their association with emotion words led researchers to conclude that social-psychological and sociological theories of mental health may be more pertinent than emotion regulation theories of mental health. For example, Stirman & Pennebaker (2001) privileged Durkheim’s social integration theory of suicide over hopelessness theories of suicide, citing the clear association between higher first person singular markers and lower first person plural markers in poets who committed suicide, compared to no main effect of negative or positive emotion on suicidality. Similarly, theories of grief highlighted the collectivistic vs. individualized aspects of the phenomenon as well as the cognitive search for meaning, and downplayed the role of emotional experience as expressed by emotional language. With LIWC being the sole software of choice for text-analysis from 1999 onwards, numerous publications have since ‘confirmed’ the privileged role of personal pronouns in emotion dysregulation, above and beyond the expression of emotions. This impressive body of consistent findings created a sense of consensus regarding the relatively marginal role of expressive emotionality in emotion dysregulation, thus contributing to the phenomenological rift between clinical theory and practice, which focus on emotional experience, and social psychological theories of emotion dysregulation that favor constructs as self-involvement, social embeddedness, and repressive coping style as the most defensible empirically validated processes underlying emotion dysregulation (other LIWC-based studies that reported the same pattern include: Campbell & Pennebaker, 2003; Gill, 2003; Oberlander & Gill, 2004; Bohanek et al., 2005; Simmons et al., 2005; Mairesse et al., 2007, among others). Measurement of Negativity Bias 21 The robustly augmented associations between the corpus-based text-analytical dictionaries reported here and measures of mental distress urge caution in accepting uncritically the above mentioned body of research, however extensive and diverse. As Table 1 shows, introducing context to the internal dictionary changes dramatically its ability to detect emotionality, leading to significantly higher correlations with mental health indices (see Table 2 for statistical significance of difference). Further, since emotions are usually attributed to persons, with several emotions being attributable or relevant to one person, it comes as no surprise that pronouns are correlated with emotional processes or states. Further, the grammatical rules concerning pronouns ensure their relative ‘inertness’ to context compared to emotions. While we have a host of linguistic qualifiers to emotions (e.g. “very happy”, “kind of happy”, “not at all happy”, etc.), no similar contextual operators exist that could change the basic meaning of pronouns. Therefore, basic grammatical rules may explain both the involvement of pronouns in emotional narratives as well as its consistency compared to those of emotion words. However, using corpus-based emotional dictionary may reveal different relative significance of emotional language compared to pronouns, when narratives about emotional processes or states are concerned. The cumulative experience with other text-analytical studies adds to the caution with which the results of this study should be interpreted. As corpus-based scales, the proposed dictionaries may be of limited generalizability due to a plethora of factors, including: corpus-size, verbal exclusivity, speech genre and speaker demographics. Despite the fact that the corpus in this study included close to 500 participants and 380,000 words, past socio-linguistics research has demonstrated inconsistencies in the estimation of word frequencies that persisted up to corpus-size of xxx words. To assess the stability of the Measurement of Negativity Bias 22 corpus, the preliminary construction of the dictionaries was based on keyword concordances of only half of the narratives, while its validation was conducted using the other half of the corpus. Added stability to corpus-based measures comes from the use of dictionaries, or aggregates of words, to estimate emotionality. Text-analytical scales that are based on orthographic transcription of speech can only detect emotionality if it is explicitly verbalized. Thus, emotional information conveyed by nonverbal means such as intonation and prosody, however significant, is left undetected. Similarly undetectable is the avoidance of explicitly verbalized emotionality (e.g. irony, sarcasm, idiosyncratic figurative language etc.), which can also serve as an effective tool for expressing emotions (e.g. Labov, 1982). In the dictionaries proposed here, laugher and cry were the only non-verbal emotionality markers that were audible and unambiguous, and their inclusion indeed contributed to the accuracy of the dictionaries. Since non-verbal emotional cues are used more in familiar, informal and interactive settings (e.g. Brown & Levinson, 1987; Biber, 1995; Clark, 1996; Hudson, 1996; Baugh, 2001; Coupland, 2001; Eckert & Rickford, 2001), the current elicitation conditions may have minimized their effect and thus contributed to the dictionaries sensitivity. The proposed dictionaries, conversely, may prove less sensitive in different interaction settings. Lastly, following speech variations, the sensitivity of corpus-based measures may vary with the demographic characteristics of the speakers, including geographic background (e.g. Coupland, 2001; Ash, 2003), race (e.g. Mufwene et al., 1998), Socio-economic status, (e.g. Labov, 1972; Wolfram & Fasold, 1974), age and generation (e.g. Snow & Hoefnagel-Hoehle, 1978; Bloom, 1994; Eckert & Rickford, 2001; Tagliamonte & D'Arcy, 2007), and gender (Coates, 1998; Cohen, in press). Measurement of Negativity Bias 23 Although concerns regarding the exhaustiveness and generalizability of corpus-based linguistic measures are the first set of unknowns to study, the current dictionaries have already demonstrated the importance of context, however narrow, in determining the sense in which an emotion word is being used. The robust, highly statistically significant and valence-consistent correlations between the corpus-based emotion dictionaries and measures of psychological distress and dysphoria has the potential of rekindling the search for theory-driven assessment of emotion dysregulation. Better estimation of emotional experience also holds a promise for a more accurate evaluation of the relative contribution that self-involvement, social embeddedness, referential activity and other constructs that rely on the context-independent nature of speech particles (e.g. Spencer & Spencer, 1993; Bucci, 1997, 2001; Stirman & Pennebaker, 2001; Campbell & Pennebaker, 2003; Pennebaker et al., 2003; Fertuck et al., 2004) play in emotion dysregulation. Measurement of Negativity Bias 24 References Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theorybased subtype of depression. Psychological review, 96(2), 358-372. Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E., Tashman, N. A., & Steinberg, D. L. et al. (1999). Depressogenic cognitive styles: Predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 37(6), 503-531. Alpers, G. W.,Winzelberg, A. J., Classen, C., Roberts, H., Dev, P., Koopman, C., and Taylor, C. B. (2005). Evaluation of computerized text analysis in an Internet breast cancer support group. Computers in Human Behavior, 21, 343–358 Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 9(3), 272-279. Ash, S. (2003). A national survey of North American dialects. American speech, suppl. 88, 5773. Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relationship schemas and the processing of social information. Psychological Bulletin. 112, 461-484. Bargh, J. A., & Tota, M. E. (1988). Context-dependent automatic processing in depression: Accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 54(6), 925-939. Baugh, J. (2001). A dissection of style-shifting. In P. Eckert & J. R. Rickford (Eds.), Style and sociolinguistic variation. (pp. 109-118). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Measurement of Negativity Bias 25 Beck, A.T. (1976). Cognitive therapies and the emotional disorders. New York: International University Press. Beck, A. T. (2002). Cognitive models of depression. In R. L. Leahy, & E. T. Dowd (Eds.), Clinical advances in cognitive psychotherapy: Theory and application (pp. 29-61). New York, NY, US: Springer Publishing Co. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., & Harrison, R. (1983). Cognitions, attitudes and personality dimensions in depression. British Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1(1), 1-16. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the beck depression inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical psychology review, 8(1), 77-100. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571. Bell, A. (1984). Language style as audience design. Language in society, 13(2), 145-204. Biber, D. (1995). Dimensions of register variation: A cross-linguistic comparison. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Biber, D., Conrad, S., & Reppen, R. (1998). Corpus Linguistics: Investigating language structure and use. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological bulletin, 99, 2, 229-246. Bloom, P. (1994). Overview: Controversies in language acquisition. In P. Bloom (Ed.), Language acquisition: Core readings (pp. 5-15). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Boals, A. & Klein, K. (2005). Word Use in Emotional Narratives about Failed Romantic Relationships and Subsequent Mental Health. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24, 252-268 Measurement of Negativity Bias 26 Bohanek, J. G., Fivush, R., & Walker, E. (2005). Memories of positive and negative emotional events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(1), 51-66. Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 21, 1, 40-70. Bucci, W., & Freedman, N. (1981). The language of depression. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 45, 4, 334-358. Cacioppo, J. T., von Hippel, W., & Ernst, J. M. (1997). Mapping cognitive structures and processes through verbal content: The thought-listing technique. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 65, 6, 928-940. Campbell, R. S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003). The secret life of pronouns: Flexibility in writing style and physical health. Psychological Science, 14, 1, 60-65. Chien, A. J., & Dunner, D. L. (1996). The tridimensional personality questionnaire in depression: State versus trait issues. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30, 21-27. Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York, NY, US: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Clark, D. A. (1988). The validity of measures of cognition: A review of the literature. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 12, 1-20. Coates, J. (ed.) (1998) Language and Gender: A Reader. Blackwell: Oxford, Malden Masc. Measurement of Negativity Bias 27 Cohen, S. J. (In press). Gender differences in speech temporal patterns detected using lagged cooccurrence text-analysis of personal narratives. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). Working models of attachment shape perceptions of social support: Evidence from experimental and observational studies. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 87, 3, 363-383. Coupland, N. (2001). Language, situation, and the relational self: Theorizing dialect-style in sociolinguistics. In P. Eckert & J. R. Rickford (Eds.), Style and sociolinguistic variation (pp. 185-210). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Cyr, J. J., McKenna-Foley, J. M., & Peacock, E. (1985). Factor structure of the SCL-90-R: Is there one? Journal of personality assessment, 49, 6, 571-578. Dahl, H., & Stengel, B. (1978). A classification of emotion words: A modification and partial test of de rivera's decision theory of emotions. Psychoanalysis & Contemporary Thought, 1, 2, 269-312. Davison, G. C., Vogel, R. S., & Coffman, S. G. (1997). Think-aloud approaches to cognitive assessment and the articulated thoughts in simulated situations paradigm. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 65, 6, 950-958. Demorest, A., Crits-Christoph, P., Hatch, M., & Luborsky, L. (1999). A comparison of interpersonal scripts in clinically depressed versus nondepressed individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 3, 265-280. Derogatis, L. R., Rickels, K., & Rock, A. F. (1976). The SCL-90 and the MMPI: A step in the validation of a new self-report scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 280-289. Measurement of Negativity Bias 28 Derogatis, L. R., & Savitz, K. L. (2000). The SCL-90-R and Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) in primary care. In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), Handbook of psychological assessment in primary care settings (pp. 297-334). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Dobson, K. S., & Breiter, H. J. (1983). Cognitive assessment of depression: Reliability and validity of three measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 107–109. Eckert, P., & Rickford, J. R. (Eds.). (2001). Style and sociolinguistic variation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Endler, N. S., Denisoff, E., & Rutherford, A. (1998). Anxiety and depression: Evidence for the differentiation of commonly co-occurring constructs. Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessment, 20, 2, 149-171. Fertuck, E. A., Bucci, W., Blatt, S. J., & Ford, R. Q. (2004). Verbal representation and therapeutic change in anaclitic and introjective inpatients. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 41, 1, 13-25. Fresco, D. M., Heimberg, R. G., Abramowitz, A., & Bertram, T. L. (2006). The effect of a negative mood priming challenge on dysfunctional attitudes, explanatory style, and explanatory flexibility. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45,2, 167-183. Giles, H. (2001). Sociopsychological parameters of style shifting. In P. Eckert & J. R. Rickford (Eds.), Style and sociolinguistic variation (pp. 211-219). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Gill, A. (2003). Personality and Language: The Projection and Perception of Personality in Computer-Mediated Communication. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh. Glass, C. R., & Arnoff, D. B. (1997). Questionnaire methods of cognitive self-statement assessment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 911-927. Measurement of Negativity Bias 29 Gotlib, I. H. (1984). Depression and general psychopathology in university students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 1, 19-30. Gotlib, I. H., Lewinson, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1995). Symptoms versus a diagnosis of depression: Differences in psychosocial functioning. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 90-100 Gottschalk, L. (1997). The unobstrusive measure of psychological traits and states. In C. W. Roberts (Ed.), Text analysis for the social sciences: Methods for drawing statistical inferences from texts and transcripts (pp. 117-129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gottschalk, L. A., & Bechtel, R. J. (1982). The measurement of anxiety through the computer analysis of verbal samples. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 23, 4, 364-369. Gottschalk, L. A., & Gleser, G. C. (1969). The measurement of psychological states through the content analysis of verbal behavior. Oxford, England: U. California Press. Haaga, D. A., Dyck, M. J., & Ernst, D. (1991). Empirical status of cognitive theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 2, 215-236. Haeffel, G. J., Abramson, L. Y., Voelz, Z. R., Metalsky, G. I., Halberstadt, L., Dykman, B. M., et al. (2005). Negative cognitive styles, dysfunctional attitudes, and the remitted depression paradigm: A search for the elusive cognitive vulnerability to depression factor among remitted depressives. Emotion, 5, 343 – 348. Hartman-Hall, H. M. & Haaga, D. A. F. (1999) Content Analysis of Cognitive Bias: Development of a Standardized Measure. Journal of Rational Emotive and Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 17, 2, 105-114 Hermans, H. J. M. (1995). From assessment to change: The personal meaning of clinical problems in the context of the self-narrative. In R. A. Neimeyer, & M. J. Mahoney (Eds.), Measurement of Negativity Bias 30 Constructivism in psychotherapy. (pp. 247-272). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; Hill, A. B., Kemp-Wheeler, S. M., & Jones, S. A. (1986). What does the beck depression inventory measure in students? Personality & Individual Differences, 7, 1, 39-47. Horowitz, L. M. (1979). On the cognitive structure of interpersonal problems treated in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 47, 1, 5-15. Hudson, R. A. (1996). Sociolinguistics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Hurlburt, R. T. (1980). Validation and correlation of thought sampling with retrospective measures. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 4, 2, 235-238. Hurlburt, R. T. (1997). Randomly sampling thinking in the natural environment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 941–949. Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information-processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 8, 5, 443-477. Ingram, R. E , Kendall, P. C , Siegle, G., Guarino, J., & McLaughlin, S. (1995). Psychometric properties of the Positive Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 7, 495-507. Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition. (pp. 179-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kelly, E. F., & Stone, P. J. (1975). Computer recognition of English word senses. Amsterdam, Netherlands: North Holland Publishing Company. Klein, M. H., Mathieu-Coughlan, P., & Kiesler, D. J. (1986). The experiencing scales. In L. S. Greenberg, & W. M. Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research Measurement of Negativity Bias 31 handbook. Guilford clinical psychology and psychotherapy series (pp. 21-71). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Kuiper, N. A., & Higgins, E. T. (1985). Social cognition and depression: A general integrative perspective. Social Cognition. Special issue: Depression, 3, 1, 1-15. Kuiper, N. A., Olinger, L. J., & MacDonald, M. R. (1988). Vulnerability and episodic cognitions in a self-worth contingency model of depression. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression. (pp. 289-309). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Labov, W. (1982). Speech Actions and Reactions in Personal Narrative. In Analyzing Discourse: Text and Talk, D. Tannen (ed.). Washington D.C.: Georgetown University Press. Lee, F., & Peterson, C. (1997). Content analysis of archival data. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 65, 6, 959-969. Lindeman, M., & Verkasalo, M. (1995). Personality, situation, and positive – negative asymmetry in socially desirable responding. European Journal of Personality, 9, 125 – 134. Luborsky, L., & Crits-Christoph, P. (1990). Understanding transference: The core conflictual relationship theme method. New York, NY, US: Basic Books, Inc. Mairesse, F., Walker, M., Mehl, M. & Moore, R. (2007). Using linguistic cues for the automatic recognition of personality in conversation and text. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 30, 457–500. Mangan, G. L., & Hookway, D. (1988). Perception and recall of aversive material as a function of personality type. Personality & Individual Differences, 9, 2, 289-295. Measurement of Negativity Bias 32 Marchitelli, L. (1983). The language of the emotions: Couples in conversation. Unpublished M.A., University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley. Marchitelli, L. & Levenson, R.W. (1992). When couples converse: the language and physiology of emotion. Paper presented at the Society for Psychophysiological Research, San Diego, CA. Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 35, 2, 63-78. Martin, M. (1985). Neuroticism as predisposition toward depression: A cognitive mechanism. Personality & Individual Differences, 6, 3, 353-365. Martin, M., Ward, J. C., & Clark, D. M. (1983). Neuroticism and the recall of positive and negative personality information. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 21, 5, 495-503. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25-50. Mergenthaler, E., & Bucci, W. (1999). Linking verbal and non-verbal representations: Computer analysis of referential activity. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 72, 3, 339-354. Mergenthaler, E., & Stinson, C. H. (1992). Psychotherapy transcription standards. Psychotherapy Research, 2, 2, 125-142. Mufwene, S.S., Rickford, J.R., Bailey, G., & Baugh, J. (Eds.). (1998). African-American English: Structure, history and use. London: Routledge. Nasby, W., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1986). Cognitive assessment of personality and psychopathology. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.), Information processing approaches to clinical psychology. (pp. 217-239). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press, Inc. Measurement of Negativity Bias 33 Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 134, 565- 584. Oberlander, J. and Gill, A. (2004). Language generation and personality: two dimensions, two stages, two hemispheres? In Papers from the AAAI Spring Symposium on Architectures for Modeling Emotion: Cross-Disciplinary Foundations, 104– 111. Oliver, J. M., & Baumgart, E. P. (1985). The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale: psychometric properties and relation to depression in an unselected adult population. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 25–40. Olkin, I., & Finn, J. D. (1990). Testing correlated correlations. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 2, 330-333. Olkin, I., & Finn, J. D. (1995). Correlations redux. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 1, 155-164. Oxman, T. E., Rosenberg, S. D., Schnurr, P. P., & Tucker, G. J. (1988). The language of altered states. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 176, 7, 401-408. Paulhus, D. L., & John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic biases in self-perception: The interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of Personality, 66, 1025 – 1060. Pennebaker, J. W., & Francis, M. E. (1996). Cognitive, emotional, and language processes in disclosure. Cognition & Emotion, 10, 6, 601-626. Pennebaker, J. W., & Francis, M. E. (1999). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC [Software Manual]. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum publishers. Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC 2001 [Software Manual]. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum publishers. Measurement of Negativity Bias 34 Pennebaker, J. W., & Graybeal, A. (2001). Patterns of natural language use: Disclosure, personality, and social integration. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 3, 90-93. Pennebaker, J. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Linguistic styles: Language use as an individual difference. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 77, 6, 1296-1312. Pennebaker, J. W., & Lay, T. C. (2002). Language use and personality during crises: Analyses of mayor rudolph giuliani's press conferences. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 3, 271-282. Pennebaker, J. W., & Stone, L. D. (2003). Words of wisdom: Language use over the life span. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 85, 2, 291-301. Pennebaker, J. W., Mayne, T. J., & Francis, M. E. (1997). Linguistic predictors of adaptive bereavement. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 72, 4, 863-871. Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547-577. Pennebaker, J. W., Zech, E., & Rimé, B. (2001). Disclosing and sharing emotion: Psychological, social, and health consequences. In M. S. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping, and care. (pp. 517-543). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association Persons, J. B., & Miranda, J. (1992). Cognitive theories of vulnerability to depression: Reconciling negative evidence. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 485–502. Peterson, C. (1992). Explanatory style. In C. P. Smith (Eds.), Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 376-382). New York: Cambridge. Polaino, A., & Senra, C. (1991). Measurement of depression: Comparison between Measurement of Negativity Bias 35 self-reports and clinical assessments of depressed outpatients. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 13, 313-324. Rector, N. A., Segal, Z. V., & Gemar, M. (1998). Schema research in depression: A canadian perspective. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science.Special Canadian perspectives on research in depression, 30, 4, 213-224. Ritterband, L. M., & Spielberger, C. D. (1996). Construct validity of the Beck Depression Inventory as a measure of state and trait depression in nonclinical populations. Depression and Stress, 2, 123-145. Rosenberg, S. D., Blatt, S. J., Oxman, T. E., & McHugo, G. J. (1994). Assessment of object relatedness through a lexical content analysis of the TAT. Journal of personality assessment, 63, 2, 345-362. Rosenberg, S. D., Schnurr, P. P., & Oxman, T. E. (1990). Content analysis: A comparison of manual and computerized systems. Journal of personality assessment, 54, 1, 298-310. Rude, S., Covich, J., Jarrold, W., Hedlund, S., & Zentner, M. (2001). Detecting depressive schemata in vulnerable individuals: questionnaires versus laboratory tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 103–116. Rude, S. S., Valdez, C. R., Odom, S., & Ebrahimi, A. (2003). Negative cognitive biases predict subsequent depression. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 27, 4, 415-429. Ruiz-Caballero, J. A., & Bermúdez, J. (1995). Neuroticism, mood, and retrieval of negative personal memories. Journal of General Psychology, 122, 1, 29-35. Rusting, C. L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psychological bulletin, 124, 2, 165-196. Measurement of Negativity Bias 36 Rusting, C. L. (1999). Interactive effects of personality and mood on emotion-congruent memory and judgment. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 77, 5, 1073-1086. Scher, C. D., Segal, Z. V., & Ingram, R. E. (2004). Beck's theory of depression: Origins, empirical status, and future directions for cognitive vulnerability. In R. L. Leahy (Ed.), Contemporary cognitive therapy: Theory, research, and practice. (pp. 27-44). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Segal, H. G., Westen, D., Lohr, N. E., & Silk, K. R. (1993). Clinical assessment of object relations and social cognition using stories told to the picture arrangement subtest of the WAIS--R. Journal of personality assessment, 61, 1, 58-80. Segal, Z. V., & Ingram, R. E. (1994). Mood priming and construct activation in tests of cognitive vulnerability to unipolar depression. Clinical psychology review, 14, 7, 663-695. Shaw, B. F., & Dobson, K. S. (1981). The cognitive assessment of depression. In T. Merluzzi, C. Glass, & M. Genest (Eds.), Cognitive assessment. New York: Guilford Press. Shedler, J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M. (1993). The illusion of mental health. American Psychologist, 48, 1117 – 1131. Shepard, C. A., Giles, H., & Le Poire, B. A. (2001). Communication accomodation theory. In P. W. Robinson & H. Giles (Eds.), The new handbook of language and social psychology (pp. 33-56). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Simmons, R. A., & Gordon, P. C., Chambless, D. L. (2005). Pronouns in marital interaction: What do ‘you’ and ‘I’ say about marital health. Psychological Journal, 16, 932-936. Sinclair, J. (1991). Corpus, Concordance, Collocation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Snow, C. E., & Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Child Development, 49, 4, 1114-1128. Measurement of Negativity Bias 37 Spencer, L. M., & Spencer, S. M. (1993). Competence at work: Models for superior performance. New York:Wiley Stirman, S. W., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2001). Word use in the poetry of suicidal and nonsuicidal poets. Psychosomatic medicine, 63, 4, 517-522. Stone, A. A., Turkkan, J. S., Bachrach, C. A., Jobe, J. B., Kurtzman, H. S., & Cain, V. S. (Eds.). (2000). The science of self-report: Implications for research and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Stone, L. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). Trauma in real time: Talking and avoiding online conversations about the death of Princess Diana. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 3, 173-183 Stone, P. J. (1997). Thematic text analysis: New agendas for analyzing text content. In C. W. Roberts (Ed.), Text analysis for the social sciences: Methods for drawing statistical inferences from texts and transcripts. (pp. 35-53). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Stone, P. J., Dunphy, D. C., & Smith, M. S. (1966). The general inquirer: A computer approach to content analysis. Oxford, England: M.I.T. Press. Sweeney, K., & Whissell, C. (1984). A dictionary of affect in language: I. establishment and preliminary validation. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 59, 3, 695-698. Tagliamonte, S. A., & D'Arcy, A. (2007). Frequency and variation in the community grammar: Tracking a new change through the generations. Language Variation and Change, 19, 2, 199-217. Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Emotional processing, three modes of mind and the prevention of relapse in depression. Behaviour Research & Therapy. Special Issue: Cognitive Behaviour Measurement of Negativity Bias 38 Therapy: Evolution and prospects.A festschrift in honour of Dr S.Rachman, Editor of Behavior Research and Therapy, 37(Suppl 1), S53-S77. Viney, L. L. (1983). The assessment of psychological states through content analysis of verbal communications. Psychological bulletin, 94, 3, 542-563. Watkins, J., & Rush, A. J. (1983). The Cognitive Response Test. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7, 425-436. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological bulletin, 96, 3, 465-490. Weintraub, W. (1989). Verbal behavior in everyday life. New York, NY, US: Springer Publishing Co; Wenzlaff, E. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59–91. Whissell, C. M., Fournier, M., Pelland, R., & Weir, D. (1986). A dictionary of affect in language: IV. reliability, validity, and applications. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 62, 3, 875-888. Williams, K. M., Paulhus, D. L., & Nathanson, C. (2003, May). Personality Correlates of Emotional Reactions to 9/11. Poster presented at the 83rd Annual Convention of the Western Psychological Association, Vancouver, BC, Canada. Winters, N., Myers, K., & Proud, L. (2002). Ten-year review of rating scales, III: Assessing suicidality, cognitive style, and self-esteem. Journal of American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1150 – 1181. Winters, K. C., & Neale, J. M. (1985). Mania and low self-esteem. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 282-290 Measurement of Negativity Bias 39 Wolfram, W., & Fasold, R. W. (1974). The study of social dialects in American English. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Zuckerman, M., Lubin, B., & Robins, S. (1965). Validation of the multiple affect adjective check list in clinical situations. Journal of consulting psychology, 29, 6, 594. Table 1 Pearson correlations between the 3 computerized text-analysis systems and various measures of mental health Dictionary 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 N=483 1. General Severity Index (GSI) -- 2. Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) .76** -- 3. Corpus-based negative emotion .41** .41** 4. Corpus-based positive emotion -.27** -.26** 5. General Inquirer negative emotion .08 .05 6. General Inquirer positive emotion -.04 -.01 7. LIWC negative emotion .04 .11* --.17** -- .28** -.06 .05 .46** .24** -- -.04 -.05 -.00 -- .50** 8. LIWC positive emotion .02 .01 .06 .05 -.03 .01 .30** Note: *) p<0.05; **) p<0.01 40 Table 2 95% Confidence intervals for the differences between the correlations of the text-analytical scales and the outcome measures, as calculated by Olkin & Finn (1990,1995) procedure. Correlation difference Correlations Correlations with GSI with BDI N=483 Corpus-based negative emotion - 0.331±0.005 0.362±0.005 0.369±0.004 0.303±0.004 -0.228±0.006 -0.251±0.006 -0.293±0.007 -0.272±0.007 General Inquirer negative emotion Corpus-based negative emotion – LIWC negative emotion Corpus-based positive emotion General Inquirer positive emotion Corpus-based positive emotion – LIWC positive emotion