Reciprocal influences of physical function and cognitive inhibition in chronic pain states Boessenkool, L.S.J. BSc 3268519 Abstract Chronic pain is associated with deficits in cognitive function and decreased physical functioning, both significantly impacting daily life of chronic pain patients. Evidence for cognitive inhibitory deficits in chronic pain patients is mixed; research in this area is complicated by the heterogeneity of chronic pain disorders and the variety of tasks used to measure cognitive inhibition. Although the exact mechanisms underlying cognitive deficits in chronic pain are currently not known, processing of pain and cognition occurs in overlapping brain areas and significant changes in grey matter density and functional changes in these areas have been observed in chronic pain patients. Because physical fitness has been associated with neuroprotective effects and improved cognition in healthy adults, it has been suggested that improving physical fitness in chronic pain patients might benefit cognitive inhibitory ability. However, some evidence is available that cognitive inhibitory capacity mediates the relationship between pain and physical function, in which case development of clinical interventions should focus on improving cognitive inhibitory ability. Future research on the mechanisms underlying cognitive inhibitory deficits and decreased physical function in chronic pain patients should aim to clarify the direction of the relationship between these factors. Examiner: D.S. Veldhuijzen, PhD Second examiner: C.H. Dijkerman, PhD Introduction Chronic pain is widespread in Western society and interferes with normal functioning (Breivik et al., 2006). Deficits in both physical (Lichtenstein et al., 1998, Edwards, 2006) and cognitive (Moriarty et al., 2011) domains have been reported, but few studies have focused on the interactions between these domains (Solberg Nes, 2009). It is difficult to treat chronic pain effectively due to incomplete knowledge of the heterogeneous mechanisms underlying different syndromes (Breivik et al., 2006). Potential shared mechanisms related to decreases of physical function and cognitive inhibitory deficits in chronic pain syndromes are discussed in this review with the aim to create new insights in chronic pain states, inspire new research and improve treatment options. Disambiguating cognitive inhibition While cognitive inhibition is a prominent concept in cognitive research (MacLeod, 2007), performances on accepted cognitive inhibitory measures do not correlate strongly (.15-.23; Miyake et al., 2000, Friedman & Miyake, 2004). Tasks like the Stroop task, antisaccade task, Go-NoGo task, Trail Making Task (TMT) and Flanker task all appear to require the ability to override an automatic or dominant response (MacLeod, 2007), but theoretical and statistical approaches suggest that this ability does not reflect one generalizable cognitive inhibitory trait, but multiple interlaced functions (Nigg, 2000, Miyake et al., 2000, Friedman & Miyake, 2004). Three distinctions are suggested by one dominant taxonomy: whether a behavioral or cognitive process should be suppressed, the conscious effort needed to suppress the process and whether one is suppressing a voluntary or involuntary process (Nigg, 2000). Another prominent approach suggests that the origin of the interference being suppressed, internal or external, might be the dividing feature (Friedman & Miyake, 2004). To prevent excluding important studies, cognitive inhibition was broadly defined in this review as the conscious or unconscious stopping or overriding of any mental or behavioral process, in whole or in part, with or without intention (MacLeod, 2007). The acute noxious stimulation and cognitive inhibitory performance in healthy participants Mixed results have been reported on the relationship between cognitive inhibitory capacity and acute noxious stimulation in healthy subjects. Recently, Stroop task performance was reported to be positively correlated to the duration of immersion on a cold pressor test (CPT; Oosterman et al., 2010). Additionally, performance on the antisaccade task was associated -2- with better performance on a tone-discrimination task during CPT (Verhoeven et al., 2011), suggesting that there is a relationship between cognitive inhibitory ability and pain. Speculatively, better cognitive inhibitory capacity enables participants to maintain attention away from pain, resulting in longer immersion times in Oosterman et al.’s (2010) study and better distractor task performance in Verhoeven et al.’s (2011) study. A paradigm where the intensity of the noxious stimulation is varied, and participants are instructed to either focus on the pain or to distract themselves from it would be useful to determine the role of cognitive inhibitory capacity in pain perception. Another study reported that both painful and non-painful heat stimulation lowered performance on the Go-NoGo task, Flanker task and Posner cueing task compared to a nostimulation condition, but no significant difference was reported between painful and nonpainful stimulation conditions (Moore et al., 2012). However, non-painful stimulation was performed at 1˚C above the detection threshold determined for the participant, and painful heat at the temperature determined as the pain threshold. Because more intense pain is associated with greater deficits in cognitive performance (Eccleston & Crombez, 1999), painful stimulation at the pain threshold might be too weak a noxious stimulus to influence task performance disproportionately. Possibly, this study measured distraction by the stimulation, rather than the effect of pain on cognitive inhibitory performance. In summary, although cognitive inhibitory capacity appears to be related to pain perception, current studies does not provide sufficient evidence to determine the mechanisms underlying this interaction. For a better understanding of the relationship between pain and cognitive inhibitory processes it is necessary to examine cognitive inhibitory performance in chronic pain patients. Cognitive inhibitory performance in chronic pain patients Mixed results have also been reported regarding cognitive inhibitory function in chronic pain patients. No deficits were found in Stroop task performance in a sample of irritable bowel syndrome patients as compared to controls (Atree et al., 2003), or in a group of heterogeneous chronic pain patients as compared to controls (Oosterhuis et al., 2012). Fibromyalgia (FM) patients did show significantly slower reaction times on a Stroop task compared to controls, but neither significantly more slowing on trials where a prepotent response should be inhibited, nor significantly more false positives than controls (Veldhuijzen et al., 2012). These results were found to be better explained by psychomotor slowing as opposed to deficits in cognitive inhibitory function. -3- Psychomotor slowing has been reliably reported in chronic pain patients (Hart et al., 2000, Moriarty et al., 2011, Oosterhuis et al., 2012). To reduce the influence of psychomotor slowing, a Go-NoGo task was adapted for a sample of FM patients by including longer latencies (Glass et al., 2011). Indeed, FM patients did not show decreased reaction time or accuracy scores compare to controls, but functional magnetic resonance imaging data gathered during response inhibition in both groups showed that FM patients had significantly decreased activation in areas associated with inhibition, motor preparation and attention (bilateral MCC/SMA, right premotor/precentral cortex, right inferior parietal lobule, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and left lentiform cortex), compared to controls. Additionally, hyperactivation was observed in areas not normally active during cognitive inhibition (inferior temporal/fusiform gyrus). Speculatively, additional brain areas were recruited to compensate for the decreased activation in the areas regularly associated with cognitive inhibitory tasks, enabling patients to attain similar reaction time scores and accuracy scores as controls (Glass et al., 2011). Several other studies observe effects of both cognitive inhibitory deficits and psychomotor slowing in chronic pain patients. A sample of chronic pancreatitis pain patients showed significant decreases in performance on the Go-NoGo task and TMT part a (but not part b or errors) as well as significantly more intrusions on the Verbal Word Learning Task, and a significant decrease in correctly answered trials on the Stroop task (Jongsma et al., 2011). While the first two tasks use reaction time measures and decreased scores might thus reflect reduced psychomotor speed, the last two measures rely less on psychomotor slowing, suggesting suboptimal cognitive inhibitory function in this patient sample. Similarly, a sample of FM patients showed significantly more false positives compared to controls on a Go-NoGo task (Correa et al., 2011). Additionally, controls showed the usual faster reaction times to valid trials compared to invalid trials, but patients did not show this pattern, suggesting psychomotor slowing as well as cognitive inhibitory deficits in this patient sample. Besides the interfering effect of psychomotor slowing, another possible cause of the influence of chronic pain on cognitive inhibitory function is the variation in clinical pain levels. It has been observed that chronic pain sufferers with high intensity pain show decreases in task performance, while low intensity pain sufferers do not score significantly different than controls (Eccleston, 1995, Grisart & Plaghki, 1999). In summary, although part of the results described above might be explained by psychomotor slowing, decreases in accuracy suggest deficits in cognitive inhibitory function in chronic pain patients. Additionally, brain activation patterns in chronic pain patients during -4- cognitive inhibition suggest altered inhibition functionality. Lastly, it appears that higher pain levels are associated with reduced task performance. Cognitive inhibition and physical function in healthy participants Recently, it has been demonstrated that physical function and cognitive abilities are closely interrelated (Vaynman & Gomez-Pinilla, 2006, Hillman et al., 2008, Gomez-Pinilla & Hillman, 2013). Research in healthy older adults indicates that physical fitness correlates negatively with age-related brain tissue loss and resultant cognitive decline (Gomez-Pinilla & Hillman, 2013). Specifically, physically fit participants were found to have larger grey matter volumes correlated to better Stroop task performance compared to participants with lower physical fitness scores (Weinstein et al, 2012). Additionally, the mediating role of physical fitness in the regulation of autonomic nervous system (ANS) arousal is proposed to influence cognitive inhibitory performance (Solberg Nes et al., 2009). Research suggests that moderate increases of autonomic nervous system (ANS) arousal facilitate cognitive inhibitory performance (Magnié et al., 2000), while large increases in ANS arousal hinder performance (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908). Indeed, accuracy scores and reaction times on a Stroop task performed during high intensity physical exercise were significantly lower than scores during moderate intensity exercise (Labelle et al., 2013). Additionally, compared to pre-exercise performance, accuracy and reaction times on a Flanker task significantly improved on a second task, performed after heart rate had returned within 10% of baseline following intense physical exercise (Hillman et al., 2003). Shorter latencies and higher peaks of the P300 event-related potential were also observed during the second Flanker task (Hillman et al., 2003), which have been associated with improved cognitive inhibitory ability (Huster et al., 2013). Because physical fitness is associated with smaller increases in ANS arousal after physical exercise (Hautala et al., 2009), and stressful cognitive tasks (Heydari et al., 2013), it might mediate the beneficial effect of physical exercise on cognitive inhibitory performance in healthy adults. Indeed, physically fit participants improved accuracy scores on a Flanker task after moderate physical exercise, while the unfit participants did not show significant differences between scores (Hogan et al., 2013). The unfit group also showed higher levels of alpha wave coherence measured with electroencephalography during the Flanker task, which was speculatively interpreted as reflecting increased effort to inhibit incorrect responses. Additionally, variability in reaction times in response to a Stroop task increased significantly more in unfit participants in response to increased exercise intensity compared to fit participants, indicating -5- proportionally more difficulty to maintain performance during increasing ANS arousal (Labelle et al., 2013). In summary, physical fitness might mediate cognitive performance in healthy adults through its associations with neuroprotective effects and efficient ANS arousal regulation. Cognitive inhibition, physical function and chronic pain Recent studies have attempted to integrate the reduced physical function (Andrews et al., 2012) and cognitive deficits (Moriarty et al., 2011) associated with chronic pain. In a sample of heterogeneous chronic pain patients, physical function, cognitive performance and pain intensity were all found to correlate significantly with one another (Pulles & Oosterman, 2011). Results indicated that psychomotor speed, but not cognitive inhibitory capacity, mediated the relationship between pain intensity and physical performance. Additionally, pain intensity in older CLBP patients was associated with decreased physical function, a relationship mediated by TMT part b scores (Weiner et al., 2006). However, because the TMT part b is a reaction time measure, these results might similarly reflect a mediating effect of psychomotor slowing, as well as cognitive inhibitory capacity. In contrast, both chronic lower back pain (CLBP) patients and controls showed decreases in gait stability and upper body mobility while performing cognitive tasks during self-paced walking, but the CLBP group showed significantly larger decreases during a simultaneous Stroop task compared to controls. Decreases observed in patients during a control colour naming task not requiring the inhibition of a prepotent response did not differ significantly from decreases in controls (Lamoth et al., 2008), suggesting a role for cognitive inhibitory capacity in gait maintenance for CLBP pain patients. It has been speculated that movement is less automated in chronic pain patients, due to recruitment of additional cognitive inhibitory resources in an effort to protect the painful area from noxious stimulation during movement (Lamoth et al., 2008). Indeed, cognitive inhibition, as measured by proportional scores on the Stroop task and TMT, also correlated significantly with gait cadence, walking speed and aerobic endurance in a sample of FM patients (Cherry et al., 2012). Additionally, attentional capacity was found to be significantly correlated with gait cadence, walking speed and psychomotor speed (Cherry et al., 2012). These results suggest that gait abnormalities in chronic pain patients are influenced by both cognitive inhibitory ability and attentional capacity. In summary, there appears to be a complex relationship between physical function and cognitive inhibitory capacity in chronic pain. Physical function shows positive correlations -6- with both cognitive inhibitory function and attentional capacity. Psychomotor slowing is suggested as a mediator in the relationship between chronic pain and physical function, as well as cognitive inhibitory capacity. Discussion To improve treatment outcomes for chronic pain patients, it is important to increase current understanding of the mechanisms underlying chronic pain states. Interactions between pain, cognitive inhibitory function, and physical function are discussed in this review and the following main conclusions can be drawn: 1) cognitive inhibitory capacity might interact with pain in healthy participants but more research is needed to determine the nature of this interaction, 2) several studies provide evidence for cognitive inhibitory deficits in chronic pain patients in addition to psychomotor slowing, and 3) while physical fitness might mediate cognitive inhibitory function through neuroprotective effects and improved arousal regulation in healthy adults, evidence suggests a more complicated interaction in chronic pain patients. Below, the clinical implications of the mechanisms underlying the interactions between these factors are discussed, particularly relating to physical fitness as a target for treatment. Chronic pain states are associated with changes in brain anatomy and function, such as decreased grey matter density (Tracey & Mantyh, 2007, Apkarian et al., 2009). One of the most prominent explanations for cognitive inhibitory deficits in chronic pain patients is the decreased cortical integrity observed in areas such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Apkarian et al., 2004, Ruscheweyh et al., 2011, Absinta et al., 2012), that are involved both in pain perception (Tracey & Mantyh, 2007, Apkarian et al., 2005) and cognitive inhibitory tasks (Mortiarty et al., 2011, Solberg Nes et al., 2009). It has been observed that the intensity and duration of pain are positively correlated to grey matter loss in chronic pain patients (Weiner et al., 2006, Apkarian et al., 2009), although it is currently unclear whether chronic pain causes these changes or individuals with certain predispositions are more likely to develop chronic pain (Apkarian et al., 2009). Considering that grey matter loss is a normal part of aging (Fjell & Walhovd, 2010), and that physical fitness appears to have a neuroprotective function in this process (Vaynman & Gomez-Pinilla, 2006, Gomez-Pinilla & Hillman, 2013), possibly the decrease in physical fitness observed in chronic pain states modulates grey matter loss in chronic pain patients. The neuroprotective effect of physical fitness observed in healthy individuals is proposed to partly depend on increased synaptic plasticity due to heightened concentrations of neurotrophins released after physical exercise, most notably Brain-Derived Neurotrophic -7- Factor (BDNF; Vaynman & Gomez-Pinilla, 2006, Gomez-Pinilla & Hillman, 2013). However, the role of BDNF in chronic pain disorders appears to be more complicated. BDNF has been reported to have anti-inflammatory effects and can help reduce pain in chronic inflammation pain disorders (Gomes et al., 2013). In contrast, it has also been suggested that observed increased levels of BDNF in FM patients are partly responsible for the etiology of the disorder (Nugraha et al., 2012). Indeed, animal models suggest that increased excitability of the central and peripheral nervous system following initial noxious stimulation might be part of the initiation and maintenance of the morphological and functional changes associated with chronic pain (Masino & Ruskin, 2013, Apkarian et al., 2009), and this process might partly depend on BDNF signaling in the spinal cord (Biggs et al., 2010). More research is required to determine whether influence of BDNF in different chronic pain disorders might be neuroprotective before conclusions can be drawn about the possible usefulness of clinical interventions aiming to improve physical fitness in chronic pain patients. Functional changes, as well as anatomical changes, characterize chronic pain states. Chronic pain patients show increased resting state activity in the medial PFC compared to controls (Apkarian et al., 2009), and spontaneous pain is associated with sustained prefrontal activation (Apkarian et al., 2005). Interestingly, practiced Zen meditators have significantly higher pain tolerance scores than controls, which are positively correlated to decreased PFC activity and increased ACC activity during acute painful stimulation (Grant et al., 2011). In contrast, chronic pain patients show increased activation in both the PFC and ACC in response to acute noxious stimulation (Apkarian et al., 2005) and cognitive inhibitory tasks (Weissman-Fogel et al., 2011) compared to controls. Clearly, modulation of activation in both areas plays a pivotal role in pain modulation. Interestingly, little evidence is available for differences in ACC and PFC activation in response to different cognitive inhibitory tasks, or different subdomains of cognitive inhibitory function using different brain areas (Barch et al., 2001). Therefore, it remains unclear whether cognitive inhibition in chronic patients is impaired in general, or if a subdomain of cognitive inhibitory ability is affected. PFC activity levels might also influence cognitive inhibitory performance via the regulation of ANS arousal through the central autonomic network (CAN). It has been suggested that measures of ANS arousal might be a sensitive measure of PFC damage (Solberg Nes et al., 2009). Chronic pain patients indeed show increased variability in the time between heart beats compared to controls, indicating less efficient ANS arousal regulation by the CAN (Solberg Nes et al., 2009). Because small increases in ANS arousal have been reported to increase cognitive inhibitory performance, but large increases in ANS arousal are -8- thought to decrease cognitive inhibitory performance (Magnié et al., 2000, Labelle et al., 2013, Yerkes & Dodson, 1908), effectiveness of ANS arousal regulation might also predict cognitive inhibitory performance (Solberg Nes et al., 2009). As physical fitness improves ANS arousal regulation through smaller increases in arousal in response to both physical exercise (Hautala et al., 2009) and cognitive tasks (Heydari et al., 2013) in healthy individuals decreased physical fitness associated with chronic pain states might result in ANS arousal dysregulation. Another possible mechanism underlying the interaction between physical function and cognitive inhibitory performance in chronic pain patients is glucose depletion. It was observed that effortful cognitive inhibition on a Stroop task lowers blood glucose levels, that lowered glucose levels corresponded to decreased performance on a subsequent Stroop task, and that glucose intake before the second Stroop task restores performance (Gailliotet al., 2007). Glucocorticoids excreted from the adrenal cortex are important for glucose production and metabolism (Solberg Nes et al., 2009). Chronic pain disorders have been observed to be associated with dysregulated hypothalamic-pituary-adrenal (HPA) axis function, leading to significantly altered cortisol levels compared to healthy individuals (Crofford et al., 1996, Jones et al., 1997, Korszun et al., 2002). Speculatively, HPA dysregulation might contribute to glucose depletion in chronic pain patients, which might partly explain reduced cognitive inhibitory performance (Solberg Nes et al., 2009). Interestingly, physical fitness has been reported to correlate positively with reduced HPA response to physical and psychological stressors (Traustadóttir et al, 2005, de Diego Acosta et al., 2001). Speculatively, decreases in physical fitness in chronic pain patients might contribute to the increased cortisol levels observed in some chronic pain disorders (Jones et al., 1997, Korszun et al., 2002). Clinical interventions to increase physical fitness in these patients might be effective to restore neurochemical balance. Recent research in animal models for chronic pain suggests another approach that might be helpful to remedy glucose depletion. High-fat, low-carbohydrate diets increase ketone bodies in the blood, which can replace glucose as an energy source in the brain. Ketogenic diets have been shown to induce hypoalgesia by altering brain energy metabolism in response to acute noxious stimulation in healthy rats, and in rat models of chronic pain (Ruskin et al., 2013, Masino & Ruskin, 2013). Interestingly, while the hypoalgesic effect of the diet persisted, blood glucose levels recovered during the diet (Ruskin et al., 2013). It has been suggested that a ketonic diet boosts adenosine signaling, a neuromodulator with analgesic properties (Masino & Ruskin, 2013). Indeed, adosine agonist treatment increases -9- the analgesic effect of physical exercise in a mouse model of chronic pain (Martins et al., 2013). Research on the effects on the cognitive inhibitory capacity of chronic pain patients on a ketogene diet, where both ketogene levels and glucose levels in the blood are monitored, might result in helpful new treatment strategies. Additionally, a personalized physical exercise program might be helpful to determine whether such a diet might have similar analgesic effects in human chronic pain patients as were found in mice models. A more psychological explanation for cognitive inhibitory deficits in chronic pain patients centers on limited available resources which have to be shared by pain and attentional processes. This hypothesis is in line with the idea that pain and cognitive inhibition partly depend on the same brain networks. Indeed, attentional processes are also associated with PFC activation, and attention to pain specifically with the anterior mid cingulate cortex and insula (Weissman-Fogel et al., 2011). With pain demanding attention, less attention is available for the cognitive inhibitory task, resulting in decreased performance (Eccleston & Crombez, 1999). Speculatively, cognitive inhibitory capacity might be important for the ability to disengage attention from pain or prevent pain from capturing attention, improving performance (Verhoeven et al., 2011). Indeed, a dual tasking study showed disproportionate increases in gait instability in CLBP patients compared to controls during a cognitive inhibitory performance, but not during a control task (Lamoth et al., 2008). Acute noxious stimulation in CLBP patients resulted in faster and stronger contraction of stabilizing trunk muscles during arm movements, proportional to subjective pain ratings, while performing a cognitive task slowed trunk muscle contraction (Larivière et al., 2013). These results suggest that chronic pain patients reduce mobility to protect the painful area by recruiting additional attentional or cognitive inhibitory resources for motor planning, but fail to maintain these strategies when these resources are also required for cognitive tasks. In conclusion, the evidence for the neuroprotective effects of physical fitness, improved ANS arousal regulation, and reduced stress responses in healthy adults, as well as possible interactions with a ketone diet in mice suggest that improving physical fitness in chronic pain patients through clinical interventions might be helpful, however several factors should be taken into consideration. First, positive and negative effects of increased BDNF levels have been reported in different chronic pain disorders, and the role of BDNF should be clarified for specific disorders before clinical interventions targeting BDNF levels are piloted. Additionally, while reducing cortisol secretion in chronic pain patients with heightened levels might aid in restoring glucose levels, the effects of increased physical fitness on chronic pain patients with reduced cortisol levels are unclear, and might possibly further decrease available - 10 - glucose. Furthermore, the analgesic effect of the ketogenic diet is speculated to depend on adenosine’s effects on opioid signaling (Masino & Ruskin, 2013) As patients with some chronic pain disorders show increased pain, rather than opioid mediated analgesia after physical exercise (Nijs et al., 2012), such a paradigm might not be effective for all chronic pain conditions. Lastly, and most importantly, while increasing physical function through clinical intervention appears promising, one study shows that cognitive inhibitory capacity might mediate the relationship between pain and physical function (Wiener et al., 2006), rather than physical function mediating the relationship between cognitive inhibitory capacity and pain. Decreases in physical performance during simultaneous physical and cognitive inhibitory performance in chronic pain patients appear to support this idea. If this is the case, improving physical fitness in chronic pain patients will probably not improve cognitive inhibitory capacity, and clinicians should focus on developing interventions to increase cognitive inhibitory ability. To guide the development of clinical interventions, future studies should aim to provide more conclusive evidence for the direction of the relationships between cognitive inhibitory capacity and physical function in chronic pain states. Because physical exercise interventions in chronic pain patients are already being conducted to improve other factors, longitudinal measurement of cognitive inhibition, physical fitness, pain and morphological changes in the brain of the patients participating in such interventions would be useful. - 11 - References Absinta M, Rocca MA, Colombo B, Falini A, Comi G, Filippi M: Selective decreased grey matter volume of the pain-matrix network in cluster headache. Cephalalgia 32(2):109-115, 2012. Andrews NE, Strong J, Meredith PJ: Activity Pacing, Avoidance, Endurance, and Associations With Patient Functioning in Chronic Pain: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 93(11):2109-2121, 2012. Apkarian VA, Baliki MN, Geha, PY: Towards a theory of chronic pain. Prog Neurobiol 87:81-97, 2009. Apkarian VA, Bushnell MC, Treede R, Zubieta J: Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain 9:463-484, 2005. Apkarian VA, Sosa Y, Sonty S, Levy RM, Harden RN, Parrish TB, Gitelman DR: Chronic back pain is associated with decreased prefrontal and thalamic grey matter density. J Neurosci 24(46):1041010415, 2004. Attree EA, Dancey CP, Keeling D, Wilson C: Cognitive Function in people with chronic illness: inflammatory bowel disease and irritable bowel syndrome. Appl Neuropsychol 10(2):96-104, 2003. Barch DM, Braver TS, Akbudak E, Conturo T, Ollinger J, Snyder A: Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. Cereb Cortex 11(9):837-848, 2001. Biggs JE, Lu VB, Stebbing MJ, Balasubramanyan S, Smith PA: Is BDNF sufficient for information transfer between microglia and dorsal horn neurons during the onset of central sensitization? Mol Pain 23:6-44, 2010. Breivik H, Collett B, Ventafridda V, Cohen R, Gallacher: Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. Eur J Pain 10(4):287-333, 2006. Cherry BJ, Zettel-Watson L, Chang JC, Shimizu R, Rutledge DN, Jones, CJ: Positive associations between physical and cognitive performance measures in fibromyalgia. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 93:62-71, 2012. Correa A, Miro E, Pilar Martinez M, Sanchez AI, Lupianez J: Temporal preparation and inhibitory deficit in fibromyalgia syndrome. Brain Cogn 75:211-216, 2011. Crofford LJ, Engleberg NC, Demitrack MA: Neurohormonal perturbations in fibromyalgia. Baillieres Clin Rheumatol 10(2):365-378, 1996. de Diego Acosta AM, García JC, Fernández-Pastor VJ, Perán S, Ruiz M, Guirado F: Influence of fitness on the integrated neuroendocrine response to aerobic exercise until exhaustion. J Physiol Biochem 57(4):313320, 2001. Eccleston C: Chronic pain and distraction: an experimental investigation into the role of sustained and shifting attention in the processing of chronic persistent pain. Behav Res Ther 33(4):391-405, 1995. Eccleston C, Crombez G: Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychol Bull 125:356-366, 1999. Edwards RR: Age differences in the correlates of physical functioning in patients with chronic pain. J Aging Health. 2006 Feb;18(1):56-69. Filevich E, Kühn S, Haggard P: Intentional inhibition in human action: The power of ‘no’. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:1107-1118, 2012. - 12 - Fjell AM, Walhovd KB: Structural brain changes in aging: courses, causes and cognitive consequences. Rev Neurosci 21(3):187-221, 2010. Friedman NP, Miyake A: The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol 133(1):101-135, 2004. Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF, DeWall CN, Maner JK, Plant EA, Tice DM, Brewer LE, Schmeichel BJ: Selfcontrol relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. J Pers Soc Psychol 2(92):325–336, 2007. Nijs J, Kosek E, van Oosterwijck J, Meeus M: Dysfunctional endogenous analgesia during exercise in patients with chronic pain: to exercise or not to exercise? Pain Physician15(3 Suppl):ES205-13, 2012. Nugraha B, Karst M, Engeli S, Gutenbrunner C: Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and exercise in fibromyalgia syndrome patients: a mini review. Rheumatol Int 32(9):2593-2599, 2012. Glass JM, Williams DA, Fernandez-Sanchez M, Kairys A, Barjola P, Heitzeg MM, Clauw DJ, Schmidt-Wilcke T: Executive function in chronic pain patients and healthy controls: Different cortical activation during response inhibition in fibromyalgia. J Pain 12(12):1219-1229, 2011. Gomes WF, Lacerda AC, Mendonça VA, Arrieiro AN, Fonseca SF, Amorim MR, Teixeira AL, Teixeira MM, Miranda AS, Coimbra CC, Brito-Melo GE: Effect of exercise on the plasma BDNF levels in elderly women with knee osteoarthritis. Rheumatol Int 2013 Jun 6; [Epub ahead of print]. Gomez-Pinilla F, Hillman C: The influence of exercise on cognitive abilities. Compr Physiol 3(1):403-428, 2013. Grant JA, Courtemanche J, Rainville P: A non-elaborative mental stance and decoupling of executive and painrelated cortices predicts low pain sensitivity in Zen meditators. Pain 152:150-156, 2011. Grisart JM, Plaghki LH: Impaired selective attention in chronic pain patients. Eur J Pain 3(4):325-333, 1999. Hart RP, Martelli MF, Zasler ND: Chronic pain and neuropsychological functioning. Neuropsychol Rev 10(3):131-149, 2000. Hautala AJ, Kiviniemi AM, Tulppo MP: Individual responses to aerobic exercise: the role of the autonomic nervous system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33(2):107-115, 2009. Heydari M, Boutcher YN, Boutcher SH: The effects of high-intensity intermittent exercise training on cardiovascular response to mental and physical challenge. Int J Psychophysiol 87(2):141-146, 2013. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF: Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Rev Neurosci 9(1):58-65, 2008. Hillman CH, Snook EM, Jerome GJ: Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function 48(3):307314, 2003. Hogan M, Kiefer M, Kubesch S, Collins P, Kilmartin L, Brosnan M: The interactive effects of physical fitness and acute aerobic exercise on electrophysiological coherence and cognitive performance in adolescents. Exp Brain Res 229(1):85-96, 2013. Huster RJ, Enriquez-Geppert S, Lavallee CF, Falkenstein M, Herrmann CS: Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: Functional networks and cognitive contributions. Int J Psychophysiol 87(3):217-233, 2013. Jones DA, Rollman GB, Brooke RI: The cortisol response to psychological stress in temporomandibular dysfunction. Pain 72(1-2):171-182, 1997. - 13 - Jongsma MLA, Postma SAE, Souren P, Arns M, Gordon E, Vissers K, Wilder-Smith O, van Rijn CM, van Goor H: Neurodegenerative properties of chronic pain: cognitive decline in patients with chronic pancreatitis. PLoS ONE 6(8):e23363, 2011 [Epub]. Korszun A, Young EA, Singer K, Carlson NE, Brown MB, Crofford L: Basal circadian cortisol secretion in women with temporomandibular disorders. J Dent Res 81(4):279-283, 2002. Labelle V, Bosquet L, Mekary S, Bherer L: Decline in executive control during acute Bouts of exercise as a function of exercise intensity and fitness level. Brain Cog 81(1):10-17, 2013. Lamoth CJC, Stins JF, Pont M, Kerckhoff F & Beek PJ: Effects of attention on the control of locomotion in individuals with chronic low back pain. J Neuroeng Rehabil 5(13):13-21, 2008. Larivière C, Butler H, Sullivan MJL, Fung, J: An exploratory study on the effect of pain interference and attentional interference on neuromuscular responses during rapid arm flexion movements. Clin J Pain 29(3):265-275, 2013. Lichtenstein MJ, Dhanda R, Cornell JE, Escalante A, Hazuda HP: Disaggregating pain and its effect on physical functional limitations. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 53(5):M361-371, 1998. MacLeod CM: The concept of inhibition in cognition. In: Inhibition in Cognition (Gorfein DS, MacLeod CM, Eds.), American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 2007, pp. 3-23. Magnié MN, Bermon S, Martin F, Madany-Lounis M, Suisse G, Muhammad W: P300, N400, aerobic fitness and maximal aerobic exercise. Psychophysiology 37:369–377, 2000. Martins DF, Mazzardo-Martins L, Soldi F, Stramosk J, Piovezan AP, Santos AR: High-intensity swimming exercise reduces neuropathic pain in an animal model of complex regional pain syndrome type I: evidence for a role of the adenosinergic system. Neurosci 234:69-76, 2013. Masino SA, Ruskin DN: Ketogenic diets and pain. J Child Neurol 28(8):993-1001, 2013. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A: The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contribution to complex 'frontal lobe' tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 41: 49100, 2000. Moore DJ, Keogh E, Eccleston C: The interruptive effects of pain on attention. Q J Exp Psychol A 65(3):565586, 2012. Moriarty O, McGuire BE, Finn DP: The effect of pain on cognitive function: A review of clinical and preclinical research. Prog Neurobiol 93:385-404, 2001. Nigg JT: On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol Bull 126(2): 220-246, 2000. Oosterman JM, Dijkerman HC, Kessels RP, Scherder EJA: A unique association between cognitive inhibition and pain sensitivity in healthy participants. Eur J Pain 14(10):1046-1050, 2010. Oosterman JM, Derksen LC, van Wijck AJM, Kessels RPC, Veldhuijzen DS: Executive and attentional functions in chronic pain: Does performance decrease with increasing task load? Pain Res Manag 17(3):159-165, 2012. Pulles WLJA, Oosterman JM: The role of neuropsychological performance in the relationship between chronic pain and functional physical impairment. Pain Med 12:1769-1776, 2011. Roy N, Volinn E, Merrill RM, Chapman C: Speech motor control and chronic back pain: a preliminary investigation. Pain Med 10(1):164-171, 2009. - 14 - Ruscheweyh R, Deppe M, Lohmann H, Stehling C, Flöel A, Ringelstein EB, Knecht S: Pain is associated with regional grey matter reduction in the general population. Pain 152(4):904-911, 2011. Ruskin DN, Suter TA, Ross JL, Masino SA: Ketogenic diets and thermal pain: dissociation of hypoalgesia, elevated ketones, and lowered glucose in rats. J Pain 14(5):467-474, 2013. Solberg Nes L, Roach AR, Segerstrom SC: Executive functions, self-regulation, and chronic pain, a review. Ann Behav Med 37:173-183, 2009. Tracey I, Mantyh PW: The cerebral signature for pain perception and it’s modulation. Neuron 55:377-391, 2007. Traustadóttir T, Bosch PR, Matt KS: The HPA axis response to stress in women: effects of aging and fitness. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30(4):392-402, 2005. Vaynman S, Gomez-Pinilla F: Revenge of the "sit": how lifestyle impacts neuronal and cognitive health through molecular systems that interface energy metabolism with neuronal plasticity. J Neurosci Res 84(4):699715, 2006. Veldhuijzen DS, Sondaal SFV, Oosterman J: Intact cognitive inhibition in patients with fibromyalgia but evidence of declined processing speed. J Pain 13(5):507-515, 2012. Verhoeven K, Van Damme S, Eccleston C, Van Ryckghem DML, Legrain V, Crombez G: Distraction from pain and executive functioning: An experimental investigation of the role of inhibition, task switching and working memory. Eur J Pain 5:866-873, 2011. Weinstein WM, Voss MW, Shaurya Prakash R, Chaddock L, Szabo A, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey E, McAuley E, Kramer AF, Erickson KI: The association between aerobic fitness and executive function is mediated by prefrontal cortex volume. Brain Behav Immun 26(5):811-819, 2012. Wiener DK, Rudy TE, Morrow L, Slaboda J, Lieber S: The relationship between pain, neuropsychological performance and physical function in community-dwelling older adults with chronic low back pain. Pain Med 7:60-70, 2006. Weissman-Fogel I, Moayedi M, Tenebaum HC, Goldberg MB, Freeman BV Davis KD: Abnormal cortical activity in patients with temporomandibular disorder evoked by cognitive and emotional tasks. Pain 152: 384-396, 2011. Yerkes RM, Dodson JD: The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol 18:459-482, 1908. - 15 -