Lindsey Younkins Empirical Report 10/13/2013 Report on the Relationship between Fear and Anxiety It has been established through past research studies that fearful temperament is a risk factor for developing anxiety disorders, however the factors that mediate the relationship between fearful temperament and anxiety disorders have not yet been established. In the examined article, the author attempts to explain the role of context in the development of a dysregulated fear response (Buss, 2011). Using a prospective longitudinal design, the author examines how toddlers respond to multiple low-threat situations and predicts that toddlers whom exhibit high fear response across situations will be more likely to show higher levels of anxiety when measured upon kindergarten entry. The sample was composed of 111 male and female, predominantly white, middle class children from intact families, between the ages of 18 and 30 months at first measurement. The experimenter compiled a ‘low-risk’ sample to control for other risk factors of anxiety disorders such as poverty and parental divorce. To test the hypothesis that fearful behavior in low-threat situations is a risk factor for developing anxiety disorders, the experimenter designed several scenarios that are mildly fear provoking, to gauge how the toddler would respond to strangers, unfamiliar objects, novel activities, and new social situations. Each toddler was subjected to twelve situations in a randomized order; five of which were designed to elicit positive emotions (such as free play with toys), and seven of which were designed to be mildly threatening and provoke fear behaviors (such as being approached by a stranger). Behavior was analyzed second by second in order to accurately measure fear and sadness in both behavior and emotional expression. Fearful behaviors were defined as freezing, diminished play, and withdrawal behaviors. Sad behaviors were defined as shoulder slumping, head bowing, and crying. Fearful expression was defined as brows raised and drawn together, eyelids raised or tense, and mouth open with corners pulled back. Sad expression was defined as brows raised at inner corners and lowered at outer corners, eyes narrowed, cheeks raised, and corners of the mouth pulled down or out. Behavior was analyzed for both duration (how long the behavior lasted) and latency (how long it took for the behavior to occur). In addition to the lab visit, mothers completed the Infant Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment at ages two and three. The author examined the items designed to assess general anxiety, separation anxiety, depression, withdrawal, and inhibition to novelty. At age four, mothers were asked to complete a Child Behavior Check List in order to assess behavioral and emotional problems present in the past six months, and given the hypothesis the author focused on the internalizing aspect of the measure. Upon kindergarten entry, both mothers and teachers were asked to complete the internalizing dimension of the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire, with items designed to assess depression, overanxiousness, separation anxiety, social withdrawal, and social inhibition. Consequent to conducting statistical analysis, the author determined if the toddlers that exhibited a dysregulated fear response also exhibited anxiety symptoms in kindergarten. In order to determine which toddlers displayed high fear in low-threat situations, the researcher first ordered the fear-provoking situations into three categories; high threat (where children were exposed to a motorized robot and spider), moderate threat (where one stranger approached the child and another stranger worked passively in the same room), and low threat (where children were exposed to a puppet show and a clown). The author defined high fear as scoring one standard deviation above the mean for fearful behavior and emotional expression and found that there was a small proportion of toddlers that exhibited high fear throughout all situations, noting that these toddlers exhibited high fear in the low threat situations. The author used a combination of three approaches to isolate the toddlers who demonstrated high fear across situations including the process described above, Latent Profile Analysis to identify the high-fear group, and identification of the toddlers that showed little or no increase in fear as threat increased. There were eight toddlers identified that fell into all three categories. To determine the association between fearful temperament at age two and anxiety at age five, the researcher ran an analysis of variance between each extreme group classification. The researcher found that toddlers that exhibited high-fear in low-threat situations later exhibited high anxiety scores at age five, which supports the hypothesis. Interestingly, toddlers who exhibited high-fear in highthreat situations had insignificant anxiety scores when measured at age five, again supporting the authors hypothesis that it is not important how much fear is expressed, but the situations in which fear is expressed that later predicts anxiety. The results implicate that the inability to regulate fear across contexts is a risk factor for developing an anxiety disorder. This research can be used to identify children with a greater risk of developing an anxiety disorder by determining which toddlers exhibit high fear in low-threat situations. The examined study could be extended by continuing research on the sample as they progress through school, to determine whether or not their anxious behaviors are persistent. By continuing to measure this sample of children throughout their education, there may be presenting evidence that anxiety symptoms diminish as the child progresses through school. It could be that the anxiety reported in the beginning of kindergarten was due to difficulties with school adjustment, and that these children merely need more time to adjust than their peers, thus appearing more anxious. By measuring the children again, further into their education, we can determine if their anxiety is related to fearful temperament or if it is merely due to difficulties with school adjustment. In the discussion, the author brought up another point that warrants further research; the fear and anxiety expressed for some of the children may only be elicited by social situations. Many of the situations that the toddlers were exposed to involve a social theme designed to prompt the child towards interaction, and social withdrawal was a feature component on the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire used by parents and teachers after kindergarten entry. It is a possibility that social competency moderates the relationship between fearful temperament and anxiety, meaning that if social competencies are present despite a fearful temperament, the child will show lower anxiety symptoms later in life. Alternatively, the lack of social competencies may put a child with fearful temperament at greater risk for the development of an anxiety disorder. This research study, like all others, is not without its limitations. The ‘low-risk’ sample comes with the cost of low generalizability. The author chose to conduct this study on a sample seemingly free of risk factors for anxiety disorders (poverty, parental divorce) but by doing so made the results less applicable to the general population. Additionally, anxiety symptoms displayed after entering kindergarten were based on parent and teacher reports, and therefore may be more subjective and less accurate than a clinician’s perspective. Despite these limitations, the researcher found evidence to support that a dysregulated fear response, shown by high fear in low-threat situations, is a risk factor for the developing an anxiety disorder. Based upon the discussed results, it can be hypothesized that anxiety disorders develop due to a maladaptive fear response across contexts. Robert Weis stated in his text that fear is a component of anxiety, so it is plausible that the inability to regulate fear could lead to the development of an anxiety disorder (2008, p. 448). It was also stated that anxiety disorders are the most frequently given diagnosis in children and adolescents, and currently about five percent of all youth are afflicted with an anxiety disorder. This high prevalence should serve as a call to action; more research is needed to better understand the causes of anxiety disorders and validate the effectiveness of interventions. The results from this study cannot only be used for early identification of at risk toddlers, but also form a foundation for further research. This study could inspire researchers to conduct a quasi-experimental study to gather more information on the etiology of anxiety disorders. This study could also facilitate the development and evaluation of efficacious treatments. The results can be applied to interventions by focusing on correcting maladaptive emotional regulation by teaching children to restructure their cognitions and appropriately re-label their emotions. By outlining a developmental trajectory of anxiety disorders, this study provides information applicable to interventions and further research. References Buss, K.A. (2011). Which fearful toddlers should we worry about? Context, fear, regulation, and anxiety risk. Developmental Psychology, 47(3), 804-819. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023227 Weiss, R. (2008). Abnormal child and adolescent psychology. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications.