RNIB Centre for Accessible Information (CAI) Literature review #7 Exploring the need for accessible images for people with dyslexia Published by: RNIB Centre for Accessible Information (CAI), 58-72 John Bright Street, Birmingham, B1 1BN, UK Commissioned by: As publisher Author: Heather Cryer (Research Officer, CAI) Tel: 0121 665 4211 Email: heather.cryer@rnib.org.uk Date: 30 May 2013 Document reference: CAI-LR7 [05-2013] Sensitivity: Internal and full public access Copyright: RNIB 2013 © RNIB 2013 Citation guidance: Cryer, H. (2013). Exploring the need for accessible images for people with dyslexia. RNIB Centre for Accessible Information, Birmingham: Literature review #7. Acknowledgements: Sarah Home Sarah Morley Wilkins John Rack (Head of Research, Development and Policy, Dyslexia Action) Sue Partridge (Associate of Dyslexia Positive) 2 © RNIB 2013 Exploring the need for accessible images for people with dyslexia RNIB Centre for Accessible Information (CAI) Prepared by: Heather Cryer (Research Officer, CAI) FINAL version 30 May 2013 Table of contents Introduction ............................................................................ 3 About dyslexia ........................................................................ 4 Dyslexia, creativity and visual-spatial skills ......................... 5 The dyslexic advantage ...................................................... 7 Dyslexia and visual learning ............................................... 8 Conclusion ............................................................................. 9 References ........................................................................... 10 Executive summary Anecdotal evidence suggests that some dyslexic people may find accessible images (such as simplified tactile or large print diagrams) helpful to their learning. This study aimed to investigate evidence around how dyslexic people use images, to understand whether accessing images can be a problem in dyslexia, and to determine whether RNIB’s accessible images could help. Little evidence was found relating to the use of images by dyslexics. However, a body of evidence linking dyslexia to creativity and visual-spatial skills was found, which may suggest that some dyslexics could perform very well at some image processing tasks, such as identifying ‘impossible figures’. These findings are linked to the idea that dyslexia is characterised by advantages and disadvantages and can be seen as a different pattern of brain organisation. Some work suggests that one characteristic of dyslexic processing may be attending more to information presented in their peripheral vision rather than central vision. This could explain difficulties with reading print – which requires clear 3 © RNIB 2013 focus on a small, central area, and could also explain difficulty processing complex, detailed images. Overall, the evidence is mixed, suggesting that whilst some dyslexics may excel in creative fields and certain image processing tasks, there may be others who struggle to process complex images. Further research is required to investigate whether accessible images could be useful to dyslexic learners. Introduction The definition of print disabled is given as: "A person who cannot effectively read print because of a visual, physical, perceptual, developmental, cognitive, or learning disability" (Reading Rights Coalition, 2012). RNIB has been working in partnership with Dyslexia Action to provide easy access to educational material for print disabled learners, through the Load2Learn project (www.Load2learn.org.uk). Load2Learn is an online collection of downloadable accessible textbooks and images to support print disabled learners. The images available on the site are specially designed 'accessible images', aimed originally at blind and partially sighted people. The images can be produced in either large print or in tactile form to be felt with the fingers. Despite being aimed at blind and partially sighted people, anecdotal evidence from project evaluations suggests that the accessible images may be being used and appreciated by learners with other print disabilities, such as dyslexia. Atkins and Walker (2012) report a case study of a dyslexic learner who felt he could benefit from tactile and large print images. The student reported struggling to take in information from diagrams with too much detail and liked how the accessible images built up detail across a number of images. He also enjoyed using the tactile images and thought the textures could help him to remember different parts of the diagram. The purpose of this literature review was to explore whether there is any research evidence to suggest that people with dyslexia may struggle with images, and whether the accessible images produced for Load2Learn could benefit this group. About dyslexia Dyslexia is a learning difficulty that primarily affects the skills involved in accurate and fluent word reading and spelling (Rose, 4 © RNIB 2013 2009, pp30). Dyslexia affects people of all intellectual abilities, and can be seen as a continuum rather than a clear cut category (Rose, 2009). Dyslexia is characterised by below-average reading speed, poor attention, difficulty with sequencing tasks, disorganisation and low working memory (Coppin, 2009). Whilst dyslexia is often associated with difficulty reading, sometimes this is not the main issue. Dyslexia has also been described as a difference in the way the brain is organised (Klein, 2003), which leads to both difficulties and abilities (Eide and Eide, 2011). Whilst there was little evidence relating to the use of images in general by dyslexic people, a body of literature was uncovered studying the link between dyslexia, creativity and visual-spatial skills. Dyslexia, creativity and visual-spatial skills Anecdotal evidence suggests that dyslexics are over-represented in training programmes and professions involving creative or spatial skills, such as art, design, architecture and engineering (for example, see Eide and Eide, 2011). It is unclear whether this is a result of avoiding subjects/professions which may require more reading (von Karolyi and Winner, 2004), or whether it reflects a tendency for dyslexics to be skilled in these areas. Other possible evidence pointing to a connection between dyslexia and visual-spatial skills is that various famous people with clear talents in such fields have been reported as having dyslexic traits or reading difficulties (see West, 2009). These include Thomas Edison, Albert Einstein and Leonardo Da Vinci. However, this does not provide firm evidence of a connection between dyslexia and creativity, as such observations fail to account for the many creative people who don’t show signs of dyslexia (Winner, von Karolyi, Malinsky, French, Seliger, Ross and Weber, 2001). Research into a possible connection between dyslexia and visualspatial skills has produced mixed results. Riley, Rankin, Brunswick, McManus, Chamberlain and Loo (2009) found no relationship between dyslexia and drawing ability. Brunswick, Martin, and Marzano (2010) tested a variety of ‘real world’ visual-spatial abilities. These included testing commonplace 5 © RNIB 2013 visual-spatial knowledge (recall of which way the Queen’s head faces on postage stamps), memory of a virtual environment (in which participants explored a virtual environment and were then asked to reconstruct it using models), and an incomplete figures test (in which simple line drawings are presented gradually on a computer and participants press a key when they can identify the image). Dyslexic and control participants were compared on performance on these tests. Overall there were no significant differences found between the groups, although overall dyslexic men performed best. The researchers suggest this could be a result of men having greater visual-spatial skills in general, though felt further research was required to better understand the findings. Winner et al (2001) carried out a series of studies with dyslexic students measuring a wide range of spatial abilities compared to controls. Measures included tests of mental rotation, visual memory, visual search, spatial orientation, spatial visualisation, drawing ability, spatial word problems and real-world spatial visualisation. On all of these tests, dyslexics did not perform significantly better than controls, and in various cases they performed worse. The only task on which dyslexics performed better than controls was a test of real-world visualisation (an engineering problem), though this result did not reach statistical significance. Winner et al conclude that further research is required with more participants and a wider range of tasks. Further research by von Karolyi (2001) found dyslexics to be significantly faster in identifying ‘impossible figures’ than controls, and that their speed did not affect their accuracy. Impossible figures are figures which appear three dimensional but could not exist is reality. The ability to identify such figures relies on global processing of the image to recognise that it is impossible. These findings were replicated by von Karolyi, Winner, Gray and Sherman (2003), who concluded that the findings suggest dyslexics to be superior at global visual-spatial processing, or perceiving the ‘big picture’. These findings are consistent with anecdotal evidence that dyslexics have a special ability to ‘see things differently’ (West, 2003). Murphy (2011) discusses this trait in relation to a career in Radiography, suggesting some dyslexic students felt they had advantages over other students in terms of spatial awareness and 6 © RNIB 2013 identification. Overall, these findings may suggest that some dyslexics could have superior skills at image processing. The dyslexic advantage Some researchers believe that dyslexia is characterised not only by deficits in the area of reading, but also by benefits in other areas. Very early research into dyslexia (Orton, 1925 cited in von Karolyi and Winner, 2004) suggested some link between dyslexia and visual-spatial talents. Geschwind (1982, cited in West, 2009) described ‘special abilities’ commonly seen in dyslexics in areas such as non-verbal skill, art and architecture, and described a “pathology of superiority”, suggesting dyslexia to be part of natural variation among humans which leaves some with talents in one area and others with talents in another. West (2009) describes this trade off as a basic quality of design, that when something is optimised for a particular purpose, it often won’t suit another purpose. West suggests that examples of dyslexics who have excelled in visual-spatial fields show not only that they have succeeded despite their disability, but perhaps that they have excelled because of dyslexia. Overall there is much theorising that there could be some advantage to being dyslexic, but what evidence underpins this? Eide and Eide (2011) describe dyslexia not as a learning disorder but as a different processing style, or different pattern of brain organisation. They suggest that this different organisation predisposes dyslexics to being weak in some areas and strong in others. Eide and Eide report research showing key differences between dyslexic and non-dyslexic brains which could explain these differences. Different areas of the brain process different types of information, and brain regions are organised to perform different functions. For more complex processing, different functional areas must communicate with each other, through connections. Research shows that dyslexic brains are organised differently to those of non-dyslexics, with dyslexic brains showing longer connections between functional areas. This means that in dyslexic brains, more widely spaced brain areas work together (long range connections), whereas in non-dyslexics, more closely related brain areas work together (local connections). This difference affects brain processing, as local connections are especially good at processing fine details, whereas long range 7 © RNIB 2013 connections are better at ‘big picture’ processing. This difference matches up with the strengths and weaknesses associated with dyslexia. Eide and Eide suggest that these strengths and weaknesses are often the flip-side of each other. For example, whilst dyslexics may be poorer at tasks involving precision and accuracy, they are better at tasks involving seeing the ‘big picture’ or identifying new connections. They may be poor at applying rules or procedures but better at finding best fits and innovative problem solving. Dyslexia and visual learning Research by Schneps, Rose and Fischer (2007) further investigated the implications of brain differences between dyslexics and non-dyslexics. People differ in their ability to attend to information in the centre versus the periphery of their visual field, and these differences affect their abilities at various visual learning tasks. For example, the central visual field is optimised for fine detail in a concentrated area, therefore is effective for use in visual search tasks. The peripheral visual field on the other hand covers a much wider area with lower resolution, making it effective for rapid processing and comparisons. Research suggests that many dyslexics tend to be biased towards information in the periphery of their visual field. Schneps et al (2007) describe how attention to one visual field reduces attention to the other, which suggests that individuals with a bias for one visual field would be very good at tasks associated with that field, but poor at tasks associated with the other. Indeed, Schneps et al found evidence of this in dyslexics, with dyslexics being poor at visual search tasks (associated with the central field) but good at impossible figures and implicit spatial learning (associated with the peripheral field). Further work in this area by Coppin (2009) described central vision as a microscopic lens, and peripheral vision as a wide angle panoramic lens. Coppin refers to dyslexics as ‘wide-angle perceivers’ and suggests that this explains some of their difficulties with reading. Reading text is a task for which central vision is useful – in terms of clear focus on a small area. Coppin suggests that for this reason dyslexics – as wide angle perceivers – may be better suited to dealing with images to present and understand ideas. Coppin goes further to suggest that wide angle perception is what causes the link between dyslexia and creativity, making dyslexics more able to think outside the box and see relationships. 8 © RNIB 2013 These findings could have implications for the use of images by dyslexic pupils. Firstly, Coppin suggests that images should be used by dyslexics to help them to play to their strengths in terms of perception. In practice, this means it is important that information displayed visually to dyslexic learners is presented in a way that they can access. Anecdotal evidence (Atkins and Walker, 2012) suggests that some dyslexic pupils struggle with complex images, and could benefit from enlarged, simplified images, or those in which an idea or concept is explained over a series of images. Difficulty with complex images could be explained by difficulties processing information from central vision, and it is possible that simplified accessible images could help in this situation. Conclusion This project aimed to investigate use of images by dyslexic people. The available literature suggests there may be a relationship between dyslexia, creativity and visual spatial talents. Some researchers believe there may be advantages to being dyslexic, with dyslexia being characterised by skills as well as deficits. This may be due to differences in brain structure between dyslexics and non-dyslexics, making dyslexia just another way of processing information. Evidence suggests that dyslexic people may be better at tasks involving their peripheral vision, and perceiving the ‘big picture’ rather than focusing on fine detail such as text. Furthermore, some dyslexics are skilled at certain types of image processing. Some researchers believe this means dyslexics may be better using images to present and understand ideas rather than text based information. Anecdotal evidence suggests that some dyslexics struggle with complex images, which could be explained by the finding that dyslexics struggle to process fine detail. This could suggest a need for simplified or enlarged images for use with this group. This is a complex area of research, and it is difficult to extrapolate scientific evidence of differences between dyslexics and non-dyslexics from anecdote and theory. No published research evidence has been found relating to the use of images in the education of dyslexic learners, therefore this may be an area to investigate through further research. This could follow the case study approach reported by Atkins and Walker (2012), through trialling accessible images with dyslexic students and gathering teacher/expert views on whether large print or tactile images could be of any benefit to this group. 9 © RNIB 2013 References Atkins, S., and Walker, C. (2012). Load2Learn phase one evaluation report. RNIB Centre for Accessible Information, Birmingham: Technical report #15. Brunswick, N., Martin, G.N., and Marzano, L. (2010). Visuospatial superiority in developmental dyslexia: Myth or reality? Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 421 – 426. Coppin, P. (2009). Using dyslexia to explore the cognitive characteristics of illustrations and text; using illustrations and text to explore the cognitive characteristics of dyslexia. In Proceedings of iConference 2009, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC. Eide, B. L., and Eide, F.F. (2011). The dyslexic advantage: unlocking the hidden potential of the dyslexic brain. New York: Hay House. Geschwind, N. (1982) Why Orton was right, Annals of Dyslexia, 32, Orton Dyslexia Society reprint no. 98. Cited in West, T. G. (2009). In the Mind’s eye: Creative visual thinkers, gifted dyslexics and the rise of visual technologies. New York: Prometheus. Klein, C. (2003). Diagnosing dyslexia (2nd edition). London: The Basic Skills Agency. Murphy, F. (2011). On being dyslexic: student radiographers’ perspectives. Radiography, 17 (2), 132 – 138. Orton, S.T. (1925) Word blindness in school children. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 14, 581 – 613. Cited in von Karolyi, C., and Winner, E. (2004). Dyslexia and visual spatial talents: are they connected? In T. Newman and R. Stemberg (Ed.s) Students with both gifts and learning disabilities. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Reading Rights Coalition (2012). The definition of 'print disabled'?. [online] Available from: http://www.readingrights.org/definitionprint-disabled accessed 14 May 2013 15:08 GMT. 10 © RNIB 2013 Riley, H., Rankin, Q., Brunswick, N., McManus, I.C., Chamberlain, R., and Loo, P.W. (2009). Inclusive practice: researching the relationships between dyslexia, personality, and art students’ drawing ability. Proceedings of the 2009 Include Conference on Inclusive Design held at the Royal College of Art, London. Available from http://include09.kinetixevents.co.uk/4dcgi/prog accessed 14 May 2013 15:09 GMT. Rose, J. (2009). Identifying and teaching children and young people with dyslexia and literacy difficulties: an independent report. Available from http://dera.ioe.ac.uk/14790/1/00659-2009DOMEN.pdf accessed 23 May 2013 14:39 GMT Schneps, M.H., Rose, L.T., and Fischer, K.W. (2007). Visual learning and the brain: implications for dyslexia. Mind, Brain and Education, 1 (3), 128 – 139. von Karolyi, C. (2001). Dyslexic strength: rapid discrimination of impossible figures. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34 (4), 380391. von Karolyi, C., Winner, E., Gray, W., and Sherman, G.F. (2003). Dyslexia linked to talent: Global visual-spatial ability. Brain and Language, 85, 427 – 431. von Karolyi, C., and Winner, E. (2004). Dyslexia and visual spatial talents: are they connected? In T. Newman and R. Stemberg (Ed.s) Students with both gifts and learning disabilities. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. West, T.G. (2003). Secret of the super successful… they’re dyslexic. Thalamus, 21 (1), 48 – 52. West, T. G. (2009). In the Mind’s eye: Creative visual thinkers, gifted dyslexics and the rise of visual technologies. New York: Prometheus. Winner, E., von Karolyi, C., Malinsky, D., French, L., Seliger, C., Ross, E., and Weber, C. (2001). Dyslexia and visual-spatial talents: compensation vs deficit model. Brain and Language, 76, 81-110. 11