Fair Process Effect Running head: FAIR PROCESS EFFECT Humans Making Sense of Alarming Conditions: Psychological Insight into the Fair Process Effect Kees van den Bos Utrecht University Chapter published in 2015 in R. S. Cropanzano & M. L. Ambrose (Eds.), Oxford handbook of justice in work organizations. New York: Oxford University Press. Keywords: fair process effect; treatment fairness; human reactions; sense making; alarm system; informational value; organizational behavior; human resource management Author Notes: I thank Maureen Ambrose, Russell Cropanzano, and Liesbeth Hulst for their comments and suggestions on an earlier draft of this chapter. Address correspondence to Kees van den Bos, Department of Social Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands. E-mail: k.vandenbos@uu.nl. 1 Fair Process Effect 2 Abstract This chapter examines the psychology of the fair process effect, which is defined as the positive effect that people's perceptions of experienced treatment fairness have on their subsequent reactions. The chapter argues that when people are confronted with potentially problematic events or personal uncertainty-provoking experiences, this signals to them that something alarming may be going on that warrants their attention. As a result, the individuals involved are likely to engage in psychological processes of trying to make sense of what is going on and what they should expect will be happening. Because perceived procedural fairness has important informational value for people, it follows that people are susceptible to issues of treatment fairness in many alarming or sense-making triggering situations. The current chapter describes this alarm-system perspective on the psychology of the fair process effect and discusses implications of this perspective for our understanding of organizational behavior and human resource management. Fair Process Effect 3 Humans Making Sense of Alarming Conditions: Psychological Insight into the Fair Process Effect In this chapter, I focus on the psychology of the fair process effect, the positive effect that people's perceptions of procedural fairness (also referred to in this chapter as experienced treatment fairness1) have on their subsequent reactions (Folger, Rosenfield, Grove, & Corkran, 1979; Van den Bos, 2005; Walker, LaTour, Lind, & Thibaut, 1974). A central outcome of the research that I will review in the chapter is that an important reason why this effect is so robust and so prevalent in many different situations is because the experience of fair treatment serves important functions in processes of sense-making. That is, when people are confronted with events such as economic problems, reorganization processes, potential layoffs (Brockner, 2010), but also more general personal uncertainty-provoking experiences (Van den Bos, 2001a), this signals to them that something potentially alarming may be going on that warrants their attention (Van den Bos, Ham, Lind, Simonis, Van Essen, & Rijpkema, 2008). As a result, the individuals involved are likely to engage in sense-making and social appraisal processes in order to make sense of what is going on and what they should expect will be happening (Van den Bos & Lind, 2013; Weick, 1995). Because perceived procedural fairness has important informational value for people (Hulst, Van den Bos, & Akkermans, 2013; Lind, 2001; Van den Bos, 2001b), it follows that people are susceptible to issues of treatment fairness in many alarming or sense-making triggering situations (Van den Bos & Lind, 2009), especially when they are interacting with supervisors, management, or other social authorities (Lind, 1995; Tyler & Lind, 1992; see also Weick, 1995). Therefore, information that conveys fair treatment by authorities or other important people will yield various types of positive reactions among the individuals involved, such as increased organizational commitment, job performance, conflict resolution, decision acceptance, and job satisfaction (Van den Bos, 2005). In contrast, information that Fair Process Effect 4 indicates unfair treatment instigates all sorts of negative responses, such as increased retaliation, theft, work stress, overt or covert disobedience, and decreased mental and physical health (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998). Thus, a central premise of the current chapter is that perceived procedural or treatment fairness fulfills important psychological functions for people trying to make sense of their worlds, especially when they are trying to manage potentially alarming situations in those worlds. Figure 1 illustrates this alarm-system perspective on the psychology of the fair process effect. In what follows in this chapter I will define the fair process effect and will give a brief overview of instances where the effect has been found. I then will go on and discuss basic psychological insights into the fair process effect. The chapter will end by drawing some conclusions of these insights for our understanding of organizational behavior and human resource management. The Fair Process Effect: Instances of the Effect and Conceptualizing the Effect In the organizational justice literature there continues to be some misunderstanding and ambiguity as to how the term "the fair process effect" should be understood (Van den Bos, 2005). It is important, therefore, to delineate how I think the term should be defined and what I mean with the term when discussing instances of the effect, basic psychological mechanisms underlying the effect, and implications of the effect for our understanding of organizational behavior and more general social behavior. This discussion will necessarily rely and build on what I said earlier about these issues in my earlier review of the fair process effect (see Van den Bos, 2005). A review of the fair process effect should start with the 1974 experiment by Walker, LaTour, Lind, and Thibaut. This experiment was the first procedural justice study that revealed a fair process effect. The experiment investigated the effects of adversary and Fair Process Effect 5 inquisitorial procedures on the reactions of disputants toward either a favorable or an unfavorable verdict in a simulated court trial. Adversary and inquisitorial procedures differ on multiple dimensions, but the key distinction that Thibaut and Walker were interested in is that adversary procedures allow people involved in court trials greater levels of control over the process used in the court trial than inquisitorial procedures do. Findings of the Walker et al. study indicated that defendants judged the way in which they had been treated to be more fair when they had experienced the adversary procedure than when they had been subjected to the inquisitorial procedure. These effects are interpreted to reflect that, compared to inquisitorial procedures, adversary procedures allow people greater levels of process control (Thibaut & Walker, 1975, 1978) and more opportunities to voice their opinions (Folger et al., 1979). More important for the current purposes is that the findings also revealed a fair process effect such that participants judged their verdict to be more fair and were more satisfied with the verdict in case of the fair (adversary) procedure as opposed to the unfair (inquisitorial) procedure. Building on the Walker et al. (1974) findings, Folger and colleagues (1979) were the first to come up with the term "the fair process effect." Extending on Hirschman's (1970) conception of voice, Folger et al. re-interpreted the procedural justice findings presented by Thibaut and Walker cum suis, and suggested that getting the opportunity to present evidence supporting one's own case in a court trial has a strong positive effect on the defendant's reactions toward the verdict. Folger et al. (1979) tested their line of reasoning by manipulating whether participants in an experimental set-up either received or did not receive an opportunity to voice their opinions about how the experimenter should divide lottery tickets between participants themselves and the other participants. As predicted, findings revealed a fair process effect such that voice procedures resulted in higher ratings of overall satisfaction than no-voice procedures. Fair Process Effect 6 Following the pioneering studies by Walker et al. (1974) and Folger et al. (1979) as well as the groundbreaking review of the psychological literature on procedural justice by Lind and Tyler (1988), many positive reactions have been reported of employees who felt they had been treated in a fair way by their management or their organization. These positive reactions include higher commitment to organizations and institutions (Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Korsgaard, Schweiger, & Sapienza, 1995; McFarlin & Sweeney, 1992; Moorman, 1991), more extra-role citizenship behavior (Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Podsakoff & MacKenzie, 1993), greater likelihood of conflict prevention and resolution (Bobocel, Agar, Meyer, & Irving, 1998), better job performance (Lind, Kanfer, & Earley, 1990), more widespread acceptance of company policy (Greenberg, 1994; Lind, 1990) and supervisor directives (Huo, Smith, Tyler, & Lind, 1996), higher levels of job satisfaction (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998), and more positive emotional feelings (Weiss, Suckow, & Cropanzano, 1999). In contrast, when employees perceive that they have been treated in an unfair way they are more likely to leave their jobs (Alexander & Ruderman, 1987), are less likely to cooperate (Lind, 2001), show lower levels of morale and higher levels of work stress and overt and covert disobedience (Huo et al., 1996), are more likely to initiate lawsuits (Lind, Greenberg, Scott, & Welchans, 2000), and may even start behaving in anti-social ways (Greenberg, 1993, 1997; Greenberg & Lind, 2000). In other social contexts than organizations the fair process effect as defined here yields reliable and important effects on human reactions as well. For example, the belief that one has been treated fairly by judges, the police, or other social authorities enhances acceptance of legal decisions (Lind, Kulik, Ambrose, & De Vera Park, 1993), obedience to laws (Tyler, 1990), more positive evaluations of public policies (Lind, 1990; Tyler, Rasinski, & McGraw, 1985), and more trust in government (Van den Bos & Van der Velden, in press), whereas the belief that one has been treated unfairly has been shown to prompt protest Fair Process Effect 7 behavior (Vermunt, Wit, Van den Bos, & Lind, 1996) and recidivism among spouse abuse defendants (Paternoster, Brame, Bachman, & Sherman, 1997). Indeed, what makes the fair process effect so important is that the effect has been shown on a wide variety of human responses (for overviews, see, e.g., Lind & Tyler, 1988; Van den Bos, 2005). I want to emphasize explicitly that the definition of the fair process effect that is used in this chapter is that the effect refers to the positive effect that people's perceptions of treatment fairness have on their subsequent reactions. Thus, treatment fairness is a term I use here to signify the fairness of how people are treated. This term refers not only (or primarily) to the perceived fairness of formal procedures, but also (or especially) to the fairness people experience in interactions with other persons. I am proposing, therefore, that the perceived fairness of interpersonal treatment is what is driving large parts of what has become known in the literature as the fair process effect. Following my take on the fair treatment effect it is important to specify what I think researchers often mean when they refer to the concept of procedure. The terms "procedure" and "procedural justice" are derived from the law literature and especially from the 1975 work by social psychologist John Thibaut and law professor Lawrence Walker. These authors and their colleagues were inspired by the psychological differences they saw between different legal procedures, and in their pioneering procedural justice experiments they took these differences as starting point for their investigation of participants' reactions toward procedures that varied the amount of process control that participants experienced in simulated court trials. Thus, Thibaut and Walker (1975) combined their mutual interests in social psychology and law and as a result they placed their studies under the heading of "procedural justice" research. However, this should not be taken too literally, since these authors clearly saw their experiments as a first step toward understanding the psychology involved in fairness and justice issues (Thibaut & Walker, 1978) and were intrigued by the Fair Process Effect 8 implication of their findings that how people are treated in courts of law can have strong impact on their reactions to judges' verdicts (Walker et al., 1974). Following the pioneering research by Thibaut and Walker (1975, 1978), scientists have deepened our understanding of the psychological processes hinted upon in this earlier work (e.g., Folger et al., 1979; Folger, 1986; Greenberg, 2000; Greenberg & Folger, 1983; Lind & Tyler, 1988; Tyler & Lind, 1992; Van den Bos & Lind, 2002) and rightfully noted that the psychological processes involved in the Thibaut and Walker simulations could be adequately expanded to incorporate how people react to fairness and justice in other contexts than legal settings. Most notably, the role of procedural justice in the workplace was recognized to be very important (Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001; Greenberg, 2000; Greenberg & Folger, 1983; Moorman, 1991). Even more important for the current purposes, during the 1975-1985 advancement of research and theory on procedural justice it became clear that what by then had become known as procedural justice effects were really effects of how fairly people felt they had been treated in the particular context under investigation (for an overview, see, e.g., Lind & Tyler, 1988). In correspondence with this latter observation, the fair process effect research that is reviewed in this chapter is about the effect of the fairness of the way in which people feel they have been treated in the workplace or elsewhere. Thus, "procedural fairness" and "procedural justice" as they are being used here, and as I think that John Thibaut really intended it to be, refer to the way people are treated. So, in essence, fair or unfair treatment in interpersonal and social interactions is the issue here. This is also the reason why in this chapter I equate the fair process effect with an effect of fair treatment. Indeed, it may be more accurate to refer to the "fair process effect" as the fair treatment effect. It is important to note that this conception of procedural justice overlaps to some extent with a notion that was developed later in the organizational justice literature, the Fair Process Effect 9 concept of "interactional justice" (see, e.g., Bies & Moag, 1986; Bies & Shapiro, 1987). One could argue that a danger of using the procedural justice label is that it may be a bit of a misnomer and that people may wrongfully misinterpret the concept to mean to refer to formal, law-like procedures. The interactional justice label has as an advantage that it clearly refers to the justice and fairness aspects of social interactions that are so important in understanding the majority of the fairness effects reported in the psychological literature. The main disadvantage of "international justice," however, is that when, in addition to the two earlier developed notions of distributive and procedural justice, researchers start using this concept they have to redefine the concept of procedural justice. That is, because of the obvious overlap of interactional justice with informal procedural justice (Tyler & Bies, 1990), introducing the concept of interactional justice forces researchers to start redefining procedural justice in terms of formal decision-making procedures. In the modern organizational justice literature there is a strong tendency to do this. However, this formal aspect was never meant to be important in the work by the founders of procedural justice. On the contrary, they were really referring to the more informal way in which people were treated in decision-making processes. It is this latter conception, the fairness of informal treatment, that I think the literature should focus on (for a similar argument, see Tyler & Bies, 1990), and I will refer to this by means of the notions that were originally developed for these effects: procedural justice (Thibaut & Walker, 1975) and the fair process effect (Folger et al., 1979). Thus, the term "fair process" in organizational justice should be used to refer primarily to the informal way in which people are treated in decision-making processes, and not (or not only) to formal procedures. Formal aspects of procedures may have a role in the fair process effect, but the fairness of the informal ways in which people are treated by other persons is what is really driving the large part of the effect. Therefore, treating the concepts Fair Process Effect 10 of "procedural justice" and "the fair process effect" in formal ways, as opposed to informal ways, would be a major error. I should emphasize that I am not stating that perceived procedural justice or treatment fairness should be equated with perceptions of distributive justice. Research has shown clearly that procedural justice or treatment fairness have quite different effects than distributive justice has (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Furthermore, it is often or typically the combined effect of procedural and distributive justice that yield precise and important insight into organizational behavior (Brockner, 2010; Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996). I also would like to note that thus far there is not a good scale that always will reliably measure the fair process effect or will assess people's procedural or treatment fairness perceptions in valid ways. This noted, recent evidence suggests that the Moorman (1991) scale might be a very good scale to measure experienced procedural justice in organizations (Miller, Konopaske, & Byrne, 2012), especially when items are tailored to fit the organization where the study is conducted and also fit the employees' experiences accurately (Lind & Tyler, 1988). It would be important for the progress in organizational justice if future studies would come up with a reliable scale of the fair process effect as defined here. Interesting in this respect is that Miller et al. (2012) recently examined the criterionrelated validity of two commonly used measures of organizational justice Colquitt, 2001; Moorman, 1991). To this end, the authors conducted a dominance analysis on the threedimension measure of organizational justice by Moorman (1991) versus the four-dimension measure of Colquitt (2001) in the prediction of Colquitt’s own outcomes. Results suggested that Moorman’s measures may dominate Colquitt’s measures on some outcomes. These findings need further replication in different samples and different work settings, but when replicated suggest that renewed consideration should be given to Moorman’s scales, as it appears that this three-factor measure of organizational justice may outperform the four- Fair Process Effect 11 factor measure in at least some instances. More generally, Miller et al. note that Moorman’s parsimonious representation of justice may be more useful than Colquitt’s version for explaining the nuances of perceptual differences regarding fairness and justice in the workplace. Clearly, attention is warranted to sort out the implications of this very interesting research as it may help to come up with a robust scale that reliably measures procedural and other justice perceptions in many different work settings. It is also important to note here that a precondition before fair process effects can occur is that the experience of a particular procedure leads to the perception of a certain level of procedural fairness. It is this procedural fairness perception that influences the person's subsequent reactions and in this way creates a fair process effect. Thus, for example, when individuals get an opportunity to voice their opinions about the way in which tasks will be distributed among employees in their department, the individuals involved may perceive the way in which they have been treated as fair. This perception of procedural fairness may then lead the department members to be more satisfied with the final decision of exactly how task assignments are distributed among them. The label "the voice effect" should be reserved, therefore, for the enhancement of procedural fairness when a procedure allows people a chance to express themselves and "the fair process effect" is the enhancement of people's evaluations, attitudes, behaviors, and other reactions following this procedural fairness perception. Thus, in contrast with what sometimes is done in the literature, a voice effect should not be equated with a fair process effect (Van den Bos, 2005). Rather the experience of voice, or encountering an accurate, a consistent, or any other particular procedure (Leventhal, 1980; Van den Bos, Vermunt, & Wilke, 1996, 1997) can affect people's fairness perceptions and these perceptions are a prerequisite before fair process effects can be found. Making a clear distinction between voice and fair process effects is also important because Fair Process Effect 12 different psychological processes may be driving voice versus fair process effects (Van den Bos, 2001a; Van den Bos, Maas, Waldring, & Semin, 2003; Van den Bos & Miedema, 2000). I think it is important to highlight the importance of the role of perceptions in the fair process effect. Fairness and justice as psychologists study these concepts are really in the eye of the beholder (e.g., Adams, 1965; Lind et al., 1990; Mikula & Wenzel, 2000; Tyler, Boeckmann, Smith, & Huo, 1997; Van den Bos & Lind, 2002). Although it might be possible to derive rational-normative principles of procedural justice and although it might be possible to come up with an objective fair process effect (see, e.g., Hare, 1981; Rawls, 1971; see also Jasso, 1994, 1999), I argue that the fair process effect in essence is a psychological effect, constructed in the head of the recipient of the procedure. Thus, the psychology of the fair process effect examines how fairness and justice judgments are constructed by people. This implies that objective conditions that researchers, organizational behaviorists, and employers think are fair or unfair do not have to viewed that way by employees and other recipients of the procedure under consideration. I also would like to emphasize the importance of the concept of fairness in our understanding of "the fair process effect" and the larger literature on "organizational justice." That is, compared to the related notions of justice and morality, fairness better connotes the subjective, ready judgment that is and has long been the true topic of psychological study (Van den Bos & Lind, 2002). Related to this, participants and respondents in research studies typically find it easier and more relevant to provide judgments of fairness than judgments of justice or morality. This is the reason that most psychologists in our field usually ask people to rate fairness rather than to rate justice or morality (cf. Tyler & Lind, 1992). This is also a main cause of why morality researchers often use "good" or "bad" judgments or "thumbs up" or "thumbs down" judgments as their dependent variables (e.g., Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom, Darley, & Cohen, 2001). These variables may be conceptually related to judgments Fair Process Effect 13 of what is moral and immoral but obviously are very different from what Kant (1785) and other rationalist moral philosophers (Beauchamp, 2001) are focusing on. Returning to the fair process effect, these considerations constitute the rationale why the central topic of consideration of this chapter is called "the fair process effect" and not "the just process effect" or "the moral process effect." The former simply reflects better both common research practices and the core belief under study. An implication of this observation is that although organizational psychologists usually call this area "organizational justice," we are in effect referring to fairness perceptions as the major antecedent of the effects we are interested in (Lind & Van den Bos, 2002; Van den Bos & Lind, 2009). Related to this is the concern whether what usually is called the fair process effect should be really conceived of as a fair process effect or that it would it be more accurate to start talking about an unfair process effect instead (cf. Greenberg & Folger, 1983). Whereas the former perspective is in line with how the effect generally is known, the latter view corresponds with observations that unfair events affect lay people's cognitions and reactions stronger than fair events (Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996; Folger, 1984; Van den Bos & Spruijt, 2002; Van den Bos & Van Prooijen, 2001; Van den Bos et al., 1997). This finding is probably caused by the important role that people's expectations have in the psychology of fairness judgments (e.g., Van den Bos et al., 1996) and the fact that unfair events may strongly violate these expectations (Van den Bos & Van Prooijen, 2001). The psychological mechanism behind this finding may well be an instance of the more general negativity effect that indicates that negative things typically have a bigger impact on psychological processes than positive things (see, e.g., Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Peeters & Czapinski, 1990). Thus, unfairness in fact may play a more prominent role and it may be better to focus on the psychology of unfairness as opposed to fairness (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998). In the current chapter, I will use the traditional term "the fair process effect," with the understanding that, Fair Process Effect 14 unless explicitly stated otherwise, this may involve the effects of both fair and unfair procedures on people's reactions. Psychological Insight into the Fair Process Effect: Alarming Conditions and Sense-Making Processes Many studies have reported fair process effects in many different contexts using different research methods. The effect has been observed most often within the organizational context (Lind & Van den Bos, 2002), but has frequently been found elsewhere as well (e.g., Paternoster et al., 1997; Vermunt et al., 1996). Furthermore, an exciting aspect of research on the effects of procedural fairness perceptions is that effects of these perceptions have been found on very different human reactions (Lind & Tyler, 1988). This is important because it suggests that fair process studies may have substantial implications for a multitude of domains of human and organizational behavior (Greenberg, 2000). Because fair procedures enhance so many important cognitions, feelings, attitudes, and behaviors, insight into what is responsible for the fair process effect is crucial for understanding how people think, feel, and behave in their social and organizational environments (Cropanzano & Folger, 1989, 1991; Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997; Greenberg, 2000; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Greenberg, 1990, 1993; Lind & Tyler, 1988; Tyler & Lind, 1992). To this end, this chapter will examine the psychology of the fair process effect. Different psychological explanations of the fair process effect exist in the organizational justice literature. These include instrumental explanations of the effect (see, e.g., Shapiro & Brett, 2005; Thibaut & Walker, 1975, 1978; Walker, Lind, & Thibaut, 1979), which emphasize the amount of control procedures give people over the outcomes they hope to get. Other explanations focus on processes of social influence (e.g., Greenberg & Folger, 1983) and note that the fair process effect can be explained in terms of people being Fair Process Effect 15 susceptible to influence via social comparison of others' opinions about whether certain outcome distributions are fair or unfair (Folger et al., 1979). Yet another important account of the fair process effect points at the role of referent cognitions in explaining and understanding the effect (Cropanzano & Folger, 1989; Folger, 1986, 1987, 1993; Folger & Baron, 1996; Folger & Skarlicki, 1998). Referent cognitions theory tends to focus on unfair procedures and argues that experiences of procedural unfairness are psychologically so meaningful to people because when a procedural rule is broken people's thinking people use a frame of reference for evaluating what happened that consists of a mental comparison to what might have happened instead. In contrast, relational models focus on experiences of fair procedures. Relational models argue that fair treatment by authorities signals that important persons in your society or organization value and respect you and therefore treat you with dignity and respect (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Tyler & Lind, 1992). Instrumental, social influence, referent cognitions, and relational models have been reviewed and discussed earlier (see, e.g., Folger & Cropanzano, 1998; Tyler & Lind, 1992; Van den Bos, 2005). Here I concentrate on a more recent psychological explanation of the effect that perhaps has the potential to integrate many aspects of the earlier proposed explanations. That is, the alarm-system perspective on the fair process effect argues that people show positive reactions to fair treatment because being treated fairly gives them a way to cope with alarming circumstances. The alarm-system account explains people's negative responses to unfair treatment by pointing out that being treated unfairly constitutes an alarming experience in itself (Van den Bos et al., 2008; Van den Bos & Lind, 2009). The alarm-system explanation focuses on how people make sense of alarming experiences, such as economic problems, reorganization processes, and potential lay-offs, but also flashing lights, exclamation points, and personal uncertainty-provoking experiences (Van den Bos et al., 2008; Van den Bos & Lind, 2009). The alarm-system perspective on the Fair Process Effect 16 fair process effect is inspired by an earlier body of research that shows that people react more vigilantly to issues of treatment fairness when they are coping with personal uncertainty (Van den Bos & Lind, 2002, 2009). One of the first studies on uncertainty and the fair process effect was a series of experiments published in 2001. In these experiments, half of the participants were asked to think about, and write a description of, the emotional experiences they feel when they encounter uncertainty in their personal lives (Van den Bos, 2001a). The other half of the participants thought about and wrote about the emotional experiences involved in their watching television, an issue which does not salientize personal uncertainty among the population of university students where we conducted our studies. In a later, ostensibly unrelated study, the same participants were exposed to standard procedural fairness manipulations: In some of the experiments this involved participants, after completing a task in which they worked with another person, either being or not being allowed to voice their views about how much they should be paid relative to the other participant. In another experiment, participants were informed that in a selection process in which they took part either all (accurate procedure) or only some (inaccurate procedure) of their test items would be taken into account. These voice and accuracy manipulations are common procedural fairness manipulations, reliably leading those who do not receive voice or who experience inaccurate procedures to feel they have been unfairly treated and those who receive voice or are accurately treated to feel they have been fairly treated. The principal dependent variable in the Van den Bos (2001a) experiments was the participants' ratings of positive or negative affect toward the way they were treated. The results support the uncertainty salience hypothesis proposed in the article: When personal uncertainty had been made salient, the fair procedures resulted in more positive and less negative affect whereas the unfair procedures yielded more negative and less positive affect than was the case when uncertainty had not been made salient. In other words, when personal Fair Process Effect 17 uncertainty was salient, the fair process effect (Van den Bos, 2005) on participants' affective treatment ratings was magnified. Other fairness studies have found similar effects not just for the salience of personal uncertainty, but also for real experienced personal uncertainty (Maas & Van den Bos, 2011) and for stable individual differences in uncertainty orientation in organizational contexts or other important real-world situations (see, e.g., Elovainio et al. 2005; Kausto, Elo, Lipponen, & Elovainio, 2005; Patterson, Cowley, & Prasongsukarn, 2006; See, 2009; Thau, Aquino, & Wittek, 2007). Devon Proudfoot and Allan Lind talk more about these and other uncertainty findings in their chapter in this volume. Here I would like to note that the implication of what I have discussed thus far seems to be that there is something about personal uncertainty that makes people to react more strongly and more vigorously toward fair and unfair events. An important way to understand these research results is provided by the idea that personal uncertainty often leads people to react more strongly toward fair and unfair events because being uncertain or being reminded about things one is uncertain about often instigates strong affective-experiential processes (Maas & Van den Bos, 2009). Thus, the idea is that experiencing feelings of uncertainty would lead people to start processing information they subsequently receive in experiential-intuitive ways (Epstein & Pacini, 1999). Cognitive-experiential self-theory distinguishes between two conceptual systems that people use to process information, namely the experiential-intuitive and the rational-cognitive systems (Epstein & Pacini, 1999). The experiential way of processing information is intuitive, preconsciously encoding information into concrete images or metaphors, and making associative connections. In experiential modes, events are experienced passively, and people can be seized by their emotions. The rationalistic way of processing information, on the other hand, is analytic, encoding information in abstract ways, based on making logical cause-and-effect connections, and requiring intentional, effortful processing. In rationalistic Fair Process Effect 18 modes of information processing, people experience events actively and consciously while thinking things over and making justifications for what happened in these events, and in these modes people are in control of their thoughts. Cognitive-experiential self-theory also assumes that the operation of experiential mindsets is intimately associated with affect-related experiences (see, e.g., Epstein & Pacini, 1999). If experiential mindsets do indeed make people's fairness reactions more susceptible to affect-related processes, then the intensity with which people react affectively to daily life events (Larsen, Diener, & Cropanzano, 1987) should interact with people's mindsets. Earlier fairness studies had shown that individual differences in affect intensity can moderate people's fairness reactions (Van den Bos et al., 2003). Integrating this line of work with cognitive-experiential self-theory, Maas and Van den Bos (2009) predicted that under conditions of uncertainty, individual differences in affect intensity (Larsen, Diener, & Emmons, 1986) would moderate people's fairness reactions, especially when they have been primed with experiential (as opposed to rationalistic) modes of information processing. To test these predictions, Maas and Van den Bos (2009) brought people in either experiential or rationalistic mindsets. To this end, we developed a manipulation that varied the mindsets (experiential or rationalistic) people are in, such that people react more based on their gut feelings when in experiential mindsets and more deliberately when in rationalistic mindsets. After thus having been brought in either experiential or rationalistic mindsets, participants responded to procedures that allowed them opportunities to voice their opinions or that denied them such opportunities (Maas & Van den Bos, 2009, Study 2). As predicted, the findings reported showed that people who had been primed with experiential mindsets and who score high on the affect intensity scale (Larsen et al., 1986) reacted more strongly to fair and unfair events (compared to those who had been brought in rationalistic mindsets and/or who scored low on affect intensity). These findings suggest that focusing on the Fair Process Effect 19 combination of affective and experiential processes is important for the social psychology of uncertainty effects reported in the literature. Affective and experiential reactions may be more likely in alarming conditions in which people are trying to assess frantically what to do and how to behave. It is to a discussion of this alarm-system based work that I now turn. The alarm-system perspective on the justice judgment process argues that the affective-experiential effects that are found under conditions of uncertainty (Maas & Van den Bos, 2009) occur because experiencing feelings of personal uncertainty constitutes an alarming experience (Van den Bos, Ham, et al., 2008). Specifically, in this line of research we examined a possible connection between, on the one hand, the augmentation of fairness effects in the presence of personal uncertainty (e.g., Van den Bos, 2001a; Van den Bos, Poortvliet, Maas, Miedema, & Van den Ham, 2005) and other self-threatening conditions (Miedema, Van den Bos, & Vermunt, 2006) and, on the other hand, related phenomena in social cognition and social neuroscience showing the possible existence of a "human alarm system" (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003). In the literatures on close relationships and on social neuroscience, it has been suggested that personal uncertainty and self-threats lead to the activation of the "human alarm system," a psychological system that people use to detect and handle alarming situations and that prompts people to process more alertly what is going on in the situations in which they find themselves. For example, Murray, Holmes, and Collins (2005) suggested that personal uncertainty and felt insecurity in close relationships activate the human alarm system with the result that people process more alertly what is happening in their relationships. Along similar lines, Eisenberger et al. (2003) have argued that being ostracized or experiencing other self-threatening events activates parts of the human brain that Eisenberger et al. have labeled the “human alarm system.” The alarm system is responsible for detecting cues that might be harmful to survival and, after activation, for recruiting attention and Fair Process Effect 20 coping responses to minimize threat. Eisenberger et al. (2003) have argued that experiencing social exclusion or other self-threatening events may be an experience of social pain. Like physical pain, the experience of social pain may trigger the human alarm system, "alerting us when we have sustained injury to our social connections" (Eisenberger et al., 2003, p. 292). From an evolutionary perspective, the working of such an alarm system would be adaptive: An activated alarm system prompts the human organism to act and respond more quickly or otherwise more alertly to what is going on in the organism's environment; this in turn makes the survival of the organism more likely. One way to triangulate the relationships linking personal uncertainty, the human alarm system, and the fairness literature is to conceptualize an overlap between the alarm system and the fairness judgment process. An intriguing hypothesis that can be derived from such a postulated overlap is that factors that people associate with alarming conditions would be expected to enhance the sensitivity of the alarm system and thus, given the postulated overlap, potentiate sensitivity to the fairness-related events people subsequently experience. So, just as Eisenberger and Lieberman (2004) postulated that the brain bases of social pain are similar to those of physical pain and hypothesized that "factors that enhance the sensitivity to one type of pain should enhance the sensitivity of this alarm system and thus potentiate sensitivity to the other type of pain as well" (p. 297). In our 2008 article, my coauthors and I postulated that presenting alarm-related symbols to people would activate the human alarm system and hence potentiate sensitivity to other types of processes associated with it as well, including enhanced sensitivity to the fairness judgment process. This would be expected to make people react more sensitively to subsequently experienced fair or unfair events (Van den Bos et al., 2008). More specifically, the literature reviewed thus far suggests two conclusions in line with this analysis: (1) personal uncertainty and other self-threatening conditions activate the Fair Process Effect 21 human alarm system (Eisenberger et al., 2003; Murray et al., 2005); (2) personal uncertainty and self-threatening conditions lead to more extreme fair process effects (Miedema et al., 2006; Van den Bos 2001a; Van den Bos et al., 2005). Thus, we now know that the same conditions that are thought to activate the human alarm system, might also lead to more extreme fairness effects. Given this observation, we proposed that activating the human alarm system directly, by presenting alarm-related stimuli to people, would lead to more extreme reactions toward fair and unfair events. Thus, an intriguing hypothesis that follows from the alarm-system perspective is that the presentation of cues that are closely, even subtly, related to alarming conditions might lead people to form more extreme judgments about subsequently presented fair and unfair events. Findings of various experiments (scenario studies, an experiential experiment, and fMRI testing) in fact provided evidence for this line of reasoning (Van den Bos et al., 2008; Van den Bos & Rijpkema, 2008). Specifically, we found that viewing large exclamation points prior to responding to accurate or inaccurate procedures and voice or no-voice procedures indeed made people react more extremely to the procedures. Similar effects were also found on participants' reactions to good versus bad outcomes, suggesting that an alarmsystem perspective on fairness reactions applies not only to the fair process effect but to fair outcome effects as well (Van den Bos, 1999). The overlap between reactions to procedural and distributive justice has been discussed elsewhere as well (see, e.g., Ambrose & Arnaud, 2005; Ambrose & Schminke, 2009; Cropanzano & Ambrose, 2001; but see also Brockner, 2010; Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996). Another experimental study replicated and extended these findings. Participants in this study were people from various parts of the Netherlands, with different educational backgrounds, and from different age groups, who were interviewed in a shopping area of a medium-sized city located in the middle of the Netherlands. Specifically, people who were Fair Process Effect 22 walking in the shopping area of the city center of Amersfoort were asked whether they would like to participate in a study on how people process information and which would take a maximum of 10 minutes of their time. Two meters behind the experimenter, an orange flashing light of 17 x 14 x 14 centimeters of the sort used on emergency vehicles had been placed on a small pedestal of 1 meter high. Both the flashing light and pedestal were ostensibly unrelated to our study and the flashing light was connected by means of an electric cable of 1.5 meters long to a large store (Vroom & Dreesmann; a Dutch equivalent of Macy's). As our alarm-related manipulation, the flashing light had been switched on for half of the participants and the flashing light was off for the other half of the participants. In this setting, participants responded to scenarios in which they were asked to image that fair or unfair events happened at their workplace. In accord with the alarm-system view of the justice judgment process, the findings revealed that when the flashing light had been switched on people showed more extreme justice judgments in response to variations in good and bad outcomes than when the flashing light had been switched off. A finding not reported in the article but that is interesting to note here was that participants also showed more extreme reactions in response to accurate versus inaccurate procedures when the flashing light was on as opposed to off.2 Thus, we found similar effects of our alarm manipulation on both reactions to variations in distributive fairness and procedural fairness, again suggesting that the alarm-system perspective is not limited to the fair process effect only but to fair outcome effects as well. More generally, perhaps this is an indication that as our field evolves our thinking about the fair process effect, the fair treatment effect, and other fairness judgment processes needs to evolve as well. Perhaps this suggests that "fair process" was a specific example of a more general phenomenon. The studies reported in the Van den Bos et al. (2008) paper were in part inspired by the conjecture that the uncertainty management findings reported in the fairness literature Fair Process Effect 23 (see, e.g., Van den Bos, 2001a; Van den Bos & Lind, 2002; Van den Bos et al., 2005) might be explained by the notion that experiences of personal uncertainty often constitute alarming events to people and that it is this alarm-related component of uncertainty manipulations that is driving some or perhaps all of the uncertainty effects reported in the social psychological literature. Especially interesting in this respect are some findings from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) testing (Van den Bos & Rijpkema, 2008). This study showed that watching an exclamation point leads to a brain activation pattern that shares areas (medial frontal gyrus, Brodmann area 9) with the brain regions that have been found to be active in personal moral judgment tasks. These brain areas are known to be sensitive to tapping the combined effects of cognitive and emotional responses (Greene et al., 2001). This might indicate that a combination of cognition and emotion best explains how people will form and respond to fairness and justice perceptions (Van den Bos, Ham, et al., 2008) and make personal moral decisions (cf. Greene et al., 2001). In other words, the social psychology of personal uncertainty and fairness judgments may well involve hot cognitive, not cold cognitive, processes (Van den Bos, 2007). Figure 1 illustrates the current thinking on the alarm-system perspective on the fair process effect. Related to other ideas of sense-making in organizations (e.g., Weick, 1995), the model assumes that issues of treatment fairness serve important functions in processes of sense-making. Especially when people encounter alarming events such as economic problems, reorganization processes, potential lay-offs, personal uncertainty-provoking experiences, and other stimuli, this signals to them that something potentially alarming may be going on that warrants their attention (Van den Bos et al., 2008). As a result, the individuals involved are likely to engage in sense-making and social appraisal processes in order to make sense of what is going on and what they should expect will be happening. Fair Process Effect 24 Therefore, the model assumes that people will start to look for information about what is going on and how they should behave and what will be happening in their near future. Information about fair and unfair treatment may have special importance in these kinds of situations. In particular, information that conveys fair treatment will calm people down (Vermunt & Steensma, 2003) and hence deactivate the alarm system. This is assumed to be associated with strong positive reactions among the individuals encountering the fair treatment. In contrast, information about unfair treatment will further activate the alarm system (Van den Bos et al., 2005), resulting in strong negative responses among the individuals involved. Conclusions: Toward an Alternate View on Organizational Behavior This chapter studied the fair process effect, defined explicitly as an effect of perceptions of treatment fairness on human reactions. The research reviewed in this chapter shows that experienced fair treatment has various positive effects on people's reactions, whereas perceived unfair treatment yields all kinds of negative reactions. A central hypothesis put forward in this chapter is that the fair process effect is found in so many circumstances on so many different dependent variables among so many different research participants and respondents because many people in many different situations are trying to make sense of what is going on in their immediate environments. This insight fits a growing recognition that sense-making in organizations and in other social situations makes up an important component of what people do in the modern world (Van den Bos & Lind, 2013; Weick, 1995). The perception of treatment fairness is assumed to fulfill important functions in these processes of sense-making (Hulst et al., 2013). Future research is needed to examine all the implications of the alarm-system model of the fair process effect and to fill in empirically all the components of the view of the sense- Fair Process Effect 25 making individual discussed here. For example, issues that should be studied include the basic psychological mechanisms underlying the fair process effect. This includes the cognitive fluency with which information pertaining to fair and unfair treatment is processed in different circumstances (e.g., Winkielman, Schwarz, Fazendeiro, & Reber, 2003). This also includes identifying the conditions and applied contexts under which the alarm-system holds particularly well as well as the circumstances in which the model might be less relevant. Fair process effect may also be contingent on other information people receive, such as information about their outcomes (Brockner, 2010; Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996), and may depend on the organizational structure in which the fair treatment is experienced (Schminke, Ambrose, & Cropanzano, 2000). Besides organizational contexts a particularly relevant situation in which the fair process effect should be studied more intensively is the courtroom (Lind et al., 1990) and other judicial settings (Lind, Kulik, Ambrose, De Vera Park, 1993; Tyler, 1990; Van den Bos & Van der Velden, in press). Appropriate attention should also be paid to cross-cultural differences how people respond to fair and unfair procedures (e.g., Leung, 2005; Van den Bos, Brockner, et al., 2010, 2013). Thus, many issues need to be studied in future research. This noted, I think that some of the implications that already follow from an alarm-system perspective on the fair process effect and a sense-making view on organizational behavior stand in contrast with the view that humans are primarily interested in material gain and try to achieve this gain by rational means. A basic assumption underlying the model presented here is that people are individuals who are frequently busy appraising what is going on in their social surroundings and how to behave in these surroundings. I argue, therefore, that the Rational-Economic Man view that one finds so often in the fields of business, economics, law, and elsewhere in society (cf. Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1986) should be replaced or at least complemented by a view of humans as Social-Appraising Individuals (Van den Bos & Lind, 2013). Fair Process Effect 26 Of course, I am not denying that some people engage in selfish, exploitative, or even fraudulent behavior. This noted, although people's primitive core may sometimes (e.g., when their cognitive capacities have been severely limited) push them in an egoistic direction, it may well be the case that frequently people try to free cognitive resources to do the right thing (Van den Bos, Peters, Bobocel, & Ybema, 2006). Thus, I work from the assumption that fairness is frequently a very real concern to people (Miller, 1999; Miller & Ratner, 1998) and that people use their fairness perceptions to try to make sense of what is going and how to behave in appropriate manners (Lind & Van den Bos, 2013; Van den Bos & Lind, 2013). It is my hope that, with the current chapter, I have contributed a bit to what I see as a modern, sense-making perspective on organizational behavior and human resource management. Fair Process Effect 27 References Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267-299). New York: Academic Press. Alexander, S., & Ruderman, M. (1987). The role of procedural and distributive justice in organizational behavior. Social Justice Research, 1, 117-198. Ambrose, M. L., & Arnaud, A. (2005). Are procedural and distributive justice conceptually distinct? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice: Fundamental questions about fairness in the workplace (pp. 59-84). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Ambrose, M. L., & Schminke, M. (2009). The role of overall justice judgments in organizational justice research: A test of mediation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 491-500. Beauchamp, T. L. (2001). Philosophical ethics: An introduction to moral philosophy (3rd ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. S. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness. In R. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiation in organizations (pp. 43-55). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (1987). Interactional fairness judgments: The influence of causal accounts. Social Justice Research, 1, 199-218. Bobocel, D. R., Agar, S. E., Meyer, J. P., & Irving, P. G. (1998). Managerial accounts and fairness perceptions in conflict resolution: Differentiating the effects of minimizing responsibility and providing justification. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 20, 133143. Brockner, J. (2010). A contemporary look at organizational justice: Multiplying insult times injury. Routledge: New York. Fair Process Effect 28 Brockner, J., & Wiesenfeld, B. M. (1996). An integrative framework for explaining reactions to decisions: Interactive effects of outcomes and procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 189-208. Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386-400. Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O. L. H., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425-445. Cropanzano, R., & Ambrose, M. L. (2001). Procedural and distributive justice are more similar than you think: A monistic perspective and a research agenda. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational behavior (pp. 119-151). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Cropanzano, R., & Folger, R. (1989). Referent cognitions and task decision autonomy: Beyond equity theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 293-299. Cropanzano, R., & Folger, R. (1991). Procedural justice and worker motivation. In R. M. Steers & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Motivation and work behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 131-143). New York: McGraw-Hill. Cropanzano, R., & Greenberg, J. (1997). Progress in organizational justice: Tunneling through the maze. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 317-372). New York: Wiley. Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294-300. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290-292. Elovainio, M., Van den Bos, K., Linna, A., Kivimäki, M., Ala-Mursula, L., Pentti, J., & Fair Process Effect 29 Vahtera, J. (2005). Combined effects of uncertainty and organizational justice on employee health: Testing the uncertainty management model of fairness judgments among Finnish public sector employees. Social Science & Medicine, 61, 2501-2512. Epstein, S., & Pacini, R. (1999). Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dualprocess theories in social psychology (pp. 462-482). New York: Guilford. Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Folger, R. (1984). (Ed.). The sense of injustice: Social psychological perspectives. New York: Plenum. Folger, R. (1986). Rethinking equity theory: A referent cognitions model. In H. M. Bierhoff, R. L. Cohen, & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Justice in social relations (pp. 145-162). New York: Plenum. Folger, R. (1987). Reformulating the preconditions of resentment: A referent cognitions model. In J. C. Masters, & W. P. Smith (Eds.), Social comparison, social justice, and relative deprivation: Theoretical, empirical, and policy perspectives (pp. 183-215). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Folger, R. (1993). Reactions to mistreatment at work. In K. Murnigham (Ed.), Social psychology in organizations: Advances in theory and research (pp. 161-183). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Folger, R., & Baron, R. A. (1996). Violence and hostility at work: A model of reactions to perceived injustice. In G. R. VandenBos & E. Q. Bulatao (Eds.), Violence on the job: Identifying risks and developing solutions (pp. 51-85). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Organizational justice and human resource management. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Fair Process Effect 30 Folger, R., & Konovsky, M. (1989). Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions to pay raise decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 115-130. Folger, R., Rosenfield, D., Grove, J., & Corkran, L. (1979). Effects of "voice" and peer opinions on responses to inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2253-2261. Folger, R., & Skarlicki, D. P. (1998). A popcorn metaphor for employee aggression. In R. Griffin, A. O'Leary-Kelly, & J. Collins (Eds.), Dysfunctional behavior in organizations: Volume 1. Violent behavior in organizations (pp. 43-82). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Greenberg, J. (1990). Organizational justice: Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Journal of Management, 16, 399-432. Greenberg, J. (1993). Stealing in the name of justice: Informational and interpersonal moderators of theft reactions to underpayment inequity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 81-103. Greenberg, J. (1994). Using socially fair treatment to promote acceptance of a work site smoking ban. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 288-297. Greenberg, J. (1997). A social influence model of employee theft: Beyond the fraud triangle. In R. J. Lewicki, R. J. Bies, & B. H. Sheppard (Eds.), Research on negotiation in organizations (Vol. 6, pp. 29-52). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Greenberg, J. (2000). Promote procedural justice to enhance acceptance of work outcomes. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), A handbook of principles of organizational behavior (pp. 181195). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Greenberg, J., & Folger, R. (1983). Procedural justice, participation, and the fair process effect in groups and organizations. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.), Basic group processes (pp. 235-256). New York: Springer-Verlag. Greenberg, J., & Lind, E. A. (2000). The pursuit of organizational justice: From conceptualization to implication to application. In C. L. Cooper & E. A. Locke (Eds.), Fair Process Effect 31 I/O psychology: What we know about theory and practice (pp. 72-105). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Greene, J. D., Sommerville, B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 21052108. Hare, R. M. (1981). Moral thinking: Its levels, method, and point. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Hirschman, A. O. (1970). Exit, voice and loyalty: Responses to declines in firms, organizations, and states. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Hulst, L., Van den Bos, K., & Akkermans, A. (2013, January). Citizens processing information when interacting with legal decision-making authorities: The role of the behavioral inhibition system in the fair process effect. Poster presented at the Social Psychology and Law Preconference of the Fourteenth Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, New Orleans, USA. Huo, Y. J., Smith, H. J., Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1996). Superordinate identification, subgroup identification, and justice concerns: Is separatism the problem; is assimilation the answer? Psychological Science, 7, 40-45. Jasso, G. (1994). Assessing individual and group differences in the sense of justice: Framework and application to gender differences in the justice of earnings. Social Science Research, 23, 368-406. Jasso, G. (1999). How much injustice is there in the world? Two new justice indexes. American Sociological Review, 64, 133-168. Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. Journal of Business, 59, 285-300. Kant, I. (1959). Foundation of the metaphysics of morals. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill. (Original work published in 1785) Fair Process Effect 32 Kausto, J., Elo, A. L., Lipponen, J., & Elovainio, M. (2005). Moderating effects of job insecurity in the relationships between procedural justice and employee well-being: Gender differences. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 14, 431-452. Konovsky, M., & Folger, R. (1991, August). The effects of procedural and distributive justice on organizational citizenship behavior. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Miami Beach, FL. Korsgaard, M. A., Schweiger, D. M., & Sapienza, H. J. (1995). Building commitment, attachment, and trust in strategic decision-making teams: The role of procedural justice. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 60-84. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Cropanzano, R. S. (1987). Cognitive operations associated with individual differences in affect intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 767-774. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1986). Affect intensity and reactions to daily life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 803-814. Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenberg, & R. H. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27-54). New York: Plenum. Leung, K. (2005). How generalizable are justice effects across cultures? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice: Fundamental questions about fairness in the workplace (pp. 555-586). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lind, E. A. (1990). Arbitrating high-stakes cases: An evaluation of court-annexed arbitration in a United States district court. Santa Monica, CA: Rand Corporation. Lind, E. A. (1995). Social conflict and social justice: Lessons from the social psychology of Fair Process Effect 33 justice judgments. Inaugural oration, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands. Lind, E. A. (2001). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational behavior (pp. 56-88). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Lind, E. A., Ambrose, M., De Vera Park, M., & Kulik, C. T. (1990). Perspective and procedural justice: Attorney and litigant evaluations of court procedures. Social Justice Research, 4, 325-336. Lind, E. A., Greenberg, J., Scott, K. S., & Welchans, T. D. (2000). The winding road from employee to complainant: Situational and psychological determinants of wrongful termination claims. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 557-590. Lind, E. A., Kanfer, R., & Earley, P. C. (1990). Voice, control, and procedural justice: Instrumental and noninstrumental concerns in fairness judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 952-959. Lind, E. A., Kulik, C. T., Ambrose, M., & De Vera Park, M. V. (1993). Individual and corporate dispute resolution: Using procedural fairness as a decision heuristic. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 224-251. Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum. Lind, E. A., & Van den Bos, K. (2002). When fairness works: Toward a general theory of uncertainty management. In B. M. Staw & R. M. Kramer (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 24, pp. 181-223). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Lind, E. A., & Van den Bos, K. (2013). Freeing organizational behavior from inhibitory constraints. Manuscript in preparation. Maas, M., & Van den Bos, K. (2009). An affective-experiential perspective on reactions to fair and unfair events: Individual differences in affect intensity moderated by experiential Fair Process Effect 34 mindsets. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 667-675. Maas, M., & Van den Bos, K. (2011). Real personal uncertainty induced by means of taskrelated feedback: Effects on reactions to voice and no-voice procedures. Social Justice Research, 24, 107-125. McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, P. D. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as predictors of satisfaction with personal and organizational outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 626-637. Mikula, G., & Wenzel, M. (2000). Justice and social conflict. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 126-135. Miller, B. K., Konopaske, R., & Byrne, Z. S. (2012). Dominance analysis of two measures of organizational justice. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 27, 264-282. Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54, 1053-1060. Miller, D. T., & Ratner, R. K. (1998). The disparity between the actual and assumed power of self-interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 53-62. Moorman, R. H. (1991). Relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 845-855. Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Collins, N. L. (2005). The relational signature of felt security. Paper presented at the 2005 Conference of the Society of Experimental Social Psychology, San Diego, USA. Paternoster, R., Brame, R., Bachman, R., & Sherman, L. W. (1997). Do fair procedures matter? The effect of procedural justice on spouse assault. Law and Society Review, 31, 163-204. Patterson, P. G., Cowley, E., & Prasongsukarn, K. (2006). Service failure recovery: The moderating impact of individual-level cultural value orientation on perceptions of justice. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 23, 263-277. Fair Process Effect 35 Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and information negativity effects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 33-60). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1993). Citizenship behavior and fairness in organizations: Issues and directions for future research. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 6, 235-247. Proudfoot, D., & Lind, E. A. (in press). Fairness heuristic theory, the uncertainty management model, and fairness at work. In M. L. Ambrose & R. S. Cropanzano (Eds.), Oxford handbook of justice in work organizations. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Rawls, J. (1992). A theory of justice. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Original work published in 1971) Schminke, M., Ambrose, M. L., & Cropanzano, R. S. (2000). The effect of organizational structure on perceptions of procedural fairness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 294304. See, K. E. (2009). Reactions to decisions with uncertain consequences: Reliance on perceived fairness versus predicted outcomes depends on knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 104-117. Shapiro, D. L., & Brett, J. M. (2005). What is the role of control in organizational justice? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice: Fundamental questions about fairness in the workplace (pp. 155-178). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Thau, S., Aquino, K., & Wittek, R. (2007). An extension of uncertainty management theory to the self: The relationship between justice, social comparison orientation, and antisocial work behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 286-295. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Fair Process Effect 36 Erlbaum. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1978). A theory of procedure. California Law Review, 66, 541-566. Tyler, T. R., & Bies, R. J. (1990). Beyond formal procedures: The interpersonal context of procedural justice. In J. S. Caroll (Ed.), Applied social psychology and organizational settings (pp. 77-98). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tyler, T. R. (1990). Why do people obey the law? Procedural justice, legitimacy, and compliance. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Tyler, T. R., Boeckmann, R. J., Smith, H. J., & Huo, Y. J. (1997). Social justice in a diverse society. Boulder, CO: Westview. Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115-191). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Tyler, T. R., Rasinski, K. A., & McGraw, K. M. (1985). The influence of perceived injustice on the endorsement of political leaders. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 700-725. Van den Bos, K. (1999). What are we talking about when we talk about no-voice procedures? On the psychology of the fair outcome effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 560-577. Van den Bos, K. (2001a). Uncertainty management: The influence of uncertainty salience on reactions to perceived procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 931-941. Van den Bos, K. (2001b). Fairness heuristic theory: Assessing the information to which people are reacting has a pivotal role in understanding organizational justice. In S. W. Gilliland, D. D. Steiner, & D. P. Skarlicki (Eds.), Theoretical and cultural perspectives on organizational justice (pp. 63-84). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Van den Bos, K. (2005). What is responsible for the fair process effect? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Fair Process Effect 37 Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice: Fundamental questions about fairness in the workplace (pp. 273-300). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Van den Bos, K. (2007). Hot cognition and social justice judgments: The combined influence of cognitive and affective factors on the justice judgment process. In D. de Cremer (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of justice and affect (pp. 59-82). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Van den Bos, K., Brockner, J., Stein, J. H., Steiner, D. D., Van Yperen, N. W., & Dekker, D. M. (2010). The psychology of voice and performance capabilities in masculine and feminine cultures and contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 638648. Van den Bos, K., Brockner, J., Van den Oudenalder, M., Kamble, S. V., & Nasabi, A. (2013). Delineating a method to study cross-cultural differences with experimental control: The voice effect and countercultural contexts regarding power distance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 624-634. Van den Bos, K., Ham, J., Lind, E. A., Simonis, M., Van Essen, W. J., & Rijpkema, M. (2008). Justice and the human alarm system: The impact of exclamation points and flashing lights on the justice judgment process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 201-219. Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2002). Uncertainty management by means of fairness judgments. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 1-60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2009). The social psychology of fairness and the regulation of personal uncertainty. In R. M. Arkin, K. C. Oleson, & P. J. Carroll (Eds.), Handbook of the uncertain self (pp. 122-141). New York: Psychology Press. Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2013). On sense-making reactions and public inhibition of Fair Process Effect 38 benign social motives: An appraisal model of prosocial behavior. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 48, pp. 1-58). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Van den Bos, K., Maas, M., Waldring, I. E., & Semin, G. R. (2003). Toward understanding the psychology of reactions to perceived fairness: The role of affect intensity. Social Justice Research, 16, 151-168. Van den Bos, K., & Miedema, J. (2000). Toward understanding why fairness matters: The influence of mortality salience on reactions to procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 355-366. Van den Bos, K., Peters, S. L., Bobocel, D. R., & Ybema, J. F. (2006). On preferences and doing the right thing: Satisfaction with advantageous inequity when cognitive processing is limited. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 273-289. Van den Bos, K., Poortvliet, P. M., Maas, M., Miedema, J., & Van den Ham, E.-J. (2005). An enquiry concerning the principles of cultural norms and values: The impact of uncertainty and mortality salience on reactions to violations and bolstering of cultural worldviews. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 91-113. Van den Bos, K., & Rijpkema, M. (2008). Studying effects of exclamation point primes using functional neuroimaging. Unpublished manuscript. Van den Bos, K., & Spruijt, N. (2002). Appropriateness of decisions as a moderator of the psychology of voice. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 57-72. Van den Bos, K., & Van der Velden, L. (in press). Legitimiteit van de overheid, aanvaarding van overheidsbesluiten en ervaren procedurele rechtvaardigheid. The Hague: Ministry of the Interior and Kingdom Relations. Van den Bos, K., & Van Prooijen, J.-W. (2001). Referent cognitions theory: The role of closeness of reference points in the psychology of voice. Journal of Personality and Fair Process Effect 39 Social Psychology, 81, 616-626. Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1996). The consistency rule and the voice effect: The influence of expectations on procedural fairness judgements and performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 411-428. Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). Procedural and distributive justice: What is fair depends more on what comes first than on what comes next. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 95-104. Vermunt, R., & Steensma, H. (2003). Physiological relaxation: Stress reduction through fair treatment. Social Justice Research, 16, 135-150. Vermunt, R., Wit, A. P., Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (1996). The effects of unfair procedure on affect and protest. Social Justice Research, 9, 109-119. Walker, L., LaTour, S., Lind, E. A., & Thibaut, J. (1974). Reactions of participants and observers to modes of adjudication. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 4, 295-310. Walker, L., Lind, E. A., & Thibaut, J. (1979). The relation between procedural and distributive justice. Virginia Law Review, 65, 1401-1420. Weick, K. E. (1995). Sensemaking in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Weiss, H. M., Suckow, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). Effects of justice conditions on discrete emotions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 786-794. Fair Process Effect 40 Footnotes 1 In fact, later in this chapter I will argue that "fair treatment" better captures what we know about the phenomenon than "fair process." This fits with my line of reasoning that it is important to conceptualize the "fair process effect" more broadly than sometimes is done in the literature, an issue to which I will return later. 2 These fair process reactions were measured after the dependent variables that were reported in Experiment 4 of the Van den Bos et al. (2008) paper. Fair Process Effect 41 Figure Captions Figure 1. An alarm-system perspective on the psychology of the fair process effect. Note. The model assumes that people try to make sense of alarming events and that information about fair treatment encountered in processes of sense-making will calm people down and hence yield positive reactions, whereas information about unfair treatment will lead to increased activation of the alarm-system and hence instigate negative reactions among humans. Fair Process Effect 42