The Retina – the Neural System of the Eye Begins on G9 p 26 Retina: A layer of cells including receptors and neurons on the back of the interior wall of the eye. Figure 2.3 from G9 The Retina - 1 2/9/2016 Major types of neural cells making up the retina include: Pigment Epithelium Cone Rod Horizontal Bipolar Amacrine Ganglion Note that light passes through ALL of the cells in the retina before it strikes the receptors. Why do the receptors face away from the light? Nutrition. It’s more efficient to supply the nutrient-carrying blood to the receptors from the back of the eye. Blood supply to the retina is not shown in this figure. The Retina - 2 2/9/2016 The receptors G9 p 26 Two types – Rods and Cones. (Hmm – why do we have two types? Why not just one?) Chemistry of the receptors Receptors are filled with a complex chemical called the visual pigment Each pigment molecule is made up of two parts – a long protein called opsin and a much smaller component called retinal. In the dark, they are joined together. When light strikes it, the combined molecule changes shape. (First response of our bodies to light.) Prolonged exposure to light. Prolonged exposure to light causes the two parts of the molecule to become separated. When its molecules are separated, the visual pigment is not sensitive to light. Prolonged exposure to darkness. The molecule components re-join and become sensitive to light again. At any given time. At any given time, a proportion of the visual pigment molecules are joined and the rest are separated. The overall sensitivity of the receptors depends on the proportion that are joined. The Retina - 3 2/9/2016 Distribution of Rods and Cones G9 p. 28 (Aren’t you curious: Why two types?) 90° 90° Cones 0° This figure, taken from the web, is not quite to scale, but it shows clearly that the center of the retina is filled with only cones, while the periphery is filled with rods and also a few cones. Blind spot Macular Degeneration –Destruction of the cones in the center of the retina, leading to loss of central vision. Retinitus pigmentosa – Destruction of the rods in the periphery of the retina, leading to loss of peripheral vision. The Retina - 4 2/9/2016 Key differences in light handling ability of rods and cones. 1) Rod visual pigment is 1000 times more sensitive to light then that of cones. 2) There are three types of cones visual pigment. More later. Dealing with overall differences in light levels G9 p 27 In daylight. Rods. Nearly all visual pigment molecules are separated. Rods are max’d out. Rods are not used for vision in daylight. Cones. A working proportion of visual pigment molecules are joined and ready to respond to light. Cones are typically used for vision in daylight. Initially in darkness. Rods. Rod visual pigment begins to recombine. Cones. A substantial proportion of cone molecules were already combined. So initially, vision is based on cones. After 2nd minute in darkness. Rods. Rod visual pigment continues to recombine. Cones. All of the cone visual pigment is available, but sensitivity plateaus cause cone visual pigment is not terribly sensitive to low light. After about 7 minutes in darkness. Rods. Rod visual pigment continues to recombine, making the rods more sensitive than the cones. Cones. Cones are “dead to vision” because the light level is too low to active the cone visual pigment. After about 30 minutes in darkness Rods. Rod visual pigment at maximum sensitivity. Cones. Cones are still “dead to vision”. So all vision is based on rods. Why did pirates wear eye patches when on their ships? The Retina - 5 2/9/2016 Differences between receptors in sensitivity to wavelength – Spectral Sensitivity G9 p 33-34. Spectral Sensitivity Curves of the four types of receptors. R= Rods Most sensitive to 500 nm S= Short wavelength cones Most sensitive to 419 nm M = Medium wavelength cones Most sensitive to 531 nm L= Long wavelength cones. Most sensitive to 558 nm 419 500 531 558 Note that no receptor responds to light that we perceive as red. This reminds us that our perception is the result of a combination of receptor activity, not the result of one receptor alone. There are far fewer S cones than there are M and L cones. This explains why it’s so difficult to read fine blue print. Cone vision’s overall curve shown in Figure 2.19 is primarily determined by the M and L cones. The S receptors are actually sensitive to ultraviolet radiation, but the lens absorbs UV radiation, so none of it strikes the receptors. Note that we do not have receptors whose maximum sensitivity is greater than 650 nm, yet we clearly have hue experiences associated with wavelengths up to 700 nm. Purkinje Shift When it’s close to dark, rods begin to take over vision and cones are not activated by the faint light. The result is that blue-green objects, like trees, become brighter relative to yellow or red objects. This shift is called the Purkinje shift. In the daylight, the red flower will usually seem brighter. The Retina - 6 2/9/2016 At night, the blue flower will seem brighter. You won’t even be able to see the red flower because night vision uses only cones when cannot respond at all to the long wavelength light that we perceive as red. The Retina - 7 2/9/2016 Neural Processing After the receptors, all responses of the visual system are responses of neurons. The Nervous System – Central Nervous System – Brain and spinal cord - About 1 trillion cells yes, TRILLION. Peripheral Nervous System – Nerve cells in the muscles, skins, etc outside the spinal cord The Brain – About 85 billion (yes, BILLION) neurons – about 8% of the central nervous system. Cerebral Cortex – About 15 – 30 billion neurons – less than 3% of the CNS A 2 mm thick sheet of neurons surrounding other brain structures Cerebellum - About 60 billion neurons A structure on the back of the brain primarily involved in motor control. Note that many more neurons are involved in motor control than are involved in thinking. Cerebral Cortex 15-30 billion neurons Cerebellum 60 billion neurons The Retina - 8 2/9/2016 Neurons – the individual cells of the cerebral cortex and the cerebellum G9 p 33 This is Goldstein’s picture of a neuron. It’s as good as any others I’ve seen. Three main parts – Dendrites – Input area Cell body – Processing area Axon – Output area Neuron Behavior G9 p 36 Neurons do two things 1) Nothing 2) Engage in action potentials each of which lasts about .001 second. So if you record the “behavior” of a neuron, you’ll either record a constant negative voltage – the neuron resting – or you’ll record a positive spike of voltage – the neuron engaging in an action potential. Action Potential G9 p 38 A sudden (.001 second or so) change in the electrical balance between the inside and the outside of a neuron. Described well on page 38. Records of neuron behavior G9 p 37 Pictures of neuron behavior often look like a series of vertical spikes. Each spike is an action potential. Soft Stimulus Medium Stimulus Strong Stimulus Every neuron responds to changes in strength of stimulation by changing its rate of response. The Retina - 9 2/9/2016 Synapses G9 p 39 Input to a neuron – getting a neuron to emit an action potential Around the dendrites of a typical neuron is a mix of chemicals around the dendrites. Some of the chemicals may be excitatory neurotransmitter substances. Others may be inhibitory neurotransmitter substances. The response rate of a neuron depends on the proportion of excitatory to inhibitory transmitter substances. If the ratio of excitatory to inhibitory substance is “typical” for a neuron, it response randomly, at what is called the base rate of activity. That rate may be 10s of action potentials per second. If the ratio of E to I is high, the neuron will respond at a rate higher than the base rate. If the ratio of E to I is low, the neuron will respond at a rate below the base rate. Above base rate At base rate Below base rate “Typical” Amount of E relative to I transmitter substance Output of a neuron G9 p 39 When each action potential of a neuron reaches the end of the axon, it causes the release of some of the neuron’s own store of neurotransmitter substance. That substance was stored in small neural containers called vesicles. Neuron communication – neuron upchucking So neurons communicate by throwing neurotransmitter substance at each other. If a neurons dumps a lot of excitatory neurotransmitter substance, its activity will cause following neurons to increase their rate of activity. If a neurons dumps a lot of inhibitory neurotransmitter substance, its activity will cause following neurons to decrease their rate of activity. The Retina - 10 2/9/2016 Synapses – gaps between neurons - the places where neurons communicate The places were neurotransmitter substances get “dumped” and then have the potential to activate other neurons are called synapses. The word, synapse, means, roughly, neural gap. It is also used as a verb – meaning to connect with, neurally. Almost all neurons synapse with 100s of other neurons. This means that the neurotransmitter dump of a neuron can influence the firing rates of 100s of other neurons. Convergence – summing of neuron output If two neurons both release excitatory neurotransmitter substances, the result on following neurons will be double the excitation of either one acting alone. If two neurons both release inhibitory neurotransmitter substances, the result on the following neurons will be double the inhibition of either one acting alone. A A B B C C Convergence of Excitation Convergence of Inhibition Implications of Summative Convergence If neither the output of A nor of B is enough to affect C’s responses, perhaps the output of both together will be. Thus, neural convergence allows “weak” inputs to summate and ultimately achieve usable effects on the neurons which receive their outputs. The Retina - 11 2/9/2016 Excitatory / Inhibitory convergence – Neural comparisons (Not in G9) Suppose that A releases excitatory neurotransmitter substance and B releases inhibitory neural transmitter substance. Building block circuit Implications – If A and B both release neurotransmitter substance, C will be unaffected because the two will cancel each other out. C will continue to respond at its base rate. But if A releases excitatory but B releases nothing, C will respond above its base rate. (Assuming A’s excitatory release is sufficient to drive C.) Or if A releases nothing but B releases inhibitory neurotransmitter substance, C will be inhibited, responding below its base rate. So this circuit is a comparison circuit. C responds based on a comparison of the activity of A and the activity of B. A greater than B: A equal to B: A less than B: C responds above its base rate. C responds at its base rate. C responds below its base rate. This simple circuit is the building block for all of neural processing. We’ll see it again and again as we study perceptual phenomena – in color perception, in motion perception, in sound localization, to name a few. The Retina - 12 2/9/2016 Rod and Cone Systems The whole collection of rod receptors and their connections to other cells form the rod system. Similarly, the whole collection of cone receptors and their connections to other cells form the cone system. Convergence differences between rod and cone systems Rods are individually more sensitive than cones – about 1000 times more. But, in addition, the rods are wired so that they are far more convergent on following cells than are cones. This makes the rod system much more sensitive than the cone system. Rod system wiring Cone system wiring In the above, a faint stimulus to the rods will have 5 times as strong an effect on the red following neuron. But a faint stimulus to the cones will have an equally faint effect on each of the 5 following neurons in the cone system. The Retina - 13 2/9/2016 Studying perception in infants – preferential looking experiments G9 46 In the study of infants, pairs of stimuli are presented. If the infant looks equally long at each, then the conclusion is that the infant cannot tell the difference between them. If the infant looks more at one than the other, then the conclusion is that the infant can tell the difference between them. Often, the stimuli are designed to take advantage of infant spontaneous looking preferences – preferences for looking at objects with interior contours rather than a homogeneous field, for example. So an infant with the appropriate ability to see detail will look more at the rightmost stimulus in the pair to the left. As the interior contours get closer and closer together, the similarity between the two stimuli becomes greater. Ultimately, the infant looks equally often at both circles. This is an indication that the infant cannot tell the difference between them. The result, from a study of visual acuity in infants, is shown in Figure 2.39 Visual acuity plateaus to a level comparable to adults at just after 1 year. Measured using evoked potentials Interestingly, the change appears to be due to decreases in the sizes of the cones as the infant ages. From Figure 2.40 . . . Measured using preferential looking The Retina - 14 2/9/2016 Questions to consider 1. Why two types of receptor? Why not just one type? One outfielder cannot cover the whole outfield, so baseball has left fielders, center fielders, and right fielders. My guess is that the range of intensities to which we’re sensitive is too large to be covered by just one receptor type. Two receptor types – one for night and one for day provides better coverage of the range of lights in which we have to operate. Range of intensities in which we exist is about 1 million to 1. That is, the brightest sunlight in which we are able to use our eyes is about 1 million times more intense than the faintest starlight in which we can still see shadows. But the range of responses of receptors and the bipolar – ganglion cells to which they connect is only about 800 to 1. This means that significant changes in intensity would be represented by very small changes in response rate of the cells involved. This would likely result in many intensity changes going unnoticed. So the two receptors “divide” up the intensity range, with the rods taking the faint lights and the cones taking the bright lights. 2. Why 3 types of cone? Why not just one type? The short answer is that having three types of cones makes it MUCH easier for us to identify different wavelengths than if we had just one. If we had only one receptor type, the world would be only shades of gray. The fact is that the more cone types you have, the better able you are to discriminate wavelengths – the richer your color perception is. More on this in the color perception chapter. 3. Why don’t we have more than 3 types of cones? Wouldn’t our color vision be even better than it is now? The answer is yes. However it turns out that each type of cone requires many thousands of extra neurons to process and integrate the information from that type. But the brain has other things to do than to provide good color vision. What about music? Form? Touch? Arithmetic? Literature? Facebook? So the choice is to devote extra neurons to better color vision or devote them to processing auditory information, or form, or touch, or something else. The bottom line is that our brain is a compromise. More on this in the chapter on color The Retina - 15 2/9/2016