EAL Reading Research: A Review of Recent Research Findings Prepared by Fiona Middleweek Department of Education, University of Oxford January 2010 The following paper reviews the current literature on reading skills in children with English as an Additional Language (EAL). The review is intended as a brief guide, and a list of references is attached for further reading. Word Reading Skills Studies of EAL children in the US, Canada and UK and language-minority children in Holland consistently report that EAL children have equal skills to their English first language (L1) peers on tests of word decoding and phonological skills (see Lesaux et al., 2008 for an analysis of studies; also Jean & Geva, 2009). For example in the UK setting, a two year longitudinal study by Hutchinson et al. (2003), matching 43 EAL with first language English (L1) children on age, non-verbal intelligence and gender, found no significant difference between the EAL and L1 children on word reading skills (for similar findings, see Burgoyne et al, 2009). In an international setting, age appropriate decoding skills have been reported in Spanish EAL children with low socioeconomic (SES) backgrounds (Nakamoto et al., 2007) and in Dutch language-minority children (Verhoeven, 1990; 2000). Similar findings have been demonstrated in studies of Canadian EAL children with high and low SES and mixed L1s (Lesaux et al., 2006, 2007; Lipka & Siegel, 2007; Jean & Geva, 2009). 1 Reading Comprehension Several studies examining children’s reading comprehension have reported significant differences between L1 and EAL children on reading comprehension measures (e.g. Burgoyne et al., 2009; Hutchinson et al., 2003). For example, Hutchinson et al.’s (2003) two year longitudinal study of EAL and L1 children found that the EAL children performed on average one year behind the L1 children on the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (NARA). Importantly, the one year differential in reading comprehension scores persisted throughout the duration of the study (from years 2 to 4). This dissociation between word reading and comprehension abilities is also observed in studies of US Spanish first language children. For example, Nakamoto et al (2007) follow reading outcomes for 218 low SES Spanish L1 children from first to sixth grade learning EAL in a US bilingual programme. The study reports a decline in EAL children’s reading comprehension scores measured by the Woodcock Johnson Test of passage comprehension from a mean percentile of approximately 49% in first grade to just over 21% in grades five and six. Although passage comprehension scores were in the average range for EAL children in first grade, their scores began to drop in third grade and were in the bottom quartile for the normative English-speaking sample by sixth grade. However, the EAL children’s basic word reading skills scores remained in the average range for the normative English-speaking sample throughout the six year data collection period. Not all studies report EAL children experiencing difficulty with reading comprehension. Three papers from Canada (Lesaux et al., 2006, 2007; Lipka & Siegel, 2007) investigate longitudinal reading outcomes for middle elementary school children of mixed L1s. 2 Reading comprehension is measured by the Stanford diagnostic reading test (SDRT) and all three studies report no difference between the EAL and L1 groups on this measure. The reading comprehension abilities of EAL children are currently being investigated in Oxfordshire schools through the Reading and Language Comprehension Project (RLCP). EAL Children and ‘Poor Comprehenders’ For English first language children, the research literature suggests that approximately 10 - 15 % of 7 -11 year old children have age-appropriate decoding skills but poor comprehension abilities (e.g. Nation, 2005; Nation & Snowling, 2004 ). Children who are poor comprehenders have difficulty in specific comprehension skills such as making inferences and comprehension monitoring. In addition, these children generally demonstrate less well- developed listening comprehension skills and lower receptive vocabulary when matched for chronological age and reading ability with children who are skilled comprehenders It is probable that the distinction between poor comprehenders and poor readers is also applicable to EAL children (Lesaux et al, 2006). Vocabulary Vocabulary knowledge has been shown to be an important factor in reading comprehension and literacy development. Reading studies suggest that preschool vocabulary knowledge is a predictor of reading comprehension in later primary. For example, Cunningham and Stanovich (1997) show that vocabulary tested at first grade is a significant predictor of reading comprehension ten years later, accounting for more than 30% of the variance in eleventh grade. 3 Socio-economic status has been shown to affect vocabulary learning in young children. For example, qualitative and quantitative differences have been observed in the words encountered by high and low SES children (Hart & Risley, 1995) and the research suggests that children who come from lowincome backgrounds tend to have lower language abilities than their peers when they begin school (Hoff, 2006; Hoff & Naigles, 2002). EAL children have been shown to possess lower receptive and expressive vocabulary skills than first language English children in a number of recent studies (see Burgoyne et al., 2009; Hutchinson et al., 2003; Jean and Geva, 2009). Jean and Geva (2009) report that EAL children living in low SES areas may therefore face an ‘added L2 disadvantage’ in comparison to their L1 peers. The influence of SES on vocabulary learning for bilingual children should not be underestimated. For example, a recent report by Paradis (2009) suggests that parent’s level of education (an indicator of SES) may be as important a factor as language spoken at home in children’s developing vocabulary knowledge. Primary school attendance may not be a sufficient source of vocabulary acquisition (Biemiller & Boote, 2006) and it is age, rather than school attendance, that appears to affect vocabulary size (Christian et al., 2000). Moreover a ‘rich get richer’ effect has been observed in vocabulary interventions with young children (Penno et al., 2002). Biemiller (2005) estimates that whereas the average child adds an average of at least 840 root meanings a year in primary school, children in the lowest quartile add an average of only 570 root word meanings. In the US, a team at Harvard University has recently developed a vocabulary teaching programme for Grade 6-8 children. The researchers report that EAL children appear to make greater progress with this programme than first language English children. More details on this programme can be found at 4 www.wordgeneration.org. The programme focuses on cross-curricular teaching of academic vocabulary and has shown consistent improvement in children’s vocabulary knowledge and writing skills. For a concise report on the programme, see also White & Kim (2009). Listening Comprehension Listening comprehension has been shown to be an important measure of oral language skills and reading comprehension for L1 children. EAL children have been found to perform relatively more weakly compared to their L1 peers on tests of listening comprehension than on tests of reading comprehension. For example, Hutchinson et al. (2003) report that the EAL children perform two years below their L1 peers on an oral version of the NARA but one year behind on the written version of the same assessment. Similarly in the US, Proctor et al. (2005) observe that the fourth grade Spanish L1 children in their study perform at near grade level on the Woodcock passage comprehension test but perform closer to L1 second grade children on an equivalent test of listening comprehension. Other Reading Related Skills The majority of research has concentrated on EAL children’s phonological, word decoding and reading comprehension skills. However studies that have examined factors such as working memory have found no evidence of differences between the English first language and EAL children (see for example Jean & Geva, 2009). Home Literacy Environment Exposure to books in the home and parental teaching approaches have been shown to affect children’s reading achievement at primary school. This may 5 be an important factor for EAL children. For more consideration of the home literacy environment, see Senechal (2006). Conclusion The research evidence suggests that EAL children may continue to have areas of difficulty in literacy, specifically related to vocabulary knowledge and comprehension, even after five years of schooling. An emphasis on teaching vocabulary, particularly abstract and academic words, may be helpful for those children identified as having poor comprehension. However, further research is needed, particularly among older EAL children. 6 References Biemiller, A. (2005) ‘Size and sequence in vocabulary development: Implications for choosing words for primary grade instruction.’ In A. Hiebert & M. Kamil (eds.) Teaching and Learning Vocabulary: Bringing Research to Practice, Mahwah, N.J., Erlbaum, 223-242. Biemiller, A. (2006) ‘Vocabulary development and instruction: A prerequisite for school learning’ in D.K. Dickinson & S.B. Neuman (eds.) Handbook of Early Literacy Research: Volume 2, New York, The Guilford Press, 41-51. Biemiller, A. & Boote, C. (2006) ‘An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades.’ Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (1), 44-62. Burgoyne, K., Kelly, J., Whiteley, H. & Spooner, A. (2009) ‘The comprehension skills of children learning English as an additional language’ The British Psychological Society, 79, 735-747. Cunningham, A.E. & Stanovich, K.E. (1997) ‘Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience 10 years later.’ Developmental Psychology, 33, 934945. Cutting, L.E. & Scarborough, H.S. (2006) ‘Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured’ Scientific Studies of Reading, 10 (3), 277-299. Gregory, E. (1996) Making Sense of a New World: Learning to Read in a Second Language, London, Paul Chapman Publishing. Hart, B. & Risley, T.R. (1995) Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children, Baltimore, Brookes. 7 Hoff, E. (2006) ‘Environmental supports for language acquisition.’ in D.K. Dickinson & S.B. Neuman (eds.) Handbook of Early Literacy Research: Volume 2, New York, The Guilford Press, 163-172. Hoff, E. (2003) ‘Causes and consequences of SES-related differences in parent-tochild speech. In M.H. Bornstein & R.H. Bradley (eds.) Socioeconomic status, parenting and child development, Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum. Hoff. E. & Naigles, L. (2002) ‘How children use input in acquiring a lexicon.’ Child Development, 73, 418-433. Hutchinson, J.M., Whiteley, H.E., Smith, C.D., & Connors, L. (2003) ‘The developmental progression of comprehension-related skills in children learning EAL’ Journal of Research in Reading, 26 (1), 19-32. Jean, M. & Geva, E. (2009) ‘The development of vocabulary in English as a second language children and its role in predicting word recognition ability.’ Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 153-185. Lesaux, N.K., Geva, E., Koda, K., Siegel, L.S. & Shanahan, T. ‘Development of literacy in second-language learners’ in D.August & T. Shanahan (2008) Developing Reading and Writing in Second-Language Learners: Lessons from the Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth, New York, Routledge, 27-59. Lesaux, N.K., Lipka, O. & Siegel, L.S. (2006) ‘The development of reading in children who speak English as a second language.’ Developmental Psychology, 39 (6), 1005-1019. Lesaux, N.K., Rupp, A.A. & Siegel, L.S. (2007) ‘Growth in reading skills of children from diverse linguistic backgrounds: Findings from a 5-year longitudinal study.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 99 (4), 821-834. 8 Lipka, O. & Siegel, L.S. (2007) ‘The development of reading skills in children with English as a second language.’ Scientific Studies of Reading, 11 (2), 105-131. Nakamoto, J., Lindsey, K.A. & Manis, F.R. (2007) ‘A longitudinal analysis of English language learners’ word decoding and reading comprehension.’ Reading and Writing, 20, 691-719. Nation, K. (2005) ‘Children’s reading comprehension difficulties.’ In M.J. Snowling & C. Hulme (eds.) The Science of Reading: A Handbook, Oxford, Blackwell Publishing, 248-265. Nation, K. & Snowling, M.J. (2004) ‘Beyond phonological skills: broader language skills contribute to the development of reading.’ Journal of Research in Reading, 27 (4), 342-356. Penno, J.F., Wilkinson, I.A.G. & Moore, D. (2002) ‘Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect?’ Journal of Educational Psychology, 94 (1), 23-33. Proctor, P.C, August, D., Carlo, M. & Snow, C. (2005) ‘Native Spanish-speaking children reading in English: Toward a model of comprehension.’ Journal of Educational Psychology, 97 (2), 246-256. Senechal, M., Ouellette, G. & Rodney, D. (2006) ‘The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading.’ in D.K. Dickinson & S.B. Neuman (eds.) Handbook of Early Literacy Research: Volume 2, New York, The Guilford Press, 173-182. Senechal, M (2006) ‘Testing the Home Literacy Model: Parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially related to Grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling and reading for pleasure’, Scientific Studies of Reading, 10 (1), 59-87. 9