Running head: EFFECTS OF OSTRACISIM 1 Research Proposal Submitted by: David Nippard Psychology 341: The Self Submitted to: Professor E. Koch November 25th, 2013 EFFECTS OF OSTRACISM 2 Literature Review Living in today’s society the importance of inclusion and social relationships is crucial for individuals of all ages. That being said, the worry of being excluded and overlooked is something that people live with daily. The worry of exclusion is what is known as “ostracism”, and is present in the lives of everyone. To be ostracized is to be ignored and excluded, and is painfully experienced by individuals on a regular basis (Wesselmann et al., 2009). Ostracism can be experienced in many different ways with different levels of severity, for example, individuals may be left out activities at school while others may be ignored by not receiving a response from an email or a text message. Such examples may seem minor; however the act of being left out or being ignored play a significant role toward someone’s level of self-esteem and self-control (DeWall et al., 2012). Research suggests that individuals who are ostracized are susceptible to an increase in depression, more specifically, if the severity of the ostracism is high, then the degree of depression may also be high (DeWall et al., 2012). Furthermore, individuals who experience ostracism may not be able to hold their willingness to control impulses, which may lead to negative consequences. There may be a spiral effect with respect to ostracism, for example, individuals who experience ostracism may become depressed which may result in behaviours that can cause them to be ostracized even further, resulting in an even higher degree of depression (Dewall et al., 2012). When frequent ostracism is experienced, individuals lose the predisposition to control their impulses. Therefore, the level of ostracism may influence the degree of depressive symptoms and the capabilities of self-control (Baumeister et al., 2005). EFFECTS OF OSTRACISM 3 Not only does ostracism affect emotional levels, mood, and self-control, it also has physiological effects. Being ostracized can have a negative physiological impact resulting in higher blood pressure and higher levels of cortisol; known as the stress hormone (Coyne et al., 2011). Children are a common target for ostracism which becomes a threat to a child’s need to belong. Unfortunately, children who are exposed to ostracism may be victims of reduced physical activity and an increase in sedentary behaviours (Barkley et al., 2012). This is alarming because when children are less active and increasingly becoming sedentary, the result is commonly related to obesity and health concerns. If children are not experiencing the feeling of inclusion and are often times left out, they no longer have the desire to participate. Research has suggested that physical pain and ostracism are similar. The brain senses the “pain” and focuses on the source of ostracism, in turn, applying the proper coping mechanism. This coping mechanism could promote an increase in sedentary behavior (Williams, 2007). With the growing research and evidence of ostracism and its effects, researchers claim that other coping mechanisms may lead to unhealthy behaviours such as increased consumption of unhealthy foods. When individuals experience ostracism, their focus is directed toward the threat which causes their attention to be drawn away from self-regulation and self-control, resulting in an increase in food consumption (Salvy et al., 2011). Surprisingly not only does the target of ostracism experience negative outcomes, the source of the ostracism also experiences a negative outcome. Within certain conditions the experience of negative outcomes may be different for the target of ostracism, and the source of ostracism. For example, the target may perceive being ostracized as unfair and inexcusable, resulting in the target being angrier. However, the source of ostracism may experience the feelings of guilt and embarrassment for excluding or ignoring the target (Poulsen et al., 2011). EFFECTS OF OSTRACISM 4 Thus far, the discussion of ostracism has been directed toward individuals who have experienced being ostracized. What about individuals who often see others being ostracized in different social situations, does the observer empathize with the victim and feel what the target of ostracism is feeling? It may be possible that observing someone experiencing ostracism is sufficient enough to cause a personal distress to the individual that is observing the action (Wesselmann et al., 2009). The power of observing ostracism is strong enough that the feelings can be felt by the observer. For example, individuals who observed others being excluded from a game of Cyberball perceived the individuals as feeling ignored and excluded. Moreover, this observation of ostracism triggered an empathetic response to the individual who simply observed the action (Wesselmann et al., 2009). There has been a wide variety of research conducted on ostracism ranging from physical activity of children to the relationship of depression and being ostracized. However, little research has examined the effects on work performance or academic performance when exposed to ostracism. This research proposal will set up empirical tests to examine if ostracism will have a negative effect on work performance and academic performance. I hypothesize in study 1 that when individuals are exposed to ostracism by viewing a film, their work performance will decrease, and will report lower levels of self-esteem. I also hypothesize that when individuals are exposed to ostracism from participating in a game of Cyberball, their academic performance will decrease, and reports of self-esteem will also be low. Introduce the topic of work/academic performance…. EFFECTS OF OSTRACISM 5 References Barkley E. J., Salvy S. J., and Roemmich N. J. (2012). The Effect of Simulated Ostracism on Physical Activity Behaviour in Children. Pediatrics, DOI: 10.1542/peds.2011-0496. EFFECTS OF OSTRACISM 6 Baumeister R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciaracco, N. J., & Twenge, J. M. (2005). Social exclusion impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 589-604. Coyne S. M., Nelson A. D., Robinson L. S., & Gundersen C. N. (2011). Is Viewing Ostracism on Television Distressing? The Journal of Social Psychology, 151(3), 213-217. DeWall C. N., Gilman R., Sharif V., Carboni I., Rice G. K. (2012). Left out, sluggardly and blue: Low self-control mediates the relationship between ostracism and depression. Personality and Individual Differences 53, 832-837. Poulsen R. J., and Kashy A. D. (2011). Two sides of the ostracism coin: How sources and targets of social exclusion perceive themselves and one another. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 15(4) 457-470. Salvy S. J., Bowker C. J., Nitecki A. L., Kluczynski A. M., Germeroth J. L., Rowmmich N. J. (2011). Appetite 56(1), 39-45. Wesselmann D. E., Bagg D., Williams D. K. (2009). “I Feel Your Pain”: The effects of observing ostracism on the ostracism detection system. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45, 1308-1311. Williams K.D. (2007). Ostracism: The kiss of social death. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 1(1), 236-247. EFFECTS OF OSTRACISM 7