On the Symmetry and Discordance of Cultural Distance in Cross-border M&As: A TwoCountry Study H. Emre YILDIZ Stockholm School of Economics Box 6501, 11383 Stockholm, Sweden Tel. +(46-8)-736-9514 Emre.Yildiz@hhs.se Carl F. FEY Nottingham University Business School China 199 Taikang East Rd., Ningbo 315100, China Tel. +(86)-574-8818-0071 Carl.Fey@nottingham.edu.cn September 2013 Note 1: This article is a draft. Please do not cite without written permission of the authors. Note 2: The authors would like to acknowledge the kind help of Wang Zhongming, Adis Murtic and Karin Wiström with data collection. Note 3: The authors thankfully acknowledge financial support from the Jan Wallander, Tom Hedelius, and Tore Browaldh Foundation. 1 On the Symmetry and Discordance of Cultural Distance in Cross-border M&As: A Two-Country Study ABSTRACT Cultural distance occupies a central role in international business studies. Notwithstanding its prevalence as a key explanatory factor for a myriad of questions studied in the field, the concept of cultural distance entails several entrenched assumptions that have not been put to sufficient systematic scrutiny. Situated within the specific context of cross-border mergers and acquisitions, we address this gap and investigate assumptions of symmetry and discordance. We collected data through decision simulations with professionals in Sweden (n=154) and China (n=222) by replicating the same vignette design in both countries. Our findings reveal that distance perceptions are asymmetric and create contrasting attitudes among Swedish and Chinese respondents. The study shows that distance perceptions from Sweden to China, as perceived by Swedish respondents, is shorter compared to the distance from China to Sweden, as perceived by Chinese respondents. We also detect mixed empirical evidence regarding discordance assumption, which suggests that perceptions and reactions towards a foreign acquirer at different ends of a given country pairing do not necessarily mirror each other. Specifically, the results from Swedish participants suggest that cultural differences are an impediment to the success of post-merger integration. However, this is not the case for Chinese respondents, which casts doubt on the discordance of national cultural distance. Findings reported in this study pinpoint important theoretical implications regarding cultural distance, which could contribute not only to cross-border M&A literature but also to general IB research concerned with cross-cultural phenomena. 2 INTRODUCTION Understanding the effects of cultural, institutional, and political differences characterize much of the field we call international business (IB). Thus, the need for investigating the effects and implications of these differences defines one of field’s main raison d'être and has been fueling the academic motivation of many IB scholars. As unerringly pointed out by Zaheer et al. (2012), international management is essentially management of distance and research in this field has been primarily concerned with the coordination of firms’ activities dispersed across geographies. Accordingly, the extent of cultural differences and their implications have received significcant scholarly attention from diverse disciplines such as management, organization, marketing and strategy (for thorough review of relevant literature, see Kirkman et al., 2006). Despite the perpetual increase in the number of empirical studies, it is intriguing to observe that the lack of empirical consensus on role of culture and cultural differences in myriad of IB phenomena persists. Management of cross-border mergers and acquisitions (M&As) is no exception to this. Review articles (e.g., Schoenberg, 2000; Stahl and Voigt, 2005) have come to the conclusion that the relationship between cultural differences among merging organizations1 and the success of M&As is vaguely understood. In response, scholars looked for alternative ways for unraveling the role of culture and its net effects in M&As. Inter alia, this included (a) disentangling different levels (i.e., national vs. organizational) of cultural differences and separately examining their effects on post-acquisition integration outcomes (e.g., Sarala, 2010; Stahl and Voigt, 2008), (b) identifying possible moderators of the relationship between cultural difference and integration outcomes, e.g., acquisition experience (e.g., Dikova and Sahib, 2012), extent of operational integration (e.g., Slangen, 2006) or partner attractiveness (e.g., Very et al., 1997), and (c) adopting a more nuanced and fine-tuned approach by considering both positive and negative consequences of cultural differences between merging organizations (e.g., Reus and Lamont, 2009; Vaara et al., 2012). 3 Individually and collectively, these endeavors have significantly furthered our understanding of the effect of culture differences in M&As. In this paper, we wish to extend this stream of literature. To that end, we will problematize the assumptions upon and mechanisms with which cultural differences are theorized to have a bearing on individuals’ perceptions and likely responses in M&As. In this vein, we will question the plausibility of two underlying assumptions behind the measurement and use of cultural differences between merging organizations. Particularly, we will build on the shrewd theoretical treatise of Shenkar (2001) and scrutinize the assumptions of symmetry and discordance and examine the conditions under which cultural distance could yield asymmetrical and/or positive effects on the perceptions and possible responses of individuals in M&As. Indeed, most studies assume that cultural distance is symmetric (e.g., that people in China perceive Sweden to be as culturally distant as people in Sweden perceive China to be). In this paper we question if this is the case and investigate this empirically. If cultural distance is not symmetric, perhaps this is one reason for some of the inconsistent findings of the effect of cultural distance. Thus, despite this study’s setting in the M&A context and specific focus, our study’s findings likely have implications beyond this context. Accordingly, we argue that the lack of systematic attention paid to the tenability of the above assumptions could be the main culprit behind the lack of empirical consensus on the net effects of cultural differences and distance. Obviously, we are not the first to put these assumptions under systematic examination in the IB literature. Theoretical and empirical studies on expatriate deployment (Brock et al., 2008), expatriate adjustment (Selmer et al., 2007), mutual and bilateral managerial perceptions (Chapman et al., 2008; Muratbekova-Touron, 2011), headquarter-subsidiary relationships (Drogendijk and Holm, 2012) and mode of entry choice (Parente et al., 2007) scrutinize the assumptions apropos symmetric effects and directional equivalence of cultural distance. Conceptual arguments and empirical results of these studies lend support to the argument that symmetry assumption of cultural distance concept could be illusionary and misleading. To the best of our knowledge, however, no previous study explicitly tested the 4 asymmetry and discordance assumptions within the context of cross-border M&As, and their implications for organizational commitment and knowledge transfer in the post-acquisition stage and there is a need for more systematic and rigorous tests of the symmetry of cultural distance in general considering the importance of this concept for international business theory. Considering the effect of cultural distance in knowledge transfer (Björkman et al., 2007) and organizational commitment (Kirkman and Shapiro, 2001), as well as the role of organizational commitment and knowledge transfer in post-acquisition success (Birkinshaw et al., 2000; Bresman et al., 1999), we identify this as an important gap in the literature. With this study, we intend to address the aforementioned gap and contribute to the specific branch of M&A literature focusing on sociocultural dynamics of post-merger integration, along with the emerging stream of research aiming at a more nuanced view of cultural distance concept. In this regard, the first research question we aim to answer in this study is whether or not cultural distance has the same effect on acquired unit employees’ perceptions and potential behaviors, regardless of the route through which this distance is measured. In other words, ceteris paribus, can we observe similar pattern of perceptions and reactions among employees who work in Country A and whose company is acquired by a firm from Country B, compared to employees who work in Country B and whose company is acquired by a firm from Country A? While the first research question is aimed to put the symmetry assumption to empirical test, we also intend to scrutinize the discordance assumption inherent in the cultural distance hypothesis. Thus, the second research question of this paper is whether and whencultural differences lead to negative and positive reactions from acquired unit employees. Some of the studies we cited above have already addressed the ‘double-edged sword’ nature of cultural distance in international acquisitions (c.f., Reus and Lamont, 2009). Nonetheless, as we shall discuss later, the majority of these studies have anchored their arguments in different conceptual footholds. As a result, opposing effects and competing hypotheses regarding cultural distance are built on totally different (and rather 5 incommensurable) theoretical bases, which points to the need for a more coherent and theoretically calibrated approach to the matter. To address this, we will borrow theoretical frameworks from social psychology and formulate our arguments regarding the negative and positive effects of cultural differences on different integration dynamics in M&As accordingly. To answer the aforementioned questions we collected individual-level data in Sweden and China. Using an under-utilized research design (i.e., policy-capturing), we presented Swedish (Chinese) respondents with a series of acquisition scenarios, in which their firm is acquired by a Chinese (Swedish) firm. Following that, we asked respondents to express their willingness to use knowledge sent by the Chinese (Swedish) acquirer as well as their possible commitment to the merged organization. Our results confirmed that cultural distance has asymmetric effects on Swedish and Chinese respondents’ perceptions and likely behaviors in the context of M&As. In particular, perceived cultural differences between Sweden and China had more negative effects on Swedish respondents’ willingness to learn and commit to a Chinese acquirer compared to Chinese respondents. Furthermore, our analyses provides mixed results regarding the discordance assumption. On the one hand, compared to a domestic acquirer, Chinese respondents displayed more favorable and positive perceptions towards a foreign (Swedish) acquirer. On the other hand, our results based on Swedish respondents, who had more favorable attitudes towards a domestic acquirer vis-à-vis a foreign (Chinese) acquirer, were in line with discordance assumption The organization of the paper is as follows. In the next section we shall provide a critical review of earlier literature. This will be followed by the description of our research design. Next, we will present our results and interpret them in relation to the extant findings from earlier literature. We will end the paper with some concluding remarks and suggestions for future research. 6 LITERATURE REVIEW Culture in Previous M&A Literature: A Critical Reading The “cultural distance” hypothesis implies that cross-cultural contact and intercultural relations gets more problematic and difficult to manage when there are large cultural differences between organizations, groups and individuals (Hofstede, 1980). Measured by the differences between countries in terms of in values, norms, business practices, political, regulative disparities etc., cultural distance has been extensively used as a key explanatory variable to understand different phenomena by IB scholars (see Tung and Verbeke, 2010). Within the specific context of M&As, cultural differences have traditionally been considered a source of problem for synergy creation for they are argued to increase misunderstandings, animosity, and unease between the members of the merging organizations (e.g., Larsson and Finkelstein, 1999). Complications arising out of cultural differences are especially pronounced for cross-border M&As where the cultures of merging organizations are likely to differ on both national and organizational levels (Barkema et al., 1996). These theoretical arguments, along with somewhat limited empirical evidence, stand to reason for a negative effect of cultural distance on the creation of value, the achievement of sociocultural integration, the emergence of trust and the transfer of capabilities between merging organizations in cross-border M&As (Stahl and Voigt, 2005). However, cumulative empirical results on the net effects of cultural differences in cross-border M&As are far from being in accord. Positive, negative and non-significant effects of cultural differences have been reported in different empirical studies (Schoenberg, 2000). These inconsistent findings have left the influence of cultural differences between acquirers and acquired firms on post-acquisition integration outcomes as an issue yet to be resolved. Motivated by this, more recent studies started to adopt a contingency approach by identifying potential moderators and mediators. For example, Slangen (2006) examined the moderating role of integration level in the cultural distance – acquisition performance 7 relationship. His results reveal that national cultural differences are detrimental to acquisition performance only if the acquired unit is tightly integrated into the acquirer, whereas the effects of cultural differences were found to be positive in the case of loose integration. Dikova and Sahib (2012) theoretically argued and empirically showed that a higher degrees of cultural distance is a problem for inexperienced acquirers whereas firms with extensive acquisition experience are able to extract positive value by taking over culturally distant target firms. Focusing on the role of acquirer’s multiculturalism (i.e., tolerance and encouragement for cultural diversity), Reus (2012) reported the counter-intuitive finding that acquirers are unable to account for and manage emotional well-being of acquired unit members in culturally distant contexts even if they have a high degree of multiculturalism. Focusing on a specific and key integration outcome (i.e., trust), Stahl et al. (2006) detected an interaction effect between cultural distance and mode of takeover. In particular, they showed that acquired unit members’ trust towards a culturally distant acquirer would be lower in case of a hostile mode of takeover. With the ambition to reconcile earlier empirical inconsistencies, yet another stream of research sees the solution in a more fine-grained approach to the level at which cultural differences are theorized to have a bearing on integration outcomes. For instance, Sarala (2010) showed that the organizational cultural differences increase the extent of postacquisition conflict whereas cultural differences at the national-level were found to have insignificant effect on post-acquisition conflict. Using the same dataset but instead focusing on the effectiveness of knowledge transfer, Sarala and Vaara (2010) also found that national cultural differences provide opportunities for successful knowledge transfer whereas organizational cultural differences were not a significant factor in knowledge transfer. As a follow-up on this, Vaara et al. (2012) further found that there is a negative relationship between the degree of social conflict and the effectiveness of knowledge transfer. Differentiating between both level of culture and measures of post-acquisition performance, the meta-analyses of Stahl and Voigt (2008) revealed that differences in national and 8 organizational cultures have a negative effect on sociocultural integration (i.e., shared identity, positive attitudes, and trust). On the other hand, neither national nor organizational cultural differences seemed to have a meaningful effect on synergy realization (i.e., accounting based performance) or shareholder value (i.e., abnormal stock market returns). While the theoretical rationale behind differentiating between national and organizational cultural differences has an intuitive appeal, we argue that trying to reconcile extant empirical inconsistencies by recognizing the issue as a typical “level-of-analysis problem” could be ineffective. This is especially the case if the ambition is to understand the drivers of sociocultural integration by adopting a micro-level focus on the perceptions, feelings, attitudes and possible behaviors of individuals in acquisitions. That is, as far as the issue of post-acquisition integration is considered at the individual-level of analysis and from a socio-cognitive vantage point, explicit differentiation between national and organizational cultures and differences stemming therefrom would be an unavailing attempt. However, an individual’s membership in a nationality or an organization (or to any other collective affiliation for that matter) could be more or less relevant when s/he is deriving his/her selfconcept and making self-other comparisons accordingly. As argued by Regnér and Zander (2011), being large and complex organizations, multinational corporations embrace various subgroups with different identity frames. Therefore, it is an open empirical question whether members of merging organizations derive a larger or smaller portion of their self-image and concept from their nationality (e.g., being German), or from their organizational membership (e.g., being a member of Daimler organization) or from their profession (e.g., being an engineer). Hence, there is no reason to expect, a priori, national and organizational cultures to have differing effects on “us vs. them” type of thinking and the ensuing process of sociocultural integration unless one can (a) observe varying degrees of salience and relevance of these two bases of identity frames, (b) theoretically establish a link between primary variables of interest and the unique attributes of a specific context which is expected to make a difference in the salience and relevance of different identity frames. Alas, those studies 9 identifying the remedy as a more precise approach to the “level of culture” fail to offer solid theoretical and analytical ground regarding either of these conditions. As a result, their conclusions suffer from the same empirical inconsistency (e.g., national cultural differences have no effect on social conflict in Sarala ( 2010), whereas for the very same dataset with Finnish acquirers, these differences are found to have a negative effect on social conflict and positive effect on knowledge transfer in Vaara et al. (2012)) they attempt to puzzle out in the first place. In addition to differentiating between the different levels of culture, recent studies have also made notable attempts at understanding the positive and negative consequences of cultural differences with the aim of deciphering the net effects of cultural differences in the context of cross-border M&As. For example, Reus and Lamont (2009) portrayed cultural distance as a ‘double-edged sword’ in international acquisitions due to its impeding effects (i.e., different communication styles and amplified perceptions of ‘us vs. them’) along with its enriching effects (i.e., additional potential and opportunities for learning and knowledge combination due to cultural diversity). These arguments were preceded by an earlier study by Morosini et al. (1998) who found a positive effect of national cultural distance due to acquirers’ ability to extract rent-yielding routines and repertoires that would otherwise be unavailable in targets coming from culturally similar contexts. Björkman et al. (2007) also put forth a similar logic and took a step further by hypothesizing a non-linear relationship between cultural differences and value creating potential of cross-border M&As in which a low to moderate degree of cultural distance provides additional learning opportunities whereas at higher degrees of cultural distance exploiting these opportunities becomes problematic. Accounting for both the advantages and disadvantages of cultural differences in cross-border M&As is an important step to facilitate a more fine-grained understanding of culture in M&As. How ever, the string of studies mentioned above border on suggesting multiple theoretical frameworks for the sake of their ex ante arguments and/or explaining 10 their ex post empirical findings without paying due attention to the commensurability of the mechanisms with which they theorize on the pros and cons of cultural differences.2 To be more specific, majority of earlier studies conjectured that a primary source of problems in culturally distant acquisitions is the amplified feelings of hostility between members of merging organizations. On the other hand, the added value of higher cultural distance is argued to lie in additional diversity and learning opportunities. Although we agree with both of these points, we argue that the former part of the argument is relevant for socio-cognitive processes regarding group membership and social comparison mechanisms observed at the micro-level (i.e., interpersonal and inter-group relations) whereas the latter half of is more pertinent for issues of strategic fit, synergy creation, and resource combination which work at the meso- and macro-levels (i.e., creation and transfer of assets between organizations and across countries). This underlines the need for a more theoretically calibrated and coherent approach to studying the positive and negative aspects of cultural differences which we shall expand on in the following section. Towards a More Rigorous Approach to the Micro-level Effects of Culture in M&As In this section, we shall motivate our ensuing empirical study by detailing the rationale behind the (a)symmetric effects of cultural distance in cross-border M&As, as well as explicating the conditions under which cultural differences could be perceived favorably vs. unfavorably by individuals engaged in acquisitions. To that end, we shall first provide a brief overview of extant studies questioning the symmetry and discordance assumptions. With his acclaimed critique of extant conceptualization and measurement of cultural distance, Shenkar (2001) was the first to draw attention to the potential perils of these assumptions. Specifically, he argued that standard conceptualizations of cultural distance suffer from the “illusion of symmetry” and noted that “distance, by definition, is symmetric: The distance from point A to point B is identical to the distance from point B to point A [...] It suggests an identical role for the home and host cultures [...] However, there are no studies 11 showing symmetry between the two nor is there a reason to assume one” (Shenkar, 2001:523). To pinpoint the assumption of discordance, he further claimed that increased distance does not necessarily translate itself into lack of fit between interacting cultures and exacerbated obstacles to the relationship. Despite Shenkarr’s (2001) ideas, there have been a surprisingly limited number of empirical attempts to test the assumption of symmetry. One such example is the comparative study on expatriate adjustment conducted by Selmer et al. (2007) which found that German expatriates in the USA achieved a higher degree of socio-cultural and psychological adjustment and adaptation to the host culture compared to American expatriates in Germany. In another study, Brock et al. (2008) showed that discrete measures of cultural distance (i.e., power distance, assertiveness, institutional collectivism and in-group collectivism) have asymmetrical effects on the likelihood of assigning and deploying expatriates to subsidiaries. Providing an indirect test of both symmetry and discordance assumptions, Drogendijk and Holm (2012) focused on headquarters’ influence on subsidiaries and found that, instead of looking at the absolute differences between power distance scores, measuring the extent to which headquarters and subsidiaries agree on accepting or rejecting power differences could provide more accurate estimates. Understanding the theoretical reasons behind asymmetric effects of culture is as important as detecting them. In this regard, we can get some inspiration from the field of social psychology. Delving into the conceptual roots of the cultural distance hypothesis, it is not difficult to determine that most of the earlier studies conceiving of cultural distance as the main culprit behind “us vs. them” type of thinking based their arguments on the classical version of similarity-attraction paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Taylor et al., 1978) which simply avers that the more similar individuals/groups are, the higher would be the extent of attraction between them. Thus, cultural differences and dissimilarities have been theorized to have a negative influence on sociocultural integration due to individuals’ innate tendency to develop 12 more positive attitudes and favorable biases towards those who share similar characteristics and demographic attributes (Byrne, 1971). While conjecturing that similarity would breed intergroup attraction and cooperation, the majority of the studies in the M&A literature have developed their hypotheses in a way as if members of merging organizations always have a more positive attitude and favorable feelings towards their own pre-acquisition culture (as an exception to this, see Very et al., 1997). Yet, whenever this ex ante positive valence linked to pre-acquisition social identity is weak or not salient, the central premise of similarity-attraction and cultural distance hypotheses, as they have been applied in cross-border M&A research, becomes rather moot. Indeed, this point is explicitly acknowledged as a scope condition of Social Identity Theory (e.g. Leonard, 1975; Tajfel and Turner, 1986). Specifically, it is shown that the strength of the link between intergroup similarity and attraction depends on the extent to which individuals have strong identification with their group and internalize that group membership prior to engaging in social contact with the members of another group (Haslam, 2004).3 In other words, a more careful reading of Social Identity Theory reveals that individuals’ attitudes and behaviors towards the ‘other’ hinges on their subjective belief structures regarding their relative standing and status vis-à-vis relevant out-groups (Ashfort and Mael, 1989). Concerning intergroup relations in the particular case of M&As, the discussion above imparts two vital points. First, whenever members of an acquired unit perceive their organization to occupy lower position vis-à-vis an acquirer with higher status or image, they “will attempt, as individuals, to disidentify and gain psychological entry to the other organization. This will increase their support for the merger and their commitment to and identification with the new, merged organization”. (Hogg and Terry, 2000:133). This stands to reason that the effects of cultural differences and similarities between merging firms in M&As are more complex than assumed by previous studies. Therefore, instead of merely envisioning any kind of similarity (dissimilarity) to instigate feelings of attraction (repulsion) and sympathy (animosity), we shall explicitly examine whether the factors that render 13 merging firms similar (dissimilar) are seen and evaluated as favorable (unfavorable) by the parties. Second, given that these evaluations are based on relative characteristics and attributes of interacting groups, the way cultural differences are perceived and evaluated from either side of a given national pairing would not necessarily mirror each other. Therefore, understanding the net effects of cultural differences depends more on the qualitative characteristics of the cultural differences as perceived at either end of a dyad, and positive and negative connotations associated therewith, (c.f. Drogendijk and Zander, 2101) instead of a simple quantitative measurement of distance from one focal point to another. As a result, the effect of cultural differences would depend on the directionality and, therefore, would not be symmetrical between two nodes. Guided by these theoretical considerations and motivated by the dearth of empirical evidence regarding the plausibility of symmetry and discordance assumptions in the context of cross-border M&AS, we formulated two research questions: (1) Do cultural differences between two countries (i.e., A and B) have the same effect on perceptions and likely behaviors of acquired unit employees in Country A whose company is taken over by an acquirer from Country B as they have for acquired unit employees in Country B whose company is taken over by an acquirer from Country B?, (2) Do acquired unit employees in Country A react equally positively/negatively to the acquirer from Country B vis-à-vis their counterparts in Country B who are facing an acquirer from Country A? In the next section, we will present the general design and research methodology of our study with which we sought systematic answers to these questions. METHODOLOGY AND RESEARCH DESIGN In this study, we employed a policy-capturing technique, which is a method used for understanding the relative importance of different contingency variables in individuals’ decision-making (Karren and Barringer, 2002). As described by Aiman-Smith et al. 14 (2002:390), in policy-capturing “participants are asked to make decisions in response to a series of decision or problem solving scenarios presented by the researcher. The researcher regresses the decision outcomes on the values of one or more cues embedded in the scenarios and then uses the resulting regression weights to make inferences about the respondents’ judgment policies”. Thus, policy capturing can be considered a decision exercise in which stylized scenarios with different combinations of experimental treatments are presented to the respondents, who are then asked to express their perceptions and likely behaviors based on the information provided in the scenarios. In other words, instead of asking questions about respondents’ past or present experiences, researcher creates a realistic and controlled situation and directs questions to the respondents based on this constructed reality. As a sort of pseudoexperimental design, policy capturing can provide several advantages. These advantages include (1) the use of clean and reliable measures of variables, (2) higher control over compounding and the ability to rule out competing explanations for the results, and (3) the ability to establish causality more credibly than other alternative cross-sectional designs (Aiman-Smith et al., 2002; Croson et al., 2007). These unique advantages of policy-capturing technique are well-suited for the specific purposes of the present investigation. Since we are primarily interested in understanding whether or not the direction of a given cultural distance (from A to B vs. from B to A) matters on its effects and implications, policy-capturing design was an effective way to make sure that the data for either of these directions were minimally affected by the extraneous sources of variation. Although experimental designs are yet to take full purchase in the IB field, recent studies have started to use this design to examine issues like entry mode decisions of executives (Buckley et al., 2007), partner selection in international joint ventures (Reuer et al., 2012), response strategies in international strategic alliances (Tjemkes et al., 2012), the role of exposure to different cultural primes on cooperative behavior (Akkermans et al., 2010) and so forth. In the specific context of M&As, experimental designs have also been used in earlier research to understand the determinants of executives’ decisions on post-acquisition 15 integration level (Pablo, 1994), antecedents of acquired unit employees’ trust towards acquirers (Stahl et al, 2006), and effects of integration strategies and patterns on support (Giessner et al., 2006) and organizational commitment (Mottola et al., 1997) of acquired and acquirer unit employees. Participants In line with editorial provisos (Bello et al., 2009), we conducted our experiments with non-student samples in both China and Sweden. 222 Chinese (average age= 31.1, average work experience=6.7 years and 61% males) and 154 Swedish (average age= 43.9, average work experience=18.9 years and 86% males) professionals took part in this study.4 All of the participants from both countries had university degrees (i.e., bachelor or higher). 74.8% of Chinese and 64.7% of Swedish participants had prior experience with acquisition in their professional career. Research Framework As noted before, the primary purpose of this paper is to examine the effects of cultural distance between Sweden and China in the context of acquisitions. To test for these effects, we specified two dependent variables. Namely, these were acquired unit members’ willingness to implement organizational practices sent by the acquirer and their organizational commitment in the aftermath of acquisition, both of which have been considered important integration outcomes in earlier M&A studies (e.g., Birkinshaw et al., 2000; Bresman et al., 1999; Mottola et al., 1997; Stahl and Voigt, 2008). In addition to country-of-origin effects, we also manipulated three other predictor variables in our experimental treatments (see Figure 1), which were intended to provide some auxiliary insights. These were technological reputation of the acquirer, change in acquired unit members’ compensation, and change in their decision-making autonomy in the aftermath of acquisition. The role of reputation in post-acquisition integration is a relatively understudied issue (c.f., Davies and Chun, 2004) which warrants further empirical examination. In addition 16 to that, given that we build on SIT, the concept of reputation and image is directly relevant for our present investigation. On the other hand, analyzing the dual effects of compensation and autonomy is an intriguing exercise when considered within the framework of Self Determination Theory (e.g., Gagné and Deci, 2005) given that each of these factors is shown to have distinct effects on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. === Insert Figure 1 about here === Measures Acquired unit members’ willingness to adopt organizational practices sent by the acquirer was measured by a single-item 7-point likert-scale question (“As the new owner, if <<acquirer>> sends new organizational practices to my unit, I would be willing to utilize and implement these practices”). In order to measure acquired unit members post-merger organizational commitment, we adopted the five-item scaled from Mottola et al. (1997). These five items were factor analyzed and loaded on one item (Cronbach’s Alpha values were .903 and .896 for Chinese and Swedish data, respectively). In Table 1, we present the textual material used to manipulate the three independent variables we used in this study. In order to avoid possible confounds due to preconceived notions and ideas about real companies, we used fictitious names while describing the acquirers in the scenarios. We used a 1/4 replicate of 2x2x2 design to avoid respondent fatigue. Accordingly, each participant answered two acquisition scenarios with different experimental manipulations of the three independent variables. In addition to that, participants were asked to read and respond to the same set of scenarios where the acquirer was a domestic company. As a result, each participant responded to a total of 4 scenarios (2 with foreign acquirer and 2 with domestic acquirer). === Insert Table 1 about here === 17 Findings As an initial test of the symmetry hypothesis, we compared inter-country differences as perceived by Swedish and Chinese respondents. For this, we asked Swedish/Chinese participants to indicate the extent to which they found China/Sweden different from their country of origin along six items (i.e., norms and values, political system, regulations and formal rules, level of economic development, relationships and ways of communication, organizational practices). We then factor analyzed the six items and all of them loaded on one factor (Cronbach’s Alpha = .758), which accounted for 51.23% of the variance. Using this latent cultural distance measurement, we then performed an independent sample t-test to see if differences between Sweden and China, as perceived by Swedish and Chinese participants, differed depending on the nationality of the respondent. The results reveal that Swedish respondents’ distance perceptions (M = -.108, SD= .840) were smaller compared to those of Chinese respondents (M = .765, SD =.903) and the difference between the two sets of responses was significant, t (358) =1. 969, p<.05, Cohen’s d = .212. In the subsequent statistical analyses, we used a linear mixed model (LMM) with restricted maximum likelihood estimation. LMM is well-suited for the analysis of repeatedmeasures data in which the dependent variable is measured more than once on the same unit of analysis across levels of a repeated-measures factor.5 Thus, LMM enables researcher to detect the role of fixed effects (i.e., those that are of primary interest) while controlling for random effects (West et al., 2007). This is especially important to control for possible correlation between observations coming from same unit of analysis and to isolate the main effects of interest from un-modeled sources of variation. In our data we assigned a unique subject identification number to each respondent and specified a sequence number (from 1 to 4) for each response to the scenarios. Both of these counters were used to account for random effects. To test whether or not Swedish participants’ possible reactions to a Chinese acquirer were the same as Chinese participants’ reactions to a Swedish acquirer (i.e., symmetry 18 assumption) we pooled the two datasets together and coded each response with the dummy variable Respondent Country (0=Chinese, 1= Swedish). We then analyzed the data with Respondent Country, Reputation (0=weak, 1= strong), Compensation (0=same, 1= increased) and Autonomy (0=lost, 1= retained) as fixed effects. If valid, the symmetry assumption would predict that a Swedish acquirer making an acquisition in China should be facing the same degree of distance perceptions with a Chinese acquirer making acquisition in Sweden. Therefore, significant coefficient estimates for the Respondent Country variable would negate the validity of this assumption. The results are reported in Table 2a and 2b. Regarding our first outcome variable, acquired unit members’ Willingness to Implement Organizational Practices it is found that respondents’ country of origin was a significant predictor, which points cultural distnace having asymmetric effects.. To be more specific, Chinese respondents displayed a stronger willingness to implement organizational practices sent by a Swedish acquirer compared to Swedish respondents’ motivation to implement practices sent by a Chinese acquirer, t (683) = 2.585, p<.05. Concerning two of the other independent variables, the effects were in line with our theoretical predictions. Namely, increased compensation (t683 =1.806, p=.071) and retained decision-making autonomy (t683 =2.852, p=.004) positively affected Swedish and Chinese respondents’ willingness to implement organizational practices sent by the acquirer. On the other hand, strength of the technological reputation of the acquirer/sender did not emerge as a significant predictor of acquired unit/recipient’s willingness to implement organizational practices (t683 =1.170, p=.243). As for the other outcome variable, acquired unit members’ post-acquisition organizational commitment, the results followed a similar pattern. To be more specific, Chinese respondents’ commitment to a Swedish acquirer was stronger than Swedish respondents’ commitment to a Chinese acquirer, t (619) = 2.173, p=.030. Similarly, increased post-acquisition compensation (t341 =1.735, p=.084) and retained decision-making autonomy (t341 =6.160, p<.001) both increased the extent of individuals’ commitment to post-merger 19 organization. However, similar to willingness to implement organizational practices, commitment to post-acquisition organization was not affected by the technological reputation of the acquirer (t341 =.363, p=.717). Yet, further analyses also revealed a significant interaction effect between respondent country and acquirer’s reputation (t341 =2.712, p=.007). Specifically, Chinese and Swedish respondents had lower willingness to commit to the merged organization when the foreign acquirer had weak reputation. On the other hand, Chinese respondents had higher organizational commitment scores than Swedes in case the reputation of the foreign acquirer was strong. We also detected another significant interaction effect between respondent country and compensation (t341 =2.209, p=.028), which denoted that Chinese respondents had lower (higher) organizational commitment than Swedes should their salaries were retained the same (increased) in the aftermath of acquisition. === Insert Tables 2a 2b about here === Overall, the above analyses were instrumental to test the assumption of symmetry in an explicit and direct way. With subsequent analyses, we also examined Swedish and Chinese datasets separately and compared Swedish (Chinese) participants’ responses to Chinese (Swedish) acquirers with a baseline scenario where the acquirer was a domestic Swedish (Chinese) company. These further analyses provided two additional insights. First, it helped us to get a better understanding regarding the extent to which aforementioned variations in Swedish/Chinese employees’ responses to a Chinese/Swedish acquirer could be attributed to the nationality of the acquirer by isolating context-specific variations, e.g., differences between Sweden and China in terms of the way acquisitions are generally perceived, interpreted and reacted upon. Accounting for possible variations in the way any given organizational phenomenon is generically perceived and responded to across different contexts and cultures (c.f. Gelfand et al., 2007) is especially important to isolate and zero in on specific effects (i.e., country-of-origin of the acquirer) we are curious about in this study. Second, by comparing Swedish/Chinese participants’ responses to Chinese/Swedish acquirers with a case where the acquirer is a domestic (Swedish/Chinese) firm, we were able to test the 20 discordance assumption, i.e., whether or not cultural differences always lead to unfavorable perceptions of and reactions to the ‘other’. The results we obtained from Swedish data (see Tables 3a and 3b) confirmed that Swedish respondents were less motivated to implement acquirer’s organizational practices (t 381.2 = 2.578, p<.05) and were less likely to show strong organizational commitment to the acquirer (t 383 = 5.251, p<.001) in case their firm is taken over by a Chinese company. Thus, as far as Swedish respondents are concerned, it is possible to deduce that the nationality of an acquirer is an important predictor of acquired unit members’ willingness to commit to the acquirer and to implement organizational practices sent therefrom. Thus, the results from Swedish data lend support to the plausibility of discordance assumption. The effects of two of the other predictor variables, i.e., Reputation (t 381.3 = 2.272, p<.05 for practice implementation and t 383 = -2.776, p<.05 for organizational commitment) and Autonomy (t 381.3 = 4.206, p<.001 for practice implementation and t 383 = 3.294, p<.001 for organizational commitment) were also significant. On the other hand, change in Compensation did not have any significant effect Swedish respondents’ willingness to implement practices (t 381.3 = 1.011, p=.313) nor did it affect their commitment to post-merger organization (t 383 = .126, p=.900). In addition to this, we also detected a significant interaction effect between acquirer’s country of origin and its technological reputation (t 381.3 = 2.978, p<.05) === Insert Tables 3a 3b about here === The country-of-origin of the acquirer emerged as a significant predictor in the Chinese data as well (see Tables 4a and 4b). However, and contrary to our expectations and to the discordance assumption, Chinese respondents showed stronger willingness to use organizational practices sent by a Swedish acquirer compared to those being sent by a domestic (Chinese) acquirer, t (292.8) = 1.882, p= .061. Similarly, Chinese respondents displayed a stronger commitment to a Swedish acquirer compared to a case where the acquirer was Chinese, t (643.3) = 2.447, p< .05.Indeed, both of these findings were in contrast 21 with the one we observed in Swedish data where responses to a domestic (Swedish) acquirer were more favorable compared to those for a foreign (Chinese) acquirer (see Figures 2 and 3). Moreover, we also uncovered an interesting and significant interaction effect between acquirer country and reputation (t 381.3 = 2.287, p=.022) in our Chinese data. To be specific, when the acquirer’s technological reputation was weak, Chinese respondents were less likely to commit to a Swedish acquirer compared to a domestic/Chinese acquirer. However, under the strong reputation treatment, Chinese respondents’ willingness to commit to a Swedish acquirer became much stronger. Taken together, our analyses of the Chinese data provided evidence against the discordance assumption given that a culturally different (Swedish) acquirer would encounter a more motivated acquired unit/recipient vis-à-vis a domestic acquirer. In the next section, we shall provide interpretation of these findings, along with others, and discuss our results in relation to the previous literature. === Insert Tables 4a 4b about here === === Insert Figures 2 and 3 about here === DISCUSSION AND CONCLUDING REMARKS Three main conclusions can be drawn from our analyses. First, by measuring perceived cultural differences at both ends of the Sweden-China pairing we were able to provide an initial answer as to whether or not perceived cultural differences between Sweden and China follow a symmetric pattern or not. Our results revealed that the distance from Sweden to China (as perceived by Swedish respondents) was shorter compared to the distance from China to Sweden (as perceived by Chinese respondents). This finding should be interpreted along with those reported in earlier studies, which emphasized differences between objective and subjective distance measurements (e.g., Drogendijk and Slangen, 2006; Moon and Park, 2011). While relative merits of different metrics is an important yet unresolved issue in IB research, we don’t intend to pursue this at length. That said, however, we argue that choice for measuring a distance between two ends of a relational dyad should 22 be guided by the specific purpose of any given study. Thus, in so far as the aim of a study is to understand socio-cognitive mechanisms operating at the individual level of analysis, these results point out the usefuleness of using perceptual measures over objective/secondary data. Second, by pooling Swedish and Chinese responses to Chinese and Swedish acquirers, we were able to detect the implications of cultural differences on knowledge transfer and organizational commitment in M&As. To begin with, Chinese respondents showed stronger willingness to implement Swedish acquirer’s organizational practices compared to Swedish respondents prceptions towards practices sent by a Chinese acquirer. This result is in line with those reported by Gupta and Govindarajan (2000), who found that relative economic development levels of home and host countries are a significant determinant of the extent of knowledge flows between sender and recipient units. We further found a similar pattern for organizational commitment, i.e., Chinese respondents’ organizational commitment to Swedish acquirers was stronger than Swedes’ commitment to a Chinese acquirer. This finding corroborates well with the earlier remarks we quoted from Hogg and Terry (2000). The effects of two of the other explanatory variables (compensation and autonomy) were in line with expectations. However, we did not detect any significant interaction between the two suggesting that crowding-out effect is not tenable for the specific context of sociocultural integration in M&As. Interestingly, acquirer’s technological reputation did not have any effect on either of our dependent variables. This implies that the role of specific status characteristics, i.e., organizational reputation, is less noticeable than diffuse status characteristics, i.e., country of origin effects, (c.f. Berger et al., 1972). Third, in order to take the analysis a step further, we also examined our datasets separately and compared our respondents’ possible reactions to a domestic vs. foreign acquirers. This way, we were able to understand whether or not the way Swedish respondents react to a Chinese acquirer showed a similar pattern to the way Chinese respondents reacted to a Swedish acquirer. Over and above gaining more insights regarding the discordance assumption, these further analyses were also important to rule out alternative explanations 23 and attribute variation in Swedish/Chinese employees’ responses to a Chinese/Swedish acquirer to the nationality of the acquirer. For both of our dependent variables, responses of Swedish participants lent support to the idea that cultural differences are an impediment for the success of post-merger integration. On the other hand, this was not the case for Chinese respondents who expressed more favorable perceptions and possible reactions to Swedish acquirers compared to domestic acquirers. One possible interpretation of this difference between Swedish and Chinese respondents lies in the importance of individuals’ perceptions of their pre-acquisition culture and how they compare their own nationality with the cultural image of the acquirer. Indeed, satisfaction with existing group membership and identity was pinpointed as a significant determinant of the way with which individuals perceive and respond to the out-group in general (Tajfel and Turner, 1979), organizational (Hogg and Terry, 2000) and M&A (Ellemers et al., 1988) contexts. It is likely that, compared to their Chinese counterparts, Swedish respondents take greater pride in their nationality, and therefore depicted more positive reactions towards a domestic acquirer. To see if that is the case, we conducted supplementary analysis based on data we gathered from the World Values Survey. In particular, we compared Sweden and China according to the answer to the question “How proud are you to be <<nationality>>?” (1=very proud, 4=not at all proud).6 The results based on our indepdent samples t-test confirm that on average Swedes are more proud of their nationality(M= 1.71, SD=.717) than Chinese are(M=2.06, SD=.766). Furtehr, the difference between the two groups is significant, t2919=11.993, p<.001. This finding underscores the role of existing group membership (i.e., nationality), and favorable/unfavorable values attached to it, while forming attitudes towards a culturally different ‘other’. In addition to this, the different patterns of responses among Swedish and Chinese participants could also be due to the differing effects of foreignness across different institutional contexts, which might yield advantages as well as create liabilities for multinational companies due to variations in institutional structures (Edman, 2009; Kostava and Zaheer, 1999; Shi and Hoskisson, 2012). 24 In summarry, this study provides important systematic evidence suggesting that cultrual distinace is not symatric as most previous management studies have treated it. Thus, this study should cause researchers to pause and reflect about how they are using cultural distannce. The limitations of our study should be seen as fruitful avenues for further research. Our primary focus in this study was the initial conditions and resulting perceptions and likely behaviors of individuals. First, it would be valuable should future studies adopt a process perspective by opening the ‘black box’ of post-merger integration in cross-border M&As via longitudinal designs (c.f., Stahl and Voigt, 2005). Thus, we believe that a more nuanced understanding of initial contact conditions and how these initial conditions determine later stages of integration would contribute to the unpacking of this black box. Furthermore, while our choice of key integration outcomes was motivated and guided by theory, we believe the research framework we presented and tested here could and should be extended in order to see whether or not symmetry and discordance assumptions hold true for other dimensions of sociocultural integration in M&As. As an initial step towards a more systematic and rigorous test of symmetry and discordance assumptions, the present study is primarily interested in whether or not cultural differences are perceived and reacted to equally and similarly from each side of a given national pairing (i.e., Sweden and China). Yet, this leaves the roots and origins of asymmetric perceptions at either end of the side as a research attempt yet to be fulfilled by future studies. In addition to that, the particular design of our study has enabled us to test the assumptions of symmetry and discordance based only on the attitudes of individuals towards a culturally different acquirer. While social psychology research has shown that attitudes are good predictors of intentions and actual behavioral outcomes (Ajzen, 2001), we invite future studies to complement our present endavor and examine these two assumptions by looking at observeable behaviors of employees in cross-border M&As. 25 REFERENCES Aiman-Smith, L., Scullen, S.E., Barr, S.H. 2002. Conducting studies of decision making in organizational contexts: a tutorial for policy-capturing and other regressionbased techniques. Organizational Research Methods, 5(4): 388-414. Ajzen, I. 2001. Nature and Operation of Attitudes. Annual Review of Psychology, 52: 27-58. Akkermans, D., Harzing, A.W., van Witteloostuijn, A. 2010. Cultural accommodation and language priming. Management International Review, 50(5): 559-583. Ashfort, B.E., Mael, F. 1989. Social identity theory and the organization. Academy of Management Review, 14(1): 20-39. Barkema, H.G., Bell, J.H., Pennings, J.M. 1996. Foreign entry, cultural barriers and learning. Strategic Management Journal, 17(2), 151-166. Bello, D., Leung, K., Radebaugh, L., Tung, R.L., van Witteloostuijn, A. 2009. From the Editors: Student samples in international business research. Journal of International Business Studies, 40: 361-364. Berger, J., Cohen, B.P., Zelditch, M. 1972. Status characteristics and social interaction. American Sociological Review, 37(3), 241-255. Birkinshaw, J., Bresman, H., Hakanson, L. 2000. Managing the post-acquisition integration process: how the human integration and task integration processes interact to foster value creation. Journal of Management Studies, 37(3): 395-425. Björkman, I., Stahl, G.K., Vaara, E. 2007. Cultural differences and capability transfer in cross-border acquisitions: the mediating roles of capability complementarity, absorptive capacity, and social integration. Journal of International Business Studies, 38, 658-672. 26 Bresman, H., Birkinshaw, J., Nobel, R. 1999. Knowledge transfer in international acquisitions. Journal of International Business Studies, 30(3), 439-462. Brock, D.M., Shenkar, O., Shoham, A., Siscovick, I.C. 2008. National culture and expatriate deployment. Journal of International Business Studies, 39: 1293-1309. Buckley, P. J., Devinney, T. M., Louviere, J. J. 2007. Do managers behave the way theory suggests? A choice-theoretic examination of foreign direct investment location decision-making. Journal of International Business Studies, 38: 1069–94. Byrne, D. 1971. The Attraction Paradigm. New York, NY: Academic Press. Chapman, M., Gajewska-De Mattos, H., Clegg, J., Buckley, P.J. 2008. Close neighbors and distant friends – perceptions of cultural distance. International Business Review, 17: 217-234. Croson, R., Anand, J., Agarwal, R. 2007. Using experiments in corporate strategy research. European Management Review, 4:173-181. Davies, G., Chun, R. 2004. A reputational perspective on mergers. Advances in Mergers and Acquisitions, 3: 131-148. Dikova, D., Sahib, P.R. 2012. Is cultural distance a bane or boon for cross-border acquisition performance? Forthcoming in Journal of World Business. Drogendijk, R., Holm, U. 2012. Cultural distance or cultural positions? Analyzing the effect of culture on the HQ-subsidiary relationship. International Business Review, 21(3): 383-396. Drogendijk, R., Slangen, A. 2006. Hofstede, Schwartz, or managerial perceptions? The effects of different cultural distance measures on establishment mode choices by multinational enterprises. International Business Review, 15(4): 361-380. Drogendijk, R., Zander, L. 2010. Walking the cultural distance: in search of direction beyond friction. In T. Devinney, T. Pedersen, and L. Tihanyi (Eds.), The Past, Present 27 and Future of International Business and Management: pp. 189-212. Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing. Ellemers, N., van Knippenberg, A., De Vries, N., Wilke, H. 1988. Social identification and permeability of group boundaries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 497- 513. Edman, J. 2009. The Paradox of Foreignness. Ph.D. dissertation. Stockholm: Stockholm School of Economics. Gagné, M., Deci, E.L. 2005. Self-determination theory and work motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26: 331-362. Gelfand, M.J., Erez, M., Aycan, Z. 2007. Cross-cultural organizational behavior, Annual Review of Psychology, 58: 479-514. Giessner, S.R., Viki, G.T., Otten, S., Terry, D.J., Tauber, S. 2006. The challenge of merging: merger patterns, premerger status, and merger support. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32:339-352. Gupta, A.K., Govindarajan, V. 2000. Knowledge flows within multinational corporations. Strategic Management Journal, 21: 473-496. Haslam, S.A. 2004. Psychology in Organizations: The Social Identity Approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Hofstede, G. 1980. Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Hogg, M.A., Terry, D.J. (2000) Social identity and self-categorization processes in organizational contexts. Academy of Management Review, 25(1): 121-140. Karren, R.J. Barringer, M.W. 2002. A review and analysis of the policy-capturing methodology in organizational research: guidelines for research and practice. Organizational Research Methods, 5(4): 337-361. 28 Kirkman, B.L., Lowe, K.B., Gibson, C.B. 2006. A quarter century of Culture’s Consequences: a review of empirical research incorporating Hofstede’s cultural values framework. Journal of International Business Studies, 37, 285-320. Kirkman, B. L., & Shapiro, D. L. 2001. The impact of cultural values on job satisfaction and organizational commitment in self-managing work teams: The mediating role of employee resistance. Academy of Management Journal, 44: 557–570 Kostova, T., Zaheer, S. 1999. Organizational legitimacy under conditions of complexity: The case of the multinational enterprise. Academy of Management Review,24: 64–81. Larsson, R., Finkelstein, S. 1999. Integrating strategic, organizational, and human resource perspectives on mergers and acquisitions: a case survey of synergy realization. Organization Science, 10(1), 1-26. Leonard, R.L. 1975. Self-concept and attraction for similar and dissimilar others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(5): 926-929. Mottola, G.R., Bachman, B.A., Gaertner, S.L., Dovidio, J.F. 1997. How groups merge: the effects of merger integration patterns on anticipated commitment to the merged organization. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27(15): 1335-1358. Moon, T.W., Park, S.I. 2011. The Effect of Cultural Distance on International Marketing Strategy: A Comparison of Cultural Distance and Managerial Perception Measures. Journal of Global Marketing, 24(1): 18-40. Morosini, P., Shane, S., Singh, H. 1998. National cultural distance and cross-border acquisition performance. Journal of International Business Studies, 29, 137-158. Muratbekova-Touron, M. 2011. Mutual perception of Russian and French managers. The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 22(8): 1723-1740. Pablo, A.L. 1994. Determinants of acquisition integration level: a decision-making perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 37(4): 803-836. Parente, R., Baack, D.W., Almeida, V. 2007. Psychic distance and directional equivalence: a 29 theoretical framework. In S. Tallman (ed.) A New Generation in International Strategic Management: 308-325. Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Regnér, P., Zander, U. 2011. Knowledge and strategy creation in multinational companies: social-identity frames and temporary tension in knowledge combination. Management International Review, 51: 821-850. Reuer, J.J., Tyler, B.B., Tong, T.W., Wu, C. 2012. Executives' Assessments of International Joint Ventures in China: A Multi-Theoretical Investigation. Management and Organization Review, 8(2): 311-340. Reus, T.H. 2012. Culture’s consequences for emotional attending during cross-border acquisition implementation. Journal of World Business, 47: 342-351. Reus, T.H., Lamont, B.T. 2009. The double-edged sword of cultural distance in international acquisitions. Journal of International Business Studies, 40: 1298-1316. Sarala, R.M. 2010. The impact of cultural differences and acculturation factors on postacquisition conflict. Scandinavian Journal of Management, 26: 38-56. Sarala, R.M., Vaara, E. 2010. Cultural differences, convergence, and crossvergence as explanations of knowledge transfer in international acquisitions. Journal of International Business Studies, 41: 1365-1390. Schoenberg, R. 2000. The influence of cultural compatibility within cross-border acquisitions: a review. Advances in Mergers and Acquisitions, 1: 43-59. Selmer, J., Chiu, R.K., Shenkar, O. 2007. Cultural distance and asymmetry in expatriate adjustment. Cross Cultural Management: An International Journal, 14(2): 150160Shenkar, O. 2001. Cultural distance revisited: towards a more rigorous conceptualization and measurement of cultural differences. Journal of International Business Studies, 32(3), 519-535. Shi, W., Hoskisson, R.E. 2012. Advantages of Foreignness: Benefits of Creative Institutional Deviance, in Laszlo Tihanyi, Timothy M. Devinney, Torben Pedersen (ed.) Institutional Theory in International Business and Management: 99-125, 30 Advances in International Management, 25. Emerald Group Publishing Limited. Slangen, A.H.L. 2006. National cultural distance and initial foreign acquisition performance: the moderating effect of integration. Journal of World Business, 41(2): 161-170. Stahl, G.K., Chua, C.H., Pablo, A.L. 2006. Antecedents of target firm members’ trust in the acquiring firm’s management: a decision-making simulation. Advances in Mergers and Acquisitions, 5: 69-89. Stahl, G.K., Voigt, A. 2005. Impact of cultural differences on merger and acquisition performance: a critical research review and an integrative model. Advances in Mergers and Acquisitions, 4: 51-82. Stahl, G.K., Voigt, A. 2008. Do cultural differences matter in mergers and acquisitions? A tentative model and examination. Organization Science, 19(1): 160-176 Tajfel, H. 1982. Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33,1-39. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. 1979. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin and S. Worchel (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations: 33–48. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Tajfel, H., Turner, J.C. 1986. The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel, and W. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of Intergroup Relations: 7-24. Chicago: Nelson Hall. Taylor, S.E., Fiske, S.T., Etcoff, N.L., Ruderman, A.J. 1978. Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36: 778-793. Tjemkes, B.V., Furrer, O., Adolfs, K., Aydinlik, A.U. 2012. Response strategies in an international strategic alliance experimental context: Cross-country differences. Journal of International Management, 18(1): 66-84. Tung, R.L., Verbeke, A. 2010. Beyond Hofstede and GLOBE: improving the quality of crosscultural research. Journal of International Business Studies, 41: 1259-1274. 31 Vaara, E., Sarala, R., Stahl, G., Björkman, I. 2012. The impact of organizational and national cultural differences on social conflict and knowledge transfer in international acquisitions. Journal of Management Studies, 49(1): 1-27. Very, P., Lubatkin, M., Calori, R., Veiga, J. 1997. Relative standing and the performance of recently acquired European firms. Strategic Management Journal, 18(8), 593614. West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Gałlechki, A. T. 2007. Linear mixed models. A practical guide using statistical software. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC. Zaheer, S., Schomaker, M.S., Nachum, L. 2012. Distance without direction: restoring credibility to a much-loved construct. Journal of International Business Studies, 43: 18-27. 32 Figure 1: Research Framework Technological Image/Reputation of The Acquirer Acquired Unit Employees’ (1) Willingness to Implement Organizational Practices sent by the Acquirer Compensation Policy of the Acquirer (2) Post-acquisition Organizational Commitment Acquired Unit Employees’ Decision Making Autonomy 33 Table 1: Different Levels of Experimental Cues and Their Textual Descriptions in the Vignettes Construct Level Strong Technological Image/Reputation of The Acquirer Weak Retained Individuals’ Decision Making Autonomy Lost Fixed Compensation Increased Text <<Acquirer>> has an established image and a strong reputation for being an innovative company, given that its products successfully appeal to the needs and preferences of its customers. Thus, <<Acquirer>> is a clear technology leader. The success of <<Acquirer>>is mainly due to its strong research and development skills as well as its investments in getting a good understanding of the market trends. Instead of developing totally new products of its own, <<Acquirer>> usually adds incremental changes to the products of other companies, and then sells them under its own brand. In other words, <<Acquirer>> is more of a follower than a developer of new technologies. Even though <<Acquirer>> made several attempts to develop and launch totally new products, these attempts largely failed. <<Acquirer>> encourages different ideas. The top management of <<Acquirer>> believes that subsidiaries shall take their own initiatives since they have better knowledge of their local markets. This implies that you will retain much of the managerial autonomy over your own decisions. Therefore, you will not have to get approval from the headquarters for every action. The top management of <<Acquirer>> believes that centralizing most of the decisions is a better way to control and coordinate its subsidiaries. As a result, you will have less autonomy to make your own decisions at your job. Unlike before, in many cases you will have to get approval from the headquarters before you take any action. <<Acquirer>> adopts a compensation policy that aims at introducing minimum changes in the salary levels of acquired unit employees. As a result, your salary will be kept at the same level as before the acquisition. <<Acquirer>> adopts a standardized compensation policy (i.e., same pay for same position). <<Acquirer>>’s existing employees who work in your position have higher salary than what you used to receive before the acquisition. As a result, your monthly compensation will be increased considerably after the acquisition. 34 Table 2a: Parameter Estimates using Pooled Swedish and Chinese Responses (Foreign Acquirers only) Parameter Intercept a Respondent Country b Reputation c Compensation d Autonomy e Age Tenure Respondent Country* Reputation Respondent Country * Compensation Respondent Country * Autonomy Estimate 5.319364 .554670 -.175908 -.271084 -.427636 .006100 -.002327 -.274716 -.266216 .016408 Std. Error .432304 .214551 .150390 .150105 .149944 .012900 .011785 .195043 .195444 .190983 df t 683 683 683 683 683 683 683 683 683 683 Sig. 12.305 2.585 -1.170 -1.806 -2.852 .473 -.197 -1.408 -1.362 .086 .000 .010 .243 .071 .004 .636 .844 .159 .174 .932 a. Dependent Variable: Willingness to Implement Practices Sent by the Acquirer b. Swedish respondents are used as the baseline, c. Strong technological reputation is used as the baseline, d. Increased compensation is used as the baseline, e. Retained autonomy is used as the baseline Table 2b: Parameter Estimates using Pooled Swedish and Chinese Responses (Foreign Acquirers only) Std. df t Sig. Error Intercept a .069888 .425351 364.792 .164 .870 b Respondent Country .370741 .170626 619.074 2.173 .030 c Reputation -.032870 .090510 341 -.363 .717 d Compensation -.156753 .090339 341 -1.735 .084 e Autonomy -.555861 .090242 341 -6.160 .000 Age .005935 .013055 343 .455 .650 Tenure -.000386 .011933 343 -.032 .974 Respondent Country* Reputation -.318345 .117367 341 -2.712 .007 Respondent Country * Compensation -.259777 .117581 341 -2.209 .028 Respondent Country * Autonomy .144241 .114882 341 1.256 .210 a. Dependent Variable: Post-Acquisition Organizational Commitment b. Swedish respondents are used as the baseline, c. Strong technological reputation is used as the baseline, d. Increased compensation is used as the baseline, e. Retained autonomy is used as the baseline Parameter Estimate 35 Table 3a: Parameter Estimates using Swedish Responses (Domestic and Foreign Acquirers) Parameter Intercept a Acquirer Country b Reputation c Compensation d Autonomy e Age Tenure Acquirer Country* Reputation Acquirer Country * Compensation Acquirer Country * Autonomy Estimate 6.451637 -.312680 -.202800 -.090107 -.374121 -.012152 .009016 .049598 -.128877 .137395 Std. Error .640369 .121282 .089242 .089099 .088941 .020955 .018866 .126044 .125823 .125644 df 130.192 381.222 381.381 381.399 381.356 126.146 126.241 381.302 381.311 381.289 t Sig. 10.075 -2.578 -2.272 -1.011 -4.206 -.580 .478 .393 -1.024 1.094 .000 .010 .024 .313 .000 .563 .634 .694 .306 .275 a. Dependent Variable: Willingness to Implement Practices Sent by the Acquirer b. Swedish acquirer is used as the baseline, c. Strong technological reputation is used as the baseline, d. Increased compensation is used as the baseline, e. Retained autonomy is used as the baseline Table 3b: Parameter Estimates using Swedish Responses (Domestic and Foreign Acquirers) Std. df t Sig. Error Intercept a .936716 .485690 131.892 1.929 .056 b Acquirer Country -.530967 .101114 383 -5.251 .000 c Reputation -.205999 .074209 383.000 -2.776 .006 d Compensation .009310 .074068 383.000 .126 .900 e Autonomy -.243729 .073989 383 -3.294 .001 Age -.009458 .015869 127.000 -.596 .552 Tenure .002759 .014284 127.000 .193 .847 Acquirer Country* Reputation -.312524 .104947 383.000 -2.978 .003 Acquirer Country * Compensation -.087316 .104748 383 -.834 .405 Acquirer Country * Autonomy -.031407 .104636 383.000 -.300 .764 a. Dependent Variable: Post-Acquisition Organizational Commitment b Swedish acquirer is used as the baseline, c. Strong technological reputation is used as the baseline, d. Increased compensation is used as the baseline, e. Retained autonomy is used as the baseline Parameter Estimate 36 Table 4a: Parameter Estimates using Chinese Responses (Domestic and Foreign Acquirers) Parameter Intercept a Acquirer Country b Reputation c Compensation d Autonomy e Age Tenure Acquirer Country* Reputation Acquirer Country * Compensation Acquirer Country * Autonomy Estimate 4.908191 -.202411 -.442955 -.538190 -.403089 .038859 -.012491 .088647 .068530 .043119 Std. Error .557891 .107546 .097637 .098200 .092798 .019586 .018766 .106471 .106830 .100758 df 223.639 292.865 214.118 214.152 214.334 211.658 211.097 215.700 215.658 214.930 t Sig. 8.798 -1.882 -4.537 -5.481 -4.344 1.984 -.666 .833 .641 .428 .000 .061 .000 .000 .000 .049 .506 .406 .522 .669 a. Dependent Variable: Willingness to Implement Practices Sent by the Acquirer b. Swedish acquirer is used as the baseline, c. Strong technological reputation is used as the baseline, d. Increased compensation is used as the baseline, e. Retained autonomy is used as the baseline Table 4b: Parameter Estimates using Chinese Responses (Domestic and Foreign Acquirers) Parameter Intercept a Acquirer Country b Reputation c Compensation d Autonomy e Age Tenure Acquirer Country* Reputation Acquirer Country * Compensation Acquirer Country * Autonomy Estimate -.152133 -.230665 -.351215 -.416530 -.411620 .027967 -.014520 .218027 .111164 .063719 Std. Error .473064 .094264 .067314 .067803 .064048 .016695 .016007 .095317 .096001 .090608 df 221.372 643.303 642.799 642.799 642.799 213.690 213.707 642.994 642.983 642.852 t Sig. -.322 -2.447 -5.218 -6.143 -6.427 1.675 -.907 2.287 1.158 .703 .748 .015 .000 .000 .000 .095 .365 .022 .247 .482 a. Dependent Variable: Post-Acquisition Organizational Commitment b. Swedish acquirer is used as the baseline, c. Strong technological reputation is used as the baseline, d. Increased compensation is used as the baseline, e. Retained autonomy is used as the baseline 37 Figure 2: Willingness to Implement Organizational Practices Sent by a Foreign vs. Domestic Acquirer Figure 3: Post-Acquisition Organizational Commitment to a Foreign vs. Domestic Acquirer 38 1 By merging organizations, we refer to those firms that engage in an M&A. Thus, in case the transaction involves one firm taking over the other (fully or partially) and qualifies more as an acquisition than merger, the term “merging organizations” refer to acquirer and acquired unit. 2 An exceptional study of note in this regard is Weber et al. (1996). There, authors specify the positive aspects of cross-border M&As, relative to domestic ones, by underscoring individuals’ possibility of making anticipatory adjustments” in their behavior since they “expect differences and the changes related to those differences, and therefore are less likely to resist them” (p.1223). 3 This is simply because of the fundamental contention of Social Identity Theory, which argues that individuals have the inherent proclivity to maintain a favorable self-concept (Tajfel, 1982) and being identified with a similar other would not serve this purpose as long as the focal actor is similar to that other in terms of a negative self-concept or inferior social position. Based on the same logic, it is plausible to claim that a dissimilar other could be seen as more attractive in so far as s/he is dissimilar because of his/her more favorable social position and/or superior skills and capabilities. 4 While there is a noticeable difference between Swedish and Chinese respondents regarding average age and work experience, we control for these variables and their role is significant only in one of the models we will report in the next section. 5 Compared to classical repeated measures ANOVA and ANCOVA methods, LMMs are more effective in dealing with missing data, as well as handling unevenly observed repeated measurements. 6 We used the 4th wave dataset of the World Values Survey where this question was enumerated as #V209 and answered by 951 Swedes and 1,970 Chinese. The full version of the questionnaire and corresponding dataset can freely be retrieved from http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org and http://www.wvsevsdb.com . 39