The Transformative Role of Norms: From Values to Behavior and Back Norms play a key role in organizations, defining what is considered a desirable and appropriate behavior in a particular organizational setting (e.g., Dutton, Ashford, O'Neill, Lawrence, 2001; O’Reilly & Chatman, 1996). Employees are exposed to various organizational contexts and are mindful to the normative knowledge prompted by context. Consequently employees aim to adapt their behavior to fit the norms salient in a specific context. This symposium sheds new light on the interplay between the organizational context, norms and behavior, by combining two perspectives of norms. The first two presentations focus on the way cultural and professional values are translated into norms,influencing behavior. Taking a "topdown" perspective, these presentations describe how norms transform the abstract into the concrete. Taking a "bottom-up" perspective, the second two presentations focus on the way behavioral norms emerge from the immediate context of the task. The first two presentations focus on well defined contexts with established normative knowledge: Arieli, Elster and Sagivhypothesize and confirm that cultural mindset impacts how people approach a problem (i.e., their working norms), which in turn influences performance. They study different problems that call for different working norms. Rubel- Lifschitz and Sagivdescribe how the norms of a professional environment (the extent to which they legitimize power) moderate the impact of power differences within dyadic teams on creative performance. The last two presentations show that individuals are likely to consistently act according to desirable norms even when it is no longer beneficial: Nakashima, Halali, Samson, and Halevy show that a third-party intervention establishes cooperative norms even following a history of competition or when incentives are no longer used to enforce cooperative behavior. Similarly, Vashdi, Bamberger and Erez, show that action teams are prone to employ supportive communication norms even when it actually interferes with the performance-oriented behaviors of at least some team members. Varying in methods and participants, the four presentations in the proposed symposium offer rich evidence for the complex associations between organizational context, norms, and performance. The combination between field studies and cutting-edge lab experiments promotes a deeper understanding of the key role norms play in the performance of individualsand teams. Cultural Mindset, Norms and Problem-Solving: The Role of Language in Facilitating Problem-Solving Sharon Arieli1, Andrey Elster2, and Lilach Sagiv2 1 The Open University of Israel, 2The Hebrew University of Jerusalem A cultural mindset is a sense-making framework containing the norms, procedures, and goals of a specific cultural orientation(Oyserman, 2011). Thus for example, the cultural mindset of individualism evokes norms of standing out, whereas the cultural mindset of collectivism evokes norms of assimilation. In this research we study the impact of cultural mindset on performance in problem solving.We propose that cultural mindset influences how people approach a problem (e.g., the normative context), which in turn impact performance. To test this proposition we studied various types of problems, including problems that are more likely to be solved under individualistic norms (e.g., requires originality) and problems that are more likely to be solved under collectivistic norms (e.g., requires context-based thinking). People have a chronic tendency to employ an individualistic or collectivistic mindset, depending on the type of culture they grew in. However, since cultures are not strictly individualistic or collectivistic, but are rather composed of both orientations, individuals can shift from one mindset to another, depending on the situation (Oyserman, Sorensen, Reber, & Chen, 2009; Schwartz, 1999; Triandis, 1995). Researchers used various situational cues to experimentally prime individualistic versus collectivistic mindsets (Oyserman& Lee, 2008). In this research we used language as a prime for cultural mindset. For that aim, we studied Arab citizens in Israel who are fluent in both Arabic and Hebrew. Relatively to the dominant Israeli culture, the Arabic culture endorses collectivism more that individualism(Cohen, 2007; Oyserman, 1993; Schwartz, 1999). We therefore expected the Arabic language to prime a collectivistic mindset and the Hebrew language to prime an individualistic mindset. Study 1 focused on problems that require novelty and originality, and are thus congruent with the norms elicited by the cultural mindset of individualism. The participants (81) were presented with a problem in the domain of new product development and were asked to propose novelideasfor how it could be solved. As hypothesized, the results indicated that language has a direct effect on the originality expressed by the participants, such that participants in the Hebrew condition generated ideas that are more rare (i.e., original) than those in the Arabic condition (M = 7.61 vs. 9.22), F(1,70) = 4.58; p = .036. Study 2 investigated two types of insight problems: Rule-based and context-based. Rulebased problems are problems in which a solution is more likely to emerge by identifying a rule and applying it. These problems thus require analytical, systematic thinking. Context-based problem, in contrast, are more likely to be solved by considering the context and the relationships between elements. These problems thus require holistic, associative thinking. Research has shown (Arieli&Sagiv, 2015) that individualism facilitates success in solving rule-based problems, whereas collectivism facilitates success in context-based problems. Results of an experiment among 102 Arab citizens in Israel showed that problemtype (rule-based vs. context-based) moderates the association between language (Arabic vs. Hebrew) and performance:Arabic facilitates performance in context-based problems, whereas Hebrew facilitates performance in rule-based problems. This research shows the power of language as an unobtrusive cultural prime. Pointing to the role of language in inducing the norms of the cultural mindsets, our findings provide a new perspective to evaluate organizational policies that aim to establish lingual homogeneity (e.g., "one language fits all"). Switching from one language to another may change the cultural mindset of employees and managers, and consequently affect their performance. By matching the language with the task in question organizations may improve performance of employees. The Normative Context of Power: Implications for Dyadic Creative Performance Tammy Rubel – Lifschitz and LilachSagiv The Hebrew University of Jerusalem Dyadic power relations are inherent in various work processes. Planning a new project with your manager, co-counseling with a senior partner or writing a paper with your adviser all include power differences. Cross cultural studies suggests that the normative context of power plays an important role in the way power is perceived (Zhong et al., 2006; Torelli and Shavitt, 2010) and exercised (Kopelman, 2009). The current research contributes to the existing literature by examining how the normative context of power influences dyadic creative performance. Organizations and organizational units differ in the extent to which they transmit, promote and legitimize power values (Sagiv& Schwartz, 2007). Business schools, for example, endorse power values such as wealth, dominance and control. In this normative context, power differences are considered highly legitimate. Social work schools, on the other hand, endorse universalism values, such as equality and social justice. In this normative context, power differences are something that should be challenged and overcome (Knafo&Sagiv, 2004; Arieli, Sagiv, & Cohen-Shalem, 2015). We suggest that the normative context of power will interact with actual power differences in affecting dyadic creative performance. Study 1 was designed to investigate a preliminary hypothesis that power differences within a dyad will hider mutual creative performance. Past research suggests that though power can increase creativity among individuals (Galinsky et al., 2008), it harms communicative processes, and could consequently impair dyadic creative performance (for reviews see Anderson & Brown, 2010; Galinsky et al, 2014; Magee and Smith, 2013; Keltner et al, 2003). We therefore hypothesized that power will have a negative effect on dyadic creative performance, which will be stronger in tasks that require extensive social interaction. Participants included 148 university students, randomly assigned into dyads. Dyads created either a poster (high-social interaction) or a slogan (low social interaction) advertising their institution. To manipulate power, half of the dyads were told that in case they win, they could decide together how to divide a cash reward (equal-power). The rest were told that in case they win s/he would decide how to divide the reward (power differences). Following the task completion, the participants voted for the most creative product, and two expert judges rated the creativity of each outcome. As hypothesized, the products of equal dyads were rated as more creative in the highly interactive poster task - but not in the less interactive slogan task, among expert judges (F (1, 74) =4.008, p<0.05) and among students (F (1, 74) = 5.08, p<0.05). Study 2 was designed to investigate whether the detrimental effect of power on dyadic creative performance depends on the normative context of power. We hypothesized that the detrimental effect will decrease when power is legitimized, and increase when power is delegitimized. Participants were 88 MBA students at the business school (a normative context that legitimizes power) and 64 MA students at the Social Work school (de-legitimacy of power). Procedure was the same as in the poster condition in Study 1. Results replicate the detrimental effect of power on dyad’s creativity: Expert judges rated the posters of equal dyads as more creative than those of dyads with power differences (F(1,74)=7.28, p<.01). Also as hypothesized, this detrimental effect of power was larger among social work students than among business students. The difference between the creative performance of equal and unequal dyads was significant for social work (t=2.704, p<.01) but not for business students (t=.996, p=ns). To manipulate the normative context, the students in Study 2 were asked to advertise their school. Consequently,their personal value priorities were confounded with the normative context of the organization. To overcome this limitation, in Study 3 the same students (business) were asked to advertise one of two targets, that differ in their normative context. Thus, all students had similar value priorities, but the normative context differed. Participants were 130 BA business students, randomly assigned into dyads. Power was manipulated using a recall task (adapted from Galinsky et al., 2003). Dyads were then asked to produce a poster advertising either the business school (a normative context that legitimizes power) or the university (control). As expected, dyads primed with equal-power were more creative when they were asked to advertise the university, whereas dyads primed with high-power or low-power were more creative when they advertised the business school. This pattern was consistent across expert judges (F(1,82)=2.67, p=.10) and student ratings (F(1,84)=4.02, p<.05). Overall, the three studies suggest that power has a detrimental effect on dyadic creative performance, whichdecreases when the normative context legitimizes power. The study contributes to our understanding of the conditions that contribute and hinder dyadic creative performance, and may be useful in designing fruitful collaborative efforts in organizations. Third-Parties as Norm Changers: Third-Party Intervention in Repeated Conflicts Nathaniel Nakashima, EliranHalali, Ophir Samson, &Nir Halevy* Stanford University, Graduate School of Business*presenting author Third-parties have played a pivotal role in peacemaking since the dawn of history. Yet, little is known about how voluntary, informal third-party intervention in conflict influences disputants’ behavior or when and why third-parties choose to intervene in others’ conflicts. Recent research introduced the Peacemaker Game to study the interdependence between disputants and third-parties in conflict (Halevy &Halali, 2015). The Peacemaker Game involves two disputants and a third-party. The disputants choose to cooperate or compete; the third-party chooses whether or not to intervene in the conflict. A third-party’s decision to intervene introduces side-payments (rewards for cooperation, punishments for competition) that transform the conflict from a highly competitive situation, captured by a Prisoner’s Dilemma game, to a highly cooperative situation, captured by a Maximizing Difference game. To reflect real world tradeoffs between intervening and not intervening in a conflict, a third-party’s choice to intervene in the Peacemaker Game is risky: Not getting involved in a conflict results in a fixed, but small, payoff to the third-party, whereas choosing to intervene makes the third-party vulnerable to the choices of the disputants. If the disputants choose to engage in conflict, then the third-party loses the resources invested in intervening. However, if the disputants behave cooperatively, the benefits of peaceful coexistence shared by the disputants also produces gains for the third-party. Research using a one-shot version of the Peacemaker Game demonstrated that the mere possibility of third-party intervention significantly increases cooperation among disputants. Conflict, however, is seldom a one-time occurrence; rather, conflict is often experienced in the context of on-going, long-term relationships between parties. In this paper, we focus on repeated conflicts and investigate: (a) How introducing the possibility of third-party intervention after a history of conflict influences disputants’ tendencies to cooperate and third-parties’ own tendencies to intervene in the conflict; and (b) How removing the possibility of third-party intervention influences disputants’ tendencies to cooperate (i.e. whether and to what extent cooperation is maintained after the possibility of third-party intervention is no longer available). We introduce a repeated-game version of the Peacemaker Game in which parties make decisions and receive real-time feedback on everyone’s choices and outcomes for 60 rounds. We employ a between-subject design with two conditions. In the Early Intervention condition, thirdparties can only intervene in rounds 1-30 of the game; in the Late Intervention condition, thirdparties can only intervene in rounds 31-60 of the game. Our sample consisted of 48 3-person groups, 24 in each of the two experimental conditions, resulting in a total of 7,200 decisions by disputants and third-parties. Disputants in the Late Intervention condition cooperated approximately 47% of the time in rounds 1-30 of the game; introducing the possibility of third-party intervention increased their cooperation rates to 79% in rounds 31-60. Third-parties in the Late Intervention condition intervened at relatively high rates (85%) despite observing a prior history of conflict between disputants. Disputants in the Early Intervention condition cooperated approximately 92% of the time in rounds 1-30 (during which third-parties intervened 99% of the time); they continued to cooperate at high levels (85%) even after the possibility of third-party intervention was removed in rounds 31-60. Our research advances knowledge of peacemaking in repeated conflicts by third-parties. First, third-parties appear to be willing to assume the risk associated with trying to promote cooperation despite observing disputants’ prior history of conflict. Second, third-party intervention has a lasting effect on disputants’ maintenance of cooperation even after third-parties no longer have the ability to intervene. These findings highlight the potential benefits of constructively engaging third-parties in conflict as game changers who alter preferences and behavior, thereby transforming harmful competition into collectively beneficial cooperation. Is supportive communication always effective? The moderating effect of task complexity on the relationship between supportive communication and performance in healthcare action teams Dana R. Vashdi1, Peter A. Bamberger2 and Miriam Erez3 1 The University of Haifa, 2Tel Aviv University, 3Technion Communication has been theorized as the essence of social systems (Katz & Kahn, 1978) and at the heart of group behavior (Shaw, 1981). Empirical studies have shown team communication to play a critical role in enhancing team performance (Dionne, et al., 2004; Gardner, Gino, &Staats, 2012; Smith et al., 1994). Most studies of team communication focus on the frequency or quality of the team communication. Yet, communication scholars, have claimed that beyond its actual content every message communicates about itself (Pearce and Conklin 1979; Watzlawick, Beavin, and Jackson 1967). This has been called the communication form. Thus, beyond frequency and quality of communication, it is likely that the form of the communication will also impact team performance. In the current study we introduce supportive communication which delineates a general form of team communications that is accepting and appreciative and propose that it will be related to team performance as apparent in the team members behaviors.In the current study we concentrate on a specific type of team, action teams, and propose that while supportive communication may be an important influence on team performance generally, it is particularly important for the performance of action teams. Action teams are defined as "teams whose highly skilled members cooperate to perform urgent, unpredictable, interdependent, and highly consequential tasks while simultaneously coping with frequent changes in team composition and training their teams’ novice members” (Klein, Ziegert, Knight, & Xiao, 2006,p. 590). Thus, the communication form between team members may be of special importance in action teams in that the more positive and supportive the communications between team members are the better the team performance will be, as such communications are likely to “kick start” the kind of reciprocal, pro-social inter-relating behaviors that in long term teams result from well-established team norms (Nielson et al., 2010; Grant, 2007). However, while intuitively one would assume that supportive communication is likely to have positive effects for action teams in any context, the intense nature of activity characteristic of some of these teams, may actually create a situation in which such communication does more harm than good. Specifically, as such action teams are often performing highly complex tasks, which demand extreme amounts of focus, as lives may be at stake (Vashdi, Bamberger &Erez, 2013), such supportive communication may actually hamper performance as it draws members’ attention away from the tasks at hand. Accordingly, the performance-related impact of supportive communication may be equivocal. We propose to solve this conundrum by taking the nature of team tasks into account, positing that team task complexity moderates the relationship between supportive communications and performance. Using a sample of 409 surgical teams from a healthcare center in Israel we find that while the extent to which the communication among team members is supportive does not have a main effect on such teams’ performance, such a relationship is apparent when tasks are relatively simple. In contrast, under conditions of high task complexity, supportive communication actually interferes with the performance-oriented behaviors of at least some team members. As research on communication has emphasized that the content of any communication only provides a low percentage of the message with nonverbal aspects actually conveying most of the message (Mehrabian 1972, Guerrero & Floyd, 2006; Burgoon et al., 1996) this study emphasizes the need for more overarching theories of team communication including more than content and frequency. In addition, this study stresses that beyond including more aspects of team communication when theorizing regarding enhanced team performance, context is also critical. Leaning on theories of emotional and cognitive resources we contribute to mid level team theories by suggesting an offset effect of supportive communication in regard to its contribution to team performance. When cognitive load is high people are less likely to be involved in perspective taking (Roβnagel, 2000), making team members less aware that their fellow team mates are in need for help or information. Thus, even if supportive communication is high, in highly complex situations, team members may not benefit from it.