Young people’s perspectives of foster placement instability: A grounded theory approach Ruth Hunter Submitted for the Degree of Doctor of Psychology (Clinical Psychology) School of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences University of Surrey Guildford, Surrey United Kingdom July 2015 1 Abstract This qualitative grounded theory study aimed to explore experiences of placement moves for young people in foster care, an area neglected in previous studies. For young people these moves were distressing experiences of loss and uncertainty that they frequently lacked support to manage. Consequently the young people felt the need to be self-sufficient for their emotional needs. However this self-sufficiency often increased instability and distress as they struggled to influence others or withdrew in self-protection. Despite this, the young people displayed resilience and were hopeful for a better life after care. 2 Acknowledgements To all those who have played a role in my path up to this point, thank you. This achievement is partly yours. Unsurprisingly this encompasses a large number of people in my personal and professional life. But I would particularly like to thank God, my parents, my family and those closest to me. Thank you sounds so simple but I hope you realise just how grateful I am. 3 Contents MRP Empirical Paper 5 MRP Empirical Paper Appendices 64 MRP Proposal 104 MRP Literature Review 124 Brief Overview of Clinical Experience 192 Table of all Academic Assessments 195 4 MRP Empirical Paper Young people’s perspectives of foster placement instability: A grounded theory approach By Ruth Joanne Hunter Submitted in partial fulfilment of the degree of Doctor of Psychology (Clinical Psychology) School of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences University of Surrey May 2015 5 Abstract This qualitative grounded theory study aimed to explore experiences of placement moves for young people in foster care, an area neglected in previous studies. For young people these moves were distressing experiences of loss and uncertainty that they frequently lacked support to manage. Consequently the young people felt the need to be self-sufficient for their emotional needs. However this self-sufficiency often increased instability and distress as they struggled to influence others or withdrew in self-protection. Despite this, the young people displayed resilience and were hopeful for a better life after care. Introduction Globally, young people in care are one of the most at-risk groups for a range of negative outcomes both in childhood and later life (Minty, 1999; Barber & Delfabbro, 2003; Tapsfield & Collier, 2005; Ford, Vostanis, Meltzer & Goodman, 2007) as a consequence of early-life experiences (Newton, Litrownik, & Landsverk, 2000; Rubin, O’Reilly, Luan, & Localio, 2007). Placement instability, including unplanned changes, is a major predictor of negative outcomes from care (Rostill-Brookes, Larkin, Toms, & Churchman, 2011). The literature 6 refers to such changes using terms such as ‘breakdowns’, ‘moves’ and ‘disruptions’ (Unrau, 2007). Research indicates that multiple placements are associated with an increased risk of emotional, behavioural and mental health problems and also future placement breakdowns (Rubin, Alessandrini, Feudtner, Mandell, Localio, & Hadley, 2007; Strijker, Knorth, & KnotDickscheit, 2008; Stott, 2011). An understanding of what influences placement stability is key in developing effective interventions to address this and minimise negative outcomes. Understanding of Placement Stability The views of all stakeholders need to be included in a comprehensive understanding of placement breakdowns and efforts to reduce these (Wilson, Sinclair & Gibbs, 2000). There is an acknowledged lack of sufficient perspectives of young people, foster carers and social workers, as well as birth families and other professionals (Rostill-Brookes et al., 2011; Khoo & Skoog, 2013). The majority of research into placement instability has used quantitative methods (Unrau, 2007; Rock, Michelson, Thomson & Day, 2013), highlighting risk factors such as child problem behaviours, longer time in care; older age of child; multiple social workers; and separation from siblings (Koh, Rollock, Cross & Emblen-Manning, 2014; Rock et al., 2013). These quantitative approaches have frequently relied on psychometric or standardised measures, often failing to reliably represent the viewpoints of children or professionals (Unrau, 2007) and the broader systemic inter-relationships and interactions that may impact stability. Consequently the complexity of the psychological wellbeing of the 7 young person has also been overlooked. Perspectives of those involved in foster care When perspectives of key stakeholders have been considered, the use of labels for behaviour such as 'difficult' or 'problematic' highlights a dominant adult perspective. This perspective has likely been influenced by the need to retain foster carers through understanding and supporting them (Sellick, 2006). This focus is reinforced by research showing that the distress and sense of failure from placement breakdowns reported by foster carers can lead to reluctance to offer future placements (Nutt, 2006). Unfortunately, this focus on foster carers has resulted in a lack of appreciation of the child's voice and child and foster carers' interpersonal dynamics. Some attempts to obtain the perspectives of young people have occurred, but the success of these has been limited by the approaches used. Some researchers have attempted an indirect approach through seeking foster carers’ and social workers’ perspectives on the young people’s feelings and experiences (Unrau, 2007). Sommer, Pramling and Hundeide (2010) highlighted that gaining valid insight into the experiences of young people relies on going beyond a 'child perspective' to also include a 'child's perspective'. A ‘child perspective’ is the adult’s perception on children’s actions and experiences and perceptions. In contrast a ‘child’s perspective’ relates to the child’s view on their actions, experiences and perceptions based on 8 what they view as important. Obtaining ‘a child’s perspective’ Despite acknowledged difficulties in obtaining young people's perspectives on care and more specifically placement stability (Gilbertson & Barber, 2002), an increasing number of studies have attempted to address this gap of knowledge from the 'child's perspective'. However, these have often focussed on young people's perspectives of what leads to placement breakdowns and the impact of the breakdown, neglecting the process. For example, Hyde and Kammerer (2009) found that adolescents in care reported mis-matches between themselves and foster carers, leading to placement breakdowns. An earlier study by Butler and Charles (1999) found young people reported difficulties forming relationships with new carers following placement changes. This gap in understanding the process has begun to be acknowledged and explored; Unrau, Seita and Putney (2008) found that foster alumni remembered breakdowns as significant losses that continued to impact upon relationship success. However, this study included alumni who had left care up to 5 decades previously, whose experiences likely differ from those going through care services today. It is also likely that their perceptions will have changed over time. Another study into the process of moves highlighted that young people (9-15 years) wished for the placement process to be more inclusive and transparent in order to help them prepare for transitions, both emotionally and practically (Rostill-Brookes et al., 2011). 9 Despite this progress, these studies remain at the individual descriptive level, failing to develop a theoretical understanding of the process and impact of placement moves for young people. Research needs to go beyond simply obtaining the ‘child’s perspective’ to responding meaningfully to this: Building a coherent understanding of the experiences and perspectives of young people, grounded in theory to inform a more comprehensive understanding of placement instability. This can then inform more effective evidence-based practises to reduce the negative impact of placement instability and improve the well-being of fostered young people. This study therefore set out to address this gap and ascertain what are young people’s perspectives of the process and impact of foster placement moves? Approach Glaser and Strauss’s (1967) Grounded Theory approach was adopted due to the suitability of this approach for the broad research question as Grounded Theory is designed to explore phenomena of which little is known. Furthermore, the inductive approach of Grounded Theory facilitates the 'discovery' of processes that unfold and give meaning to social interaction, going beyond merely descriptive accounts of phenomena (Charmaz, 2006). Stance of the researcher Given the approach adopted, an avoidance of a priori assumptions was supported through a 10 non-theoretical stance adopted at the outset of the research. The gap in the area of research was identified but an exploration of potential applicable theories through a detailed exploration of the literature was avoided. The aim was to avoid the development of assumptions and preconceived theories that could increase bias in the exploration of the data and evolution of the consequent theory. Consequently, the research question was broad. 1 Aim This study aimed to explore the process and impact of placement moves from the perspective of young people in foster care to develop a preliminary foundation of understanding regarding the psychological processes and behaviour related to these perspectives. This work will also aim to provide an early platform for other researchers to further develop a coherent and theoretically-based account of these phenomena. Method The limited knowledge around young people’s perspectives of placement moves determined a qualitative methodology for the research. It was considered that young people would recount 1 The researcher’s stance and consequent impact on the process are discussed within the method 11 their experiences in relation to the social meaning they had attributed to the events, influenced by their social context. This social constructionist ontological position adopts a perspective that reality is influenced by social narratives rather than being fixed, with one 'truth'. This approach enables consideration of how different perspectives may be located in wider frameworks such as social, familial, cultural and political contexts. Greig, Taylor and MacKay (2007) described this as particularly valuable for research involving children, to enable ethical and useful research that is completed 'with' young people rather than being 'about' them. Historically, the voices of young people in foster care have been overlooked or moderated by adults and legal frameworks. Therefore the value of an approach that enables young people's voices to retain their own narratives in research is clear. Data Analysis: Abbreviated Grounded Theory Grounded Theory does not only aim to collect these experiences but to develop a framework of understanding. Indeed Glaser and Strauss (1967) designed the methodology to enable new theories to 'emerge' whilst retaining grounding in the context of the data. Further development to this approach has acknowledged the impact of the researcher’s role in the process (Willig, 2008) resulting in Charmaz's (2006) Social Constructionist version of Grounded Theory. This version acknowledges that the actions and influence of the researcher led to an interpretation of reality rather than a positivist 'true theory'. Furthermore, this study aimed to take an inductive approach. It was acknowledged that prior to this study, an 12 appreciation of the wider topic area was gained from scoping the research. It was understood that such exploration of the literature could influence the researcher's analysis of the data, alongside the researcher's previous experience and values. Reflections were used to help identify and minimise this influence. Previously highlighted difficulties with recruiting young people successfully for studies about foster care led to an anticipation that recruitment was likely to be difficult in this study (Gilbertson & Barber, 2000), which would have potential consequences for being able to saturate the data. Therefore Willig's (2008) Abbreviated Grounded Theory approach was adopted to enable development of a preliminary framework despite these anticipated limitations. It ensures analysis goes beyond descriptive accounts, though is more limited in its ability to broaden and refine the analysis through obtaining further data than a full Grounded Theory approach2. It was hoped that this project would work as a 'foot-in-the-door' study (Burger, 1999) developing understanding upon which to develop and direct further research. Ethics The University of Surrey Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences Ethics Committee gave a favourable ethical opinion for the study (See Appendix A). The research was also undertaken in line with British Psychological Society (BPS) and Health and Care Professions Council 2 See limitation section for discussion of key limitations. 13 (HCPC) guidelines (BPS, 2010; HCPC, 2012). Inclusion & Exclusion Criteria Inclusion criteria were broad, as an appropriate starting point for collection of data in a Grounded Theory study. Some criteria aimed to contain the parameters of exploration, given the small scale of the study. The exclusion criteria mainly focussed on ethical considerations, to prevent inappropriate participation in the study from causing distress or harm. Inclusion Criteria: Young people who experienced a placement breakdown in foster care after the age of 11 years, to avoid confounding placement transition issues with school transition issues. Young people 12-21 years of age. This ensured placement breakdowns had been experienced after age 11 but also gave recent care leavers an opportunity to share their views. Young people who were in mainstream schooling. Interviewing individuals with additional needs would have increased the likelihood of engagement difficulties and the risk of emotional distress that could arise. Exclusion criteria: Young people who experienced a placement breakdown only in kinship foster care, 14 due to the different arrangements associated with this form of foster care. Young people currently receiving mental health support. This was to avoid risking causing further distress to individuals potentially already distressed. Young people who had experienced a placement breakdown within the last six months or were at imminent risk of breakdown, due to the increased distress or difficulties that could result from discussion of placements. Sampling strategy and recruitment Given the historical difficulties in recruiting to studies regarding care experiences, theoretical sampling of participants was anticipated to be difficult. Due to time and resource constraints, selective sampling was used, based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. A number of strategies were used to recruit participants used. This included: using a social networking site, Twitter, to advertise to young people directly and to charities and organisations working with young people; emailing agencies and social service teams working with young people in and just after leaving care. In total 48 organisations were emailed. 108 followers on Twitter were obtained, including organisational and personal accounts. On Twitter, one young person was recruited from seven expressions of interest. Six further participants were recruited, from a charity and two social 15 care organisations. As only seven participants were obtained it was not possible to be selective in choosing which individuals to interview, limiting opportunities to discover further concepts, variations among concepts and gain further detail in relation to categories. Therefore the findings of this study remain preliminary and tentative. The process of recruitment ensured that participants had time to consider their involvement. Professionals passed the study information to young people, who then contacted the researcher directly to discuss the study and consider participation. Young people on Twitter contacted the researcher directly. Recruitment methods aimed to reduce perceived pressure to participate and facilitate young people to express their views freely. Consequently, young people were only contacted directly if they initiated contact or initial agreement to participate had been gained through associated adults. Accessible information to obtain informed consent; time to decide whether to participate; explicit opportunities to withdraw and to ask questions were all elements which supported the non-pressurising agenda. Careful consideration was given to issues of confidentiality, informed consent and maintaining participant wellbeing. Participants could withdraw from the study at any time. To manage wellbeing, the exclusion criteria excluded individuals currently experiencing distress 16 and after the interview, written and verbal debriefs were given (Appendices B & C). Followup contacts to monitor wellbeing were provided, with six choosing to access this from the support worker or social worker, and one from a direct follow-up contact with the researcher. Participant characteristics Seven participants aged 16-21 were included in the study: three females and four males. Six were White British and one was Romany-Gypsy. Six had left care in the last three years and one was just about to leave foster care. Demographic details are in Appendix D. One potential participant felt too nervous about participating and seven young people did not meet the criteria. Procedure Pilot interview and subsequent development of the interview schedule. To ensure the interview questions appropriately supported the collection of the young people’s perspectives on the research topic a consultation with a care leaver was undertaken. This provided confidence that the questions were clear and open and did not reinforce issues 17 of power3 (See Appendix E for the initial interview questions). Feedback resulted in the rephrasing of the questions: The term ‘placement breakdown’ was replaced with ‘unplanned placement moves’,4 understood as placements ending earlier than agreed in the child’s care plan (Rostill-Brookes et al., 2011). The initial interview questions were broad to ensure participants were able to raise the issues they perceived to be relevant. Additional interviews had more specific questions that arose from analysis of the data after each interview. (See Data Analysis section for further detail). (See Appendix F for later interview schedule). For example, some of the early interviews highlighted a reactive element to the experience of placement moves but later interviews explored what determined or influenced this reaction. (See Appendix G for example of how analysis contributed to interview schedule). The broad questions were retained in the schedule to ensure opportunities for further areas of relevance to arise in the interviews. Although all seven participants had multiple experiences to report on, analysis suggested that data saturation was not fully achieved. Some areas of data appeared saturated. These tended to be those that had arisen from the start and were discussed in each consequent interview, for example the impact of support on the experience and appraisal of the moves. However areas such as hopefulness (factors that influenced this) and impact on biological family 3 The intention was to support the young people to perceive themselves as an expert of their experience in the interview. Therefore, using terminology that the young people associated with authorities over them or found oppressive or biased were removed. This term was preferred by the consultee as it was perceived as more common language and that 'placement breakdown' was a more research-based term. Though the terminology was changed in the interviews, unfortunately this consultation occurred after advertisement and therefore the language of the recruitment material reflected previously used terminology. The term was defined at the start of the interview. 4 18 relationships appeared lacking in richness and depth at the end of the process. Questions remained about these areas and exceptions and disconfirmations were continuing to arise in the last interviews. Informed consent All participants were aged over 16 and able to provide consent. Written consent was obtained at the start of the interview following reiteration of the study details. (See Appendix H & I for Information and Consent sheets). Interviews Six face-to-face interviews were undertaken. The seventh interview was a phone interview at the request of the young person. The young people chose when and where to undertake the interview. The interviews lasted between 35 and 55 minutes and comprised demographic questions and a semi-structured interview. Data Analysis 19 The data were analysed using Willig's (2008) Abbreviated Grounded Theory approach, based on Charmaz's (2006) full version. (See Appendix J for a comprehensive description of the data analysis). Each interview was recorded and transcribed verbatim by the researcher. (See Appendix K for example of transcript and coding). Before the next interview, transcripts were coded line by line to identify initial codes, using the language in the transcripts to stay grounded in the data and limit interpretation. These codes were built up into themes by identifying similarities and links between these to build theoretical codes. (To maintain a stance that allows theories to emerge from the data, more prescriptive elements of some approaches to Grounded Theory were not used, such as ‘conditional matrices’ or an ‘axial coding paradigm’ (Strauss & Corbin, 1990)). Memos were used to explore questions and reflections that arose from the coding process. This helped the researcher to identify gaps of information (detail) to explore in future interviews. Theoretical sampling was also used to help direct further comparisons within the data. The memos also facilitated the researcher’s reflections on the data, to help identify reactions, assumptions and biases that could lead data collection and analysis in directions unintended by the young people. The approach of the interview was to acknowledge the young people as the expert of their experience. Clarifications and summaries were used to help ensure the 20 interviewer understood what was being shared by the young people and reduce the influence of researcher assumptions. Additional data from each interview enabled further exploration and validation of the theoretical codes that were emerging from the analysis. The aim was to help the researcher stay grounded in the data whilst developing a comprehensive understanding of concepts that would facilitate movement towards theoretical integration and allow for the development of a theoretical framework. Theoretical credibility was built through ensuring analytical credibility, using comparative analysis to build the initial and theoretical codes. Despite not being able to be more selective about future participants to expand on concepts and categories, systematic follow-up of elements in future interviews aimed to improve theoretical credibility of the relationships that emerged. However, the inductive approach of allowing participants to choose the stories they shared before focussing in on more specific lines of inquiry also helped to ensure additional experiences were not overlooked in favour of identified gaps from theoretical sampling (Dey, 1999). Following recommendations by Yardley (2000), supervision from a senior researcher was used throughout the analysis in order to build and maintain theoretical credibility, seeking guidance and feedback on the emerging codes and framework. This input also provided a space for reflection and transparency on the thought processes which influenced, and were 21 developed from, the data analysis. Results The following model (Figure 1) represents what the young people said about their move experiences, which centred around two main themes: Understanding and Survival. The main theme of ‘Understanding’ contains two subthemes of ‘Experience’ and ‘Support’. The ‘Survival’ theme contains three subthemes of ‘Action to Influence’, ‘Self-protection’ and ‘Future Focus’. The two main themes influence each other. However, not all the subthemes directly link to all others but are influential on a meta-level to the other main theme and the subthemes within it. It is emphasised that full saturation of the data was not achieved and therefore these results may lack some additional relevant details. Figure 1: Model of results 22 Understanding of placement moves was constructed from appraisals of past experience and influenced by the presence or absence of support. Experiences used in these appraisals were those from both within care and before care. 'Understanding' shed light on reasons for placement move experiences and informed expectations of the future. These informed survival responses subsequently. 'Survival' responses included active strategies intended to influence events or self-protective strategies to manage what was perceived as inevitable and hopeless (as informed by the ‘Understanding’ of placement moves). Hopelessness was also managed through a future focus on getting to the end of the care system. Reflecting on their experiences informed their understanding of the moves, which influenced the actions taken to survive them. The results of these actions impacted their subsequent experiences and support received, which in turn influenced future understanding and survival 23 responses. This highlights the cyclical nature of the model. The main purpose of this cycle appeared to be for the young people to reduce their distress, particularly from loss and lack of support. In the following sections the model will be illustrated by highlighting the key elements and then drawing attention to the links between the themes and subthemes.5,6 Understanding Accounts of placement moves were predominantly highly negative. Loss was highlighted as one of the most distressing elements of placements moves, exacerbated by the fact that they were frequently multiple and varied. “Absolutely horrible. I was crying … I just felt like all my life was gone between my feet.” (Amy) 5 6 Pseudonyms have been used to protect confidentiality. Appendix M examples a finding evidenced by multiple interviews. 24 The participants’ accounts emphasised their constant appraisals regarding their placement moves. These appraisals informed some emotional responses to the moves and expectations for the future. “Why is this happening? These are the things that were going through my mind. ‘What have I done that’s so wrong for me to deserve this?” (Lisa) Key in influencing these appraisals were their experiences; both pre-care and in-care. In addition, there was a clear emphasis on the influence of support on their appraisals. Experience Attempts to understand the events of placement moves were informed by previous experiences: the young people drew upon their pre-care experiences as well as prior experiences in care (if applicable). 25 “They don't really listen to you.…. So... they kind of do what they want and put you wherever you are and you can't really control [any of what happens].” (Rich) Pre-care experiences influenced the young people’s sense of responsibility around difficulties with placement moves. Those who entered care later had a greater understanding of the responsibility of adults to provide care for them and consequently attributed some of the responsibility for difficulties with moves to the professionals around them. Anger arose from perceptions that appropriate care was not being provided. Those who understood the responsibility of adults to provide appropriate care adopted an active stance to influence these adults. They also felt less guilt around the ‘disruptive’ behaviours used to achieve this. In contrast, the participants who entered care at a younger age took greater responsibility for difficulties associated with placement moves, acknowledging difficulties engaging with the process and giving greater credence to this as the reason for distressing placement moves rather than to adults around them. 26 “…’ where do you want to live?’ ‘Well, I don’t really know. I don’t really know what foster care is’... So a lot of the time I was just like ‘I don’t mind”. (Rich) Their own perceived failings led to a sense of hopelessness about being able to influence future move experiences and a tendency to focus on self-protection to minimise distress from future events. “I would love my choice to be increased but until I hit eighteen everyone's always trying to tell me what to do because in their eyes I'm still a child...” (Sian) Other pre-care experiences had a more negative impact on the move process. A common example of this was experiences of abuse creating difficulties in young people’s abilities to communicate their needs or dissatisfaction to those with power over their care. Such reflections only occurred in their later years in care, resulting in an increased sense of responsibility for difficulties with placement moves in the meantime. “I never felt…able to say to my social worker what I wanted …if you relate it to like my childhood, where I was abused... a lot of kids, they daren’t say something… about their parent figure as they feel that they’re going to get hurt.” (Jake) 27 This sense of responsibility and feeling unable to influence the situation was linked to attempts to protect themselves from anticipated future negative experiences, feeling unable to act to change these. “I got too attached and it really hurt when I left’… So I became very ‘to myself’ and I was like … ‘I’m not gonna talk to [foster carers]’.’’ (Amy) Experiences of being unable to make their needs known in care highlighted that in-care experiences also influenced appraisals of placement moves. Information from previous experiences in care was used to understand the actions of those influencing placement moves. For example, experiences of being ignored by social workers when trying to make their views heard or feeling their foster carers didn’t genuinely care about them were used to understand why they felt unprotected and uncared for during moves. “… I'd often overheard the stuff that [foster carer] would say like; 'Oh we wish we'd never taken her on' and all of this... I came back one day and [foster carer]’d packed my stuff and I just felt as if she was like, 'Yes, she's going. Let's get rid of her'.” (Lucy) 28 Such experiences consequently influenced expectations of future care moves, for example that their wellbeing would continue to be overlooked in favour of saving money and professionals could not be trusted to behave otherwise. “Participant: I was just so angry that my welfare… comes second to money. Like, it might be a job to them, like having to move me and you know, paying for it, but it’s my life. Interviewer: Did that have any impact on you? Participant: I didn’t trust Social Services… that was one.”(Adam) A hopeless sense of future care and issues of mistrust of professionals and foster carers were common and frequently followed by a sense of need for self-sufficiency; the need to take action to survive placements moves. “Social Services... they work to ensure my wellbeing. If they're not going to work to ensure my wellbeing then I damn well am.”(Jake) 29 Support Young people felt repeatedly rejected and unwanted from the experiences of moving placements and, with a lack of emotional and practical support, felt alone and afraid. Lack of support and information given influenced the conclusions around placement moves as young people frequently relied on their own experiences and interpretations. Positive support around understanding placement moves was uncommon and frequently selfblame was experienced. This went beyond a contextual conclusion regarding their behaviour to an integrated part of their identity. “Participant: I didn’t really have anyone talking to me about it, just, ‘You’re moving.’ … It really shocked me...What have I done that’s so wrong for me to deserve this? Interviewer: And what were the answers you came up with at the time? Participant: I was naughty. I was a naughty person.” (Amy) The lack of perceived entitlement to good care that resulted meant that such individuals attempted to survive care more often by taking personal responsibility. They adopted self- 30 protection strategies rather than acting to influence others, even though these strategies could be harmful (isolation, low mood, reduced access to support). “I didn’t want to focus on my day-to-day life so…I learnt to block things out...when I first went I didn’t know how to block so I just started trying to hurt myself...because I thought I was to blame for everything that went wrong in my life.”(Sian) However, one young person displayed no self-protective strategies despite a sense of responsibility around care moves, due to a more prominent sense of hopelessness. "I felt like ‘what’s the point in doing anything anymore,’ like, I started to give up on myself.”(Lisa) Negative views of themselves also came from adults around them in care, and influenced future expectations of their care. A particular sense of hopelessness along with self-blame followed these incidents. “They were like …‘You know no one’s going to want to foster you with your referral, you know that.’ So… everybody knew that… no foster placement would have me…”(Rich) 31 This profound sense of hopelessness was linked to attempts to self-protect and place a sense of hope in the end of their time in care. When positive support was received, the contrast in their understanding and consequent emotional and practical responses was very evident. Such support was often informative, empathetic and caring, providing both instrumental guidance and also a mindful awareness of the young person’s situation. In addition, young people’s understanding of their moves facilitated descriptions of what support would improve these experiences. “I think they should be allowed...they can keep in contact with them... it is like leaving your family...It's really sad. It's traumatic.” (Jake) Relational losses were repeatedly highlighted as the most distressing elements of moves. However, young people emphasised that support around this was only helpful when the professional (social worker, foster carer, other professional) supporting them knew them well. 32 “... I met my Social …on the day that I left…She was trying to comfort me because I was upset and I felt a bit... I felt a bit like she was intruding...”(Lucy) Positive support reduced the need to self-protect, providing emotional support and confidence to influence placement moves. Initially it also appeared that positive support relieved a sense of responsibility around placement moves. “My CAMHS worker said to me, ‘It’s not because you’re naughty, it’s because people don’t understand you.’ And then, that’s how I figured out that they obviously don’t understand me.” (Sian) However on exploring this in consequent interviews it became apparent that regardless of previous experiences, and whether they had fostered a sense of self-value, all participants appeared to feel some sense of responsibility for the difficulties they experienced around placement moves. When combined with a lack of support, this perception of responsibility led the young people to articulate that they had to survive by being self-sufficient as no one else would prioritise their emotional needs. 33 “… if I hadn't had that, like the strong will to do what was best for me and been articulate enough to tell everybody that that was what I wanted and been determined enough to fight for it, then I probably wouldn't have gotten what I needed.” (Lucy) However, this was the only participant to indicate that they felt they had got some of their needs met. Others indicated that their actions had only reduced distress. Relational losses were often of those who were supporting them, whether carers, biological family or friends. This reinforced the need for self-sufficiency to protect against these significant loss experiences. They invested less in these relationships as a self-protective device, which also reduced opportunities for future support to manage other distresses. “Cause I could have got closer to them…and it could have hurt me a lot… I was like, ‘No. I’m gonna cut all the ties…and not talk to any of them.”(Amy) Overall, the young people’s ‘understanding’ of placement moves frequently led to a conclusion that they needed to be self-sufficient to manage the distress resulting from placement moves and to minimise anticipated future distress. This came from a pervasive 34 lack of support during the moves, throughout and prior to care. The young people’s careful appraisals of placement moves influenced their beliefs around self, others and the future. These consequently informed their ideas about how to manage future experiences: how to survive placement moves. Survival As highlighted previously, whilst in care the young people’s understanding of their placement moves informed their survival responses; these centred on managing their current distress, and future anticipated sources of distress, and fell into three themes. ‘Action to Influence’ aimed to influence future events to negate distress whereas ‘Self-protection’ aimed only to limit the negative impact of what they were to experience. The third theme, ‘Future Focus’ centred on hope for the future. A focus on self-sufficiency was prevalent in all these themes, a clear result of the lack and loss of consistent support leading young people to depend on themselves. 35 “I would have been incredibly unhappy….But I wanted to fight for what was best for me rather than just…having adults tell me what to do.” (Adam) These attempts at ‘survival’ influenced future experiences: how the participants engaged with support; how they viewed their placement moves; their subsequent approach to surviving the experience. This illustrates the cyclical nature of the process whereby experiences and evaluations affected actions, which in turn influenced experiences and evaluations and so on. What was clear was the ‘no win’ situation the young people found themselves in, with attempts to reduce distress often creating alternative distress. A focus on the future after care provided a hopeful outlook in the midst of these dilemmas. Initially, the descriptions of survival responses appeared to be an ‘either/or’ scenario regarding the use of actions to influence. However further exploration in subsequent interviews identified that some young people engaged in both of these response approaches during a move. For example, they refused to engage with foster carers to reduce distress but also ran away from placements to influence their move. Less-distressing experiences were appraised as requiring fewer survival responses. For example, young people who felt known by, and who trusted, their social workers were more 36 accepting and less anxious of the placements chosen for them. They did not feel the need to run away from or refuse these placements (at least initially). “I could really tell how excited she was for me and that got me excited, ‘cause I was like ‘[she] knows me really well and I’ve got a good relationship with her.” (Rich) For these young people there was less of a need to be self-sufficient, giving more opportunities for their needs to be met by others and reinforce the benefits (and drawbacks) of this. In contrast, some of the young people were clear that the lack of positive support compromised their success in placements. “…you know if they'd have said ‘you're here for so long’…then I could have taken that in and gone ‘ok, I know my time here is limited’ I think I would have been a lot calmer... and stood a good chance of staying in the placement.”(Sian) However some responses were reactive rather than based on appraisals of the moves, such as becoming aggressive towards their social workers during the process of being moved. The young people then thought about the consequences of these actions and used these appraisals to decide whether to act the same way again in future. 37 “...That move was obviously immediate ‘cause I ran away to my dad’s… But then it changed...‘Cause the reason I [would] run away was …would be a lot of the time because I wasn’t being listened to.”(Rich) Action to Influence For some young people there was a motivation to survive and reduce distress that culminated in actions to achieve their desired improvements. Attempts to influence moves often focussed around the high level of distress associated with loss of relationships. Actions were intended, for example, to obtain better support from the social worker. Actions were often aimed at those whom they perceived had control over their placement moves: Social Services, foster carers, etc. They included direct negotiations regarding placement moves, ‘kicking off’, running away and refusal behaviours. Attempts to influence those in ‘control’ also included gaining support from other professionals to oppose a move. 38 “...She... listened to my Psychiatrist and listened to me… she overruled everything that [the social worker] said and I managed to stay in the place...” (Amy) Whilst young people’s actions to influence were generally meant to create positive change, they had mixed results. In fact, a key negative consequence of some behaviours intended to influence moves was the negative perception of the young person that resulted. This was part of a context of experiences of negative perceptions of young people in care that was prevalent in the accounts: actions that risked reinforcing these were heavily justified. For example, one young person attributed their ‘kicking off’ to their mum’s emphasis that they should fight to get their needs met, thereby reducing their sense of shame at being aggressive towards their social worker. “My mum raised me …that I was... like, responsible for my own wellbeing.”(Sian) Such examples repeatedly highlighted that the need to be self-sufficient rarely provided the young people with ‘ideal’ solutions to reduce future distress, and frequently resulted in cognitive dissonance. The individual from the quote above believed she was of value and entitled to receive good care but did not perceive she was receiving this. To resolve this, she ensured she gained the care she needed by ‘kicking off’ to get those around her to listen and respond to her requests. Though this succeeded, further cognitive dissonance arose from her 39 perceiving herself as a ‘bad person’ that conflicted with her belief that she deserved good care. This reinforced her need to justify herself, both whilst in care and afterwards to manage this dissonance. The need to continue to justify herself in the present highlighted the on-going fear of being perceived negatively, that had started in her childhood and not yet been resolved. However, though some behavioural responses proved effective at influencing individuals and circumstances, ineffective behavioural responses engendered a sense of hopelessness and led to an alternative approach of self-protection from the perceived inevitable future difficulties. An exception to this was one young person, who became self-protective before her first move, following failed attempts to engage foster carers in fighting for her to be allowed to stay in the placement. Self-protection Young people with greater perceptions of hopelessness and self-blame for the difficulties with moves engaged in more self-protective strategies. This was also reinforced by previous failed attempts to influence placement moves. The intention of these behaviours was to minimise distress, most frequently around relationships and loss. This included denial at the loss of relationships as well as reluctance to engage in new relationships. 40 “I didn’t wanna…make new friends ‘cause I already had friends over here. So I didn’t really wanna go out and make a new circle of friends and end up losing them again...” (Lisa) Although these self-protective responses were perceived to be somewhat effective at reducing future hurt, the young people acknowledged that they often brought with them isolation and depression. This further reduced opportunities to obtain support, leading to further hopelessness and reinforcing the need to engage in self-protective behaviours. “I become really depressed. I didn’t want to go out. I didn’t talk to anyone.” (Lucy) The dissonance caused by these negative outcomes was resolved by some of the young people justifying that these experiences made them emotionally stronger. “It’s made me stronger.…. I don’t think anyone else realizes how strong it actually does make young people.” (Jake) 41 Acceptance of acting to survive as best they could was common throughout the narratives, though disruptive and aggressive behaviours were often discussed with a sense of embarrassment. However an exception to this was one young person who expressed ongoing regret at engaging in self-protective approaches: She felt that had she ‘acted to influence’ those in charge of her care that she would have avoided the depression and consequent educational failures that she attributed to taking a self-protective approach. This perspective existed despite an acknowledgement of the hopelessness that had led her to take a ‘selfprotective’ approach. “I definitely think … I don't want to sound like I wanted to cause trouble… I should have fought my corner more. But yeah, I just never said: I just felt so on my own like no-one would listen to me at all.” (Sian) An acknowledged positive outcome of self-protective behaviours for all the young people, was the avoidance of shame often associated with more active and disruptive strategies, stereotyped to be common amongst young people in care. This reinforced their use in future care moves. Future Focus 42 Another approach utilised by the young people was one of focussing on life beyond care; hoping that life would improve after care. The young people identified their time in care as a discrete chapter in their lives, enabling them to be hopeful about the future; a time beyond the trials and tragedies of care. Reflections after care maintained this perception of care as a discrete chapter that impacted on their achievements and successes, and continued to limit their lives. “[Social workers] did whatever they [wanted]…I just went along with it and then as soon as I was independent…I was like ‘…It’s time to get all the things I’ve wanted to do.’ So it was quite a relief...”(Rich) This focus on the future enabled individuals to separate themselves from the behaviours they had engaged with in care and give the care system responsibility for this, rather than perceive themselves as fundamentally ‘bad’. However narratives contained repeated emphasis on this separation, highlighting an on-going fear of being perceived negatively as a child in care. “I'm not violent now or nothing like, I’ve left it behind” (Amy) 43 Despite the acknowledged difficulties and distresses with placement moves, the young people all perceived themselves as survivors, though acknowledging some of their on-going difficulties as a result of these experiences. “It’s in the past and I’ve tried to keep it that way. To be reminded of it.. It makes me feel …quite low about myself. .. I mean I’ve made myself the person I am today… It’s made me stronger... I don’t think anyone else realizes how strong it actually does make young people.” (Jake) What was clear across the narratives was a sense of survival and relief at leaving care, or hopeful anticipation of it for those still within the care system. However, what was unclear was the extent to which negative perceptions about themselves were concealed, in a wish to distance themselves from the ‘bad’ image of a child in care. Within the overall theme of ‘survival’, the majority of the young people engaged in both active and self-protective strategies, rather than just one or the other. Switching most commonly occurred from active to self-protective strategies, as actions to influence failed or hope around better experiences and support disappeared. However, alongside acknowledgement of the insufficiency of these approaches to prevent future distress, the young people highlighted the double bind they were caught in: act to improve their situation 44 and risk being stigmatised further, or focus on protecting the self and risk isolation and depression. In summary, the results of this study indicate a cycle of appraisal and response in relation to placement moves, to reduce distress. Placement move experiences were appraised and informed consequent responses, which then influenced placement moves and led to further evaluations to inform future responses. Sometimes losses were averted and support was provided through these responses, but often responses came with drawbacks as well as successes. The need for self-sufficiency indicated in the appraisals led to responses that often furthered isolation and reduced opportunities for support that could have relieved distress. Consequently, further distress from multiple placement moves led to a sense of hopelessness and the young people ‘survived’ this by focussing on their future beyond care, as they recognised their needs but were unable to get them met sufficiently. Discussion This study aimed to explore the process and impact of placement moves from the perspective of young people in foster care to develop a preliminary foundation of understanding regarding the psychological processes and behaviour related to these perspectives. It was 45 intended that this work would function as an early platform for other researchers to further develop a coherent and theoretically-based account of these phenomena, to inform effective and evidence-based interventions. The themes to emerge from the data highlight the attempts of young people to understand and survive these experiences in order to manage their distress and minimise the development of future trauma. In their reflections, they were mindful of the impact of these experiences on their lives, with the consequential benefits of acquired resilience as well as the considerable difficulties. The landscape was filled with multiple losses, with a backdrop of lack of information, uncertainty, and lack of support. The themes of understanding and survival encapsulate the young people’s process to manage their experiences. Their ability to recognise their own and others’ contribution to the systemic problem of the moves was articulated in multiple layers focussing on economic, emotional and social values. This discussion will explore how the results of this study fit with theoretical models and previous research with young people in care, highlighting areas that require further exploration or indicating areas for intervention within care services. Theoretical models focussed on will include trauma, learning, attachment and resilience. These models have been included due to their relevance to the results. Other theories have been rejected due to a lack of congruence with the results. For example, psychoanalytical developmental models have 46 not been included here due to the lack of evidence in the results to substantiate such congruence or divergence of the findings of this study with such a theory7. Resolving Trauma The narratives in this study clearly fit with the criteria for Developmental Trauma (Van der Kolk, 2005) (Figure 2), drawing into sharp focus that instead of providing nurturance, care system moves played a role in maintaining and exacerbating early trauma that young people often bring with them into care (Takayama, Wolfe & Coulter, 1998). Secure bases, necessary for the development of attachments and sustaining relationships, were lacking. Such experiences explain the young people’s need to ‘understand’ and ‘survive’ placement moves and indicate a need for trauma-based support. Figure 2: Developmental Trauma 7 Appendix N examples a search based on the result 47 Advocation of trauma-based support for young people in care is not a new idea (Hyde & Kammerer, 2009; Thomas & Philpot, 2009; Webb, 2006; Cairns, 2002). For example, Hyde & Kammerer advocated trauma-based support for young people after finding that young people brought behaviours resulting from their trauma to placements, that carers were then unable to manage, resulting in further breakdowns. The results of this study advocate the implementation of such support, given the findings that placement moves themselves were traumatic and resulted in behaviours to reduce associated distress that then risked increasing further trauma from additional moves. 48 Fight, Flight or Freeze The results also highlight the stress response of the young people. Though their attempts to ‘survive’ placement moves were attributed mainly to appraisals, the narratives indicated the stress response present in these experiences. For example, the ‘flight’ of the young man to his father’s house at the threat of being moved. Though this later came to be a strategy of influence for him, it indicated the trigger of the stress response during moves. Younger children are less able to engage in ‘fight’ and ‘flight’ strategies and therefore more frequently engage in ‘freeze’ (Lowenthal, 2012). In addition to having a reduced sense of entitlement to care, this may shed further light on why children who entered care at a younger age engaged with professionals in charge of moves by actions such as becoming emotionally unavailable (freeze). Such inferences are supported by previous propositions about foster children’s responses to psychological stress (America Academy of Paediatrics (AAP), 2000). Understanding New Worlds The narratives highlight that placement moves were often a learning process for the young people: they did not know what to expect from the moves initially and consequently often had to piece together information and experience themselves to make sense of them. Their 49 conclusions then informed their responses (particularly to manage distress). Often, the stress responses undertaken in the immediate moment were also retained in the considered responses: aggression and running away (fight and flight) were associated with ‘action to influence’ whereas withdrawal (freeze) encapsulated self-protective strategies. Stress impacts the ability to learn (Thompson, 2014) and it may be that this chronic and ongoing stress limited the effective appraisal and response decisions for the young people in regard to their placement moves. However the responses often increased the distress of the young person. For example they withdrew from relationships to avoid further loss but also missed out on support. These unsuccessful actions to reduce distress can be understood in the context of a learning process: children try to make sense of tasks and experiences (identify the meaning of them) and errors occur when social contexts are unfamiliar as the young person has no framework or scaffold to inform their actions (Donaldson, 1984). The lack of framework for placement moves highlighted by the young people in this study emphasises the ‘disembedded’ thinking of the young people and helps to understand their difficulties in comprehending and responding to their experiences in a way that benefitted them. Errors due to a lack of framework highlight the dependency on adults around them to help them navigate unfamiliar situations successfully by providing information from a ‘child perspective’ (Donaldson, 1984) in order for young people to appraise situations correctly and 50 respond constructively. Local and national approaches support this principle of including young people in decisions about their care (Wright, Turner, Clay & Mills, 2006.), but it is employed to a limited extent according to this study’s participants. The on-going lack of support and the reactive nature of the placement moves prevented preparation or meaningful discussion. Consequently dysfunctional scripts were not corrected and young people continued to respond in ways that compromised their future needs being met. Attachment and Emotion Regulation The participants described a lack of support and consistency of relationships, alongside distress from loss, feelings of abandonment and associated vulnerability. These are consistent with the emotional responses expected when an individual has experienced insecure or disorganised attachments (Prior & Glaser, 2006). Such attachments are prevalent in young people who have been fostered, due to trauma in pre-care attachments and/or during care (Wekerle & Wolfe, 1998; McCarthy & Taylor, 1999; Crittenden & DiLaila, 1988). However the youth in this study highlighted how their experiences of relationships around placement moves (rejection, lies, absence of support when needed) influenced their ability to engage in relationships with those caring for them. Lack of trust and repeated loss of relationships led to distrust and perceptions of ‘carer’ relationships as harmful and to be protected against, which 51 were characteristics of young people in care also found by Unrau et. al. (2008). This suggests a process by which insecure or disorganised attachments may be exacerbated by care events. Developed by Bowlby (1951), attachment theory proposes that the relationship between infant and mother forms the basis of a sense of security for the infant, informs emotional development and future relationships. Further work distinguished privation (no attachments formed) from deprivation (loss or damage to attachments) (Rutter, 1981; Hodges & Tizzard, 1989). The need to self-protect against relationships, thereby reducing opportunities for attachment relationships to develop highlights how foster care can increase this risk of privation. Moderate difficulties with affect regulation during the interviews also drew attention to attachment difficulties (Fahlberg, 1991), given that emotional regulation develops through secure attachment relationships. For many of the young people, their emotions appeared disorganised, unregulated, absent or masked. Strategies to manage these difficulties included suppression, minimising or dismissing their experience of emotions, changing the topic and actively stating this was in the past. Emotional regulation is adaptive for helping young people achieve their goals and fostering a positive sense of self, a characteristic largely absent in the young people in this study, despite efforts to conceal difficulties in the present time. 52 Though the need for positive, stable relationships in order to facilitate secure attachment in foster care is acknowledged in theory, research and practise, implementation appears to remain problematic (Luke & Coyne, 2008; Samuels, 2009). Given the development of secure attachments will not only significantly reduce distress and instability in care (through preventing harmful self-protective or active strategies) but also improve multiple outcomes beyond care, associated with secure attachment relationships (Prior & Glaser, 2006), this needs to be a priority. Internal representations Rather than their placement moves being conceptualised as the result of specific externally driven factors, the young people related them to internal global attributions which impacted on the development of their self-identity. In light of the loss of relationships and role models early in life it is unsurprising that many of the young people had poorly-attuned internal working models (IWMs), demonstrated by the way they viewed themselves and others. Bowlby (1973) explained that mental representations of the parent-child relationship, built through interactions with primary caregivers contain expectations of caregiver behaviour in the future, such as availability or responsiveness. They also contain a ‘complementary and mutually confirming view of the self as worthy or unworthy of care’ (Bowlby, 1973 p, 238). 53 Clear negative examples of this came from participants in this study: unavailability or rejection from carers and professionals and consequent insecurity and lack of trust led to selfreliance for care needs and a negative sense of self, occasionally partially negated by positive experiences of care and support. Kools (1997) and the AAP (2000) emphasised the negative impact of foster care on the development of young people’s identity, resulting from poor treatment from professionals around them. Resilience Within this study the focus on survivorship of the traumatic experiences of placement moves indicated resilience. Newman and Blackburn (2002, p3) describe resilient children as ‘equipped to resist stress and adversity, cope with change and uncertainty, and to recover more successfully from traumatic events or episodes’. Originally described by Rutter in 1990, resilience is a dynamic process of managing experiences of adversity and learning how to adapt and accommodate to these life challenges to achieve effective adult adjustment (Luther, Cicchetti & Becker, 2000). Resilience in young people in foster care has gained great attention in recent years, given the ability of some young people to achieve favourable outcomes despite the traumatic experiences of their lives prior to and within foster care 54 (Cameron, 2007; Pryce & Samuels, 2010; Wilson, Sinclair, Taylor, Pithouse & Sellick, 2004). In this study, survival often took the form of self-reliance, as the young people took it upon themselves to address their emotional needs by reducing distress from moves. This finding was also ascertained by Samuels and Pryce (2008) who explored reflections of young people ageing out of foster care. As in this study, they also identified that whilst self-reliance could be a source of resilience it could also interfere in accessing and developing supportive relationships associated with positive outcomes in adulthood. The young people who retained optimism highlighted problem-solving skills, maintained a sense of autonomy over their situation and engaged in more hopeful responses to improve their situation. These are three of four attributes of resilient children highlighted in research by Bernard (1995) and ascertained to be factors related to positive outcomes in young people in foster care (Edmond, Auslander, Elze & Bowland, 2006; Hass & Graydon, 2009). However within this research, optimism often appeared to be influenced by sense of autonomy and positive sense of self, highlighting an area to be explored further. A focus on the future was one method of survival identified by the young people, alongside attempts to reduce distress. AAP (2000) highlighted that adults cope with impermanence by 55 building self-reliance and focussing and preparing for a time of greater constancy, an approach that appeared to be adopted by many of the young people in this study and facilitated their resilience. A distinction between life in care and after care enabled young people to separate themselves from previous ‘bad’ behaviour and retain (or develop) a positive sense of self. The majority of the young people interviewed reported improvements in mood, behaviour, relationships and achievements after care, despite the intense difficulties experienced previously. It is unclear what facilitated this resilience. However, all of the young people reported one key positive source of support (usually not linked to care services) before, during or after care. One main positive source of support was identified as a key factor in resilience for children by Newman & Blackburn (2002) and supported in consequent research, highlighting the significance of this. A few young people who entered care expressed some entitlement to having their needs met. This appeared to be associated with greater resilience to care move events as they internalised less responsibility for difficulties with care moves. Such findings suggest that these individuals entered care with an (at least partly) positive IWM (though the majority did not). These were the young people that entered care later, having had time to develop these (AAP, 2000). This indicates that positive IWMs may potentially be a source of resilience for young people in care and highlights the consequent need to facilitate these. 56 Resiliency theory highlights the important of having some autonomy or knowledge over the breadth of changes occurring, in order to be able to prevent these experiences hindering young people’s development (Newman & Blackburn, 2002). Young people themselves were aware of specific elements of support that could have reduced the trauma of moves. For example, social and contextual factors such as having a trusted consistent relationship with an adult, knowing what to expect, being able to prepare for moves and having some control to ensure the decisions taken met their needs. Therefore this study supports other work which challenges the exclusion of young people from decisions and information, not just to reduce distress but to aid development of resilience. In addition, the alignment of the young people’s identification of their needs with theory about resilience adds weight to the argument to listen and respond to young people individually: they can be accurate experts of their needs. Areas for intervention Despite the tentative nature of the findings of this research, through the triangulation of the findings with theory the need to address support for trauma, attachment, resilience and emotional regulation in placement moves has been indicated. However, much of this fits with the work being undertaken by those such as Golding (2004; 2007; 2008; Golding & Hughes, 57 2004.) and Hughes (2004; 2006; 2007, 2009.) to develop approaches to address trauma and attachments in young people in care. Further evidence for the effectiveness of these approaches is still required to justify broader implementation. Limitations This study aimed to explore young people’s experiences of UK foster placement moves. The applicability of this research is limited by the restricted variation in the participants’ ethnic, cultural and social backgrounds. These are important elements given that in the UK, African Caribbean and mixed African Caribbean and white heritage children are overrepresented in foster care (Thoburn, 2007) and locations such as Scotland have care systems run by their own government. A greater number of participants may have led to a greater depth and breadth of information around the process of managing placement moves. It may also have facilitated fuller saturation of the data, which has consequently rendered the results a preliminary understanding of placement moves. However, participants’ experiences of multiple moves at least partially compensated for the small number of participants. Furthermore, though general guidelines suggest numbers of 20-30 participants for Grounded theory studies (Charmaz, 58 2006; Creswell, 1998) justification of these numbers is lacking. Guest, Bunce and Johnson (2006) found that they had generated 34 of their 36 codes within the first six interviews of their study, with all 36 generated by interview 12. This also suggests the small number of participants may not be as significantly limiting as may first appear. The small number of participants also prevented theoretical sampling which may have rendered data more fully saturated with a small number of participants. Conclusion This research has enabled the development of a preliminary understanding of the key elements of placement moves from the ‘child’s perspective’. It has highlighted the process used to inform young people’s attempts to survive the often-traumatic experiences of placement moves: past experiences inform future expectations and consequent responses to minimise future distress. Frequently a lack of support compounds the trauma of placement moves and results in a need to be emotionally self-sufficient. However, these self-sufficient responses often have a negative impact on distress and stability. It is acknowledged that these results require further exploration to obtain further understanding of factors that influence resilience and distress from placement moves. 59 However, the findings of this study support current attempts to develop and evaluate interventions to: facilitate consistent and positive relationships; aid the young people in development of resilience; and manage and reduce trauma in foster care. Despite the largely negative descriptions of care moves, this study has highlighted the pervasively resilient nature of young people in care. But this must not be an excuse to overlook the shortcomings of care. The present research has also found that (at least some) young people are able and willing to report on difficult experiences from foster care. This emphasises that there is no excuse to neglect a ‘child’s perspective’ in research or in practice. 60 References American Academy of Paediatrics. (2000). Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption and Dependent Care. Paediatrics, 106, 1145-1150. Barber, J., & Delfabbro, P. (2003). Children in foster care. New York: Routledge. Bernard, B. (1995). Fostering Resilience in Children. San Francisco: Far West Laboratory for Educational Research and Development. Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. World Health Organization Monograph, 3, 355-534. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol. II: Separation. New York: Basic Books. The British Psychological Society. (2010). Code of Human Research Ethics. Leicester: Author. Burger, J. (1999). The foot-in-the-door compliance procedure: A multiple-process analysis and review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 303-325 Butler, S. & Charles, M. (1999). 'The past, the present, but never the future': Thematic representations of fostering disruption. Child & Family Social Work, 4, 9-19. 61 Cairns, K. (2002). Attachment, trauma and resilience: Therapeutic caring for children. London: British Association for Adoption and Fostering. Cameron, C. (2007). ‘Education and Self-Reliance among Care Leavers’. Adoption & Fostering, 31, 39-39. Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory. London: Sage. Cresswell, J. (1998). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions. London: Sage. Crittenden, P. (1985). Maltreated infants: Vulnerability and resilience, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 85-96. Crittenden, P. & DiLalla, D. (1988). Compulsive compliance: The development of an inhibitory coping strategy in infancy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 585-599. Dey, I. (1999). Grounding Grounded Theory: Guidelines for Qualitative Enquiry. London: Academic Press. Donaldson, M. (1984). Children's Minds. London: Fontana. Edmond, T., Auslander, W., Elze, D. & Bowland, S. (2006). Signs of resilience in sexually abused adolsecent girls in the foster care system. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 15, 1-28. 62 Fahlberg, V. (1991). A child'’s journey through placement. Indianapolis: Perspectives Press. Ford, T., Vostanis, P., Meltzer, H. & Goodman, R. (2007). Psychiatric disorder among British children looked after by local authorities: comparison with children living in private households. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190(4), 319-325. Gilbertson, R. & Barber, J. (2002). Obstacles to involving children and young people in foster care research. Child and Family Social Work, 7, 253–258. Glaser, B. & Strauss, A. (1967). The Discovery of Grounded Theory. New York: Aldine Publishing Company; Golding, K. (2004). Fostering Attachments in Looked after Children: Further Insight into the Group-Based Programme for Foster Carers. Adoption & Fostering, 32, 64-76. Golding, K. (2007). Developing group-based training parent training for foster and adoptive parents. Adoption & Fostering, 31(3), 39-48. Golding K. S. (2008). Nurturing attachments. Supporting children who are fostered or adopted. Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Golding, K. and Picken, W. (2004). Group work for foster carers caring for children with complex problems. Adoption & Fostering, 28, 25-37. Greig, A., Taylor, J. & MacKay, T. (2007). Doing Research With Children. London: Sage. 63 Guest, G., Bunce, A. & Johnson, L. (2006). How many interviews are enough: An experiment with data saturation and variability. Fields Methods, 18, 59-82. Hass, M. & Graydon, K. (2000). Sources of resiliency amongst successful foster youth. Children and Youth Services Review, 31. 457-463. Health Care Professions Council. (2012). Guidance on conduct and ethics for students. London: Author. Hodges, J. & Tizard, B. (1989) Social and family relationships of ex-institutional adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 77-97. Hughes, D. (2004). An attachment-based treatment of maltreated children & young people. Attachment & Human Development, 6, 263 –278. Hughes, D. (2006). (2nd ed.) Building the Bonds of Attachment: Awakening Love in Deeply Troubled Children, Maryland: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers Inc. Hughes, D. (2007). Attachment: Focussed family therapy. Norton: New York. Hughes, D. (2009). Attachment-Focused Parenting: Effective Strategies to Care for Children. New York: Norton. Hyde, J. & Kammerer, N. (2009). Adolescents' Perspectives on Placement Moves and Congregate Settings: Complex and Cumulative Instabilities in Out-of-Home Care. Children and Youth Services Review 31, 265-273. 64 Khoo, E. & Skoog, V. (2013). The road to placement breakdown: Foster parents’ experiences of the events surrounding the unexpected ending of a child’s placement in their care. Qualitative Social Work, 13, 90-104. Koh, E., Rolock, N., Cross, T. & Eblen-Manning, J. (2014). What explains instability in foster care: Comparison of a matched sample of children with stable and unstable placements. Children and Youth Services Review, 37, 36-46. Kools, S. (1997). Adolescent Identity Development in Foster Care. Family Relations, 46, 263-271. Lowenthal, B. (2012). Child Maltreatment: Effects on development and learning. Child Maltreatment, 17, 365-371. Luke, N. & Coyne, S. (2008). Fostering self-esteem: exploring adult recollections on the influence of foster parents. Child & Family Social Work, 12, 402-410. Luthar, S., Cicchetti, D. & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562. McCarthy, G., & Taylor, A. (1999). Avoidant/Ambivalent attachment style as a mediator between abusive childhood experiences and adult relationships. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 465–477. Minty, B. (1999). Annotation: Outcomes in Long-term Foster Family Care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 991-999. Newman, T. & Blackburn, S. (2002). Transitions in the Lives of Children and Young People; Resilience Factors. Scotland: Scottish Executive Education Department. 65 Newton, R., Litrownik, A., & Landsverk, J. (2000). Children and youth in foster care: Disentangling the relationship between problem behaviours and number of placements. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24, 1363-1374. Nutt, L. (2006). The lives of foster carers: Private sacrifices, public restrictions. London: Routledge. Prior, V. & Glaser, D. (2006). Understanding attachment and attachment disorders: Theory, evidence and practice. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Pryce, J. & Samuels, G. (2010). Renewal and Risk: The duel experience of motherhood and ageing out of the child welfare system. Journal of Adolescent Research, 25, 205-230. Rock, S., Michelson, D., Thomson, S. & Day, C. (2013). Understanding foster placement instability for Looked After Children : A systematic review and narrative synthesis of quantitative and qualitative evidence. British Journal of Social Work, 43, 1-27. Rostill-Brookes, H., Larkin, M., Toms, A. & Churchman, C. (2011). A shared experience of fragmentation: Making sense of foster placement breakdown. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 103-127. Rubin, D., Alessandrini, E., Feudtner, C., Mandell, D., Localio, R. & Hadley, T. (2004). Placement Stability and Mental Health Costs for Children in Foster Care. Paediatrics, 113, 1336-1341. Rubin, D., O’Reilly, A., Luan, X., & Localio, A. (2007). The impact of placement stability on behavioural well being for children in foster care. Paediatrics, 119, 336-344. 66 Rutter, M. (1981). Stress, coping and development: some issues and some questions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22: 323–356. Rutter M. Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. In (eds): Rolf, J., Masten, A., Cicchetti, D., Nuechterlein, K., & Weintraub, S. (1990). Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology. New York: Cambridge. Samuels, G. (2009). Ambiguous loss of home: The experience of familial (im)permanence among young adults with foster care backgrounds. Children and Youth Services Review, 31, 1229-39. Samuels, G. & Pryce, J. (2008). What doesn't kill you makes you stronger: Survivalist selfreliance as resilience and risk among young adults ageing out of foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 30, 1198-1210. Sellick, C. (2006). ‘United Kingdom’, in Colton, M. & Williams, M. (eds), Global Perspectives in Foster Family Care. Lyme Regis: Russell House. Sommer, D., Pramling, S. & Hundeide, K. (2010). Child Perspectives and Children’s Perspectives in Theory and Practice. London: Springer. Stott, T. (2011). Placement instability and risky behaviours of youth ageing out of foster care. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 29, 61-83. Strauss, A. & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures and techniques. California: Sage. 67 Strijker, J., Knorth, E., & Knot-Dickscheit, J. (2008). Placement history of foster children: A study of placement history and outcomes in long-term family foster care. Child Welfare, 87, 107–124. Takayama, J., Wolfe, E. & Coulter, K. (1998). Relationships between reason for placement and medical findings among children in foster care. Pediatrics, 101, 201–207. Tapsfield, R., & Collier, F. (2005). The cost of foster care: investing in our children's future. London: British Agencies for Adoption and Fostering/ Fostering Network. Thoburn, J. (2007) Globalisation and child welfare: Some lessons from a cross-national study of children in out-of-home care. Norwich, UEA Social Work Monographs. Thomas, M. & Philpot, T. (2009). Fostering a child's recovery: Family placement for traumatized children. London: Jessica Kingsley. Thompson, R. (2014). Stress and child development. Future Child, 24, 41-59. Unrau, Y. (2007). Research on placement moves: Seeking the perspective of foster children. Children and Youth Services Review, 29, 122-137. Unrau, Y., Seita, J. & Putney, K. (2008). Former foster youth remember multiple placement moves: A journey of loss and hope. Children and Youth Services Review, 30, 1256-1266. Van der Kolk, B. (2005). Developmental Trauma Disorder: Toward a rational diagnosis for children with complex trauma histories. Psychiatric Annuals, 35, 401-408. 68 Webb, N. (2006). The impact of trauma on youth and families in the child welfare system. In: Webb, N. (Ed.), Working with traumatized youth in child welfare (pp. 13-26). New York: Guilford Press. Wekerle, C., & Wolfe, D. (1998). The role of child maltreatment and attachment style in adolescent relationship violence. Developmental Psychopathology, 10, 571−586. Willig, C. (2008). Introducing qualitative research in psychology. England: Open University Press. Wilson, K., Sinclair, I. & Gibbs, I. (2000). The trouble with foster care: The impact of stressful events on foster carers. British Journal of Social Work, 30. 191-209. Wilson, K., Sinclair, I., Taylor, C., Pithouse, A. & Sellick, C. (2004) Fostering success: An exploration of the research literature in foster care. Social Care Institute for Excellence. Wright, P., Turner, C., Clay, D. & Mills, H. (2006). The participation of children and young people in developing social care. Practice guide 6. Yardley, L. (2000). Dilemmas in qualitative health research. Psychology and Health, 15, 215–228. 69 Appendix A: Ethics Approval 70 71 Appendix B: Debrief Statement 72 Thank you for your help with this study. Here is a debrief sheet that has my contact details if you have any further questions that you would like answered about the study or decide you would like a copy of the report that I produce when I have completed the study. I would also like to call you a in a few days to ensure that you are ok from having discussed these difficult topics today. May I ask how you are feeling after the topics we discussed today...? What support or what do you think may be helpful for you after today…? Thank you again for your help with this study. 73 Appendix C: Debrief sheets 74 Debrief Sheet: Young Person A study of the experience and impact of foster placement breakdowns: the young people in foster care’s perspective Debrief This study has investigated how individuals who experienced breakdowns in foster care placements viewed these situations and their views on what support they felt helped or didn't help during these experiences. Placement breakdowns are often difficult experiences. Research in the past has asked foster carers and social workers about the experience but not young people in foster care and young people. This was sometimes because they did not want to upset young people by talking about these sad or difficult changes in foster care. This study aimed to ask your views in order to help services understand how to support you and other young people in foster care better during these times. We are very grateful for your help with this study and being willing to talk about possible difficult memories. If taking part in this research has made you feel upset you may wish to talk to someone further. You could contact one of the following organisations for support by telephone or online via email. Childline 0800 1111 http://www.childline.org.uk/Pages/Home.aspx The Samaritans 08457 90 90 90 jo@samaritans.org 75 Support Line 0208 554 9004 info@supportline.org.uk Alternatively, if you feel very upset you should speak to your foster carer or social worker who may take you to your GP to get some more support. This website also has information for young people on mental health and feeling well emotionally: http://www.youngminds.org.uk/ 76 Debrief Sheet: Young Person (18+) A study of the experience and impact of foster placement breakdowns: the young people in foster care’s perspective Debrief This study has investigated how individuals who experienced breakdowns in foster care placements viewed these situations and their views on what support they felt helped or didn't help during these experiences. Placement breakdowns are often difficult experiences. Research in the past has asked foster carers and social workers about the experience but not young people in foster care and young people. This was sometimes because they did not want to upset young people by talking about these sad or difficult changes in foster care. This study aimed to ask your views in order to help services understand how to support you and other young people in foster care better during these times. We are very grateful for your help with this study and being willing to talk about possible difficult memories. If taking part in this research has made you feel upset you may wish to talk to someone further. You could contact one of the following organisations for support by telephone or online via email. The Samaritans 08457 90 90 90 jo@samaritans.org Support Line 0208 554 9004 info@supportline.org.uk 77 Alternatively, if you feel very upset you may wish to speak to your GP for some further support. This website also has information for young adults on mental health and feeling well emotionally: http://www.mind.org.uk/ If you have any other questions about this study you can contact me or my university supervisor. Our details our listed below. 78 Appendix D: Participant Demographics 79 Table 1: Participant Demographic Information Participant Age Gender Ethnicity No. of foster placements (not residential) No. of placements after (moved into) 12 years of age Still in Care 1 20 Male White British Multiple 2 No 2 19 Male White British 2 2 No 3 21 Male White British 9 6 No 4 20 Female White British 2 2 No 5 19 Female RomanyGypsy 2 1 No 6 17 Female White British 3 3 Yes 7 17 Female White British 1 1 No 80 Appendix E: Initial Interview Schedule 81 Initial Interview Schedule 1) I wonder if you could tell about one of your placements where there was an unplanned ending? (about what happened when it ended?) 2) Could you tell me about what happened next? (In terms of going to your next placement)? 3) Can you tell me what it was like when your placement was ending/ ended? These were broad initial prompt questions that were intended to get the interview started, build rapport and lead to a focus on the experience of the placement breakdown and consequences /impact/views on this. Further questions followed the young person’s lead in relation to these issues, feelings and experiences they highlight in relation to the placement breakdown from their perspective in line with Willig’s (2008) Abbreviated Grounded Theory Approach. Interview were then transcribed and analysed, identifying key elements and components to explore further that then impacted the schedule of future interviews as they were prioritised and explored further. This process was in line with Willig’s Abbreviate Grounded Theory approach (2008). 82 Appendix F: Later Interview Schedule 83 Later Interview Schedule 1. I wonder if you could tell me about one of your placements where there was an unplanned ending? (about what happened when it ended?) 2. Could you tell me about what happened next? (In terms of going to your next placement? 3. Can you tell me what it was like when your placement was ending/ ended? 4. What did you know about what was going to happen when you moved? How did you find this out? 5. Why do you think the move process happened like this / this way? How did you understand what happened 6. How did you react/respond to what happened? Why? 7. Can you tell me about your other placement moves when the placement had an unplanned ending? 8. (If any,) what was the impact of these experiences? 9. What did you expect in future placement moves? Why do you think you expected this? 10. Can you tell me what was important to you when you moved placements and why? 11. How do you think placement moves should be done? / What do you think should be done differently? NB: These questions were often not asked in an ordered and structured way; sometimes the answers came up in discussions about the process and were offered without being directly asked. Sometimes questions were repeated within an interview in relation to different placement moves experiences. 84 Appendix G: Example of analysis producing interview schedule changes 85 Example of transcript analysis: Participant: They'd just say, ‘oh you're going to a new place’. Interviewer: And did you get much warning? Limited explanation No, they'd just show up Participant: No, they'd just show up. Interviewer: Oh okay, and then you'd go with them immediately? Participant: Yeah. Pretty much. I was living out of black Go immediately Living out of black bin bags No hope in permanency No stability No security Always ready in case Fear of losing things Yeah. Pretty much (go immediately) I was living out of black bin bags It got to point where I literally didn't even bother taking my clothes out of the bin bags or anything like that. ‘cause I knew that I'd be… I could go at any time so was just a case of keep my stuff in there, where I can just grab it bin bags. It got to point where I literally didn't even bother taking my clothes out of the bin bags or anything like that. ‘cause I knew that I'd be… I could go at any time and so was just a case of keep my stuff in there, where I can just grab it. Interviewer: Okay, so you just started living as if they were going to turn up any time? Participant: Yeah Confidence knocked Constantly settling and moving Impact on now – difficulty settling Used to play on mind it's knocked my confidence because, I’d settle down somewhere then you get moved again. You settle down again, you get moved again, it just is like a vicious circle It’s why it's like now, why I find it hard to settle in it’s not always in the back of my mind now but it was a few years ago like, I know I'm going to move again soon Interviewer: And how do you think that kind of affected you, kind of thing? Participant: Well, it's knocked my confidence. Just because, I’d settle down somewhere and then you get moved again. You settle down again, you get moved again, it just is like a vicious circle. It’s why it's like now, why I find it hard to settle in, cos it’s, it’s not always in the back of my mind now, but it was a few years ago, it was in the back of my mind like, I know I'm going to move again soon........ 86 Associated memo – written after initial coding: ***** seems to be really impacted by this constant moving and lack of preparation for doing so. I wonder if others also experienced this lack of warning in moving and the effect it had on them – how did they respond, does it still affect them as it does *****. For ***** it seemed that he took on looking after himself in relation to being prepared to move, a kind of independence and I wonder if others responded the same or differently. There is a great protectiveness over his items that he also mentioned earlier in the interview and this process triggered risks of losing his items as well as reinforced beliefs that he would be moved without warning repeatedly. There is a sense that he has to look after himself and his belongings, given this lack of information (warning) about moving. I realised I did not ask if he felt not unpacking his belongings had any effect on the placement he moved to and I wonder if I felt nervous to ask too many questions about this particularly emotional and distressing part of the account for him. I wonder if others responded in this practical way of not unpacking their items if they perceived they would always move. Did they perceive they would always move and anticipate placements would not work out as ***** appears to? Outcome: The reflections from after the interview, the coding and memos helped identify further question to add to the interviews. In this example, the questions added included: • What did you know about what was going to happen when you moved? How did you find this out? - and how did you respond to this? / did this effect you ? – how? • What did you expect in future placement moves? Why do you think you expected this? • Can you tell me what was important to you when you moved placements and why? 87 Appendix H: Participant Information Sheet 88 Participant information sheet (Young person 16+) The experience and impact of foster placement breakdowns: The young person’s perspective A research project Please will you help with my research? This sheet gives some information about the project to help you decide if you would like to participate in this study. Who am I? My name is Ruth Hunter and I am training to become a Clinical Psychologist. I am undertaking a study and would like to invite you to participate in this. This information sheet is designed to help you understand what the project is about and what would be involved in participating in this. What is the research? 89 My research is looking into collecting the views of individuals who have been in foster care and experienced placement breakdowns during this time. Why is the research being done? Placement breakdowns in foster care are distressing times for young people but research into how individuals would like to be supported during these times has often focussed on only asking adults around the young person their views rather than asking the young people themselves. This has led to people not knowing exactly how it feels for young people to experience a placement breakdown and the impact it has on them. We also do not know young people’s views on what could help make it better. This research aims to begin to fill in these gaps in information in order to help guide professionals to better support young people during placement breakdowns. What will my participation involve? I am looking to meet with individuals to ask them some questions about their last placement breakdown; what this experience was like for them and what the impact of it has been; how young people reacted and responded to this event. I would like to meet to ask you these questions but can ask these over a telephone conversation if this is easier, or over email. I will only need to do this once and it will not take more than one hour. You will be offered a £15 iTunes voucher for participating in the study to thank you for giving up your time to meet with me. Who will be in the project? I am hoping to meet with individuals between 12 and 21 years of age who are or have been in foster care and experienced a placement breakdown after the age of 12. What will happen with my information? I will record the interview I undertake with you to help me remember what we discussed and then transcribe this. Your information will be anonymised before I analyse what you say and when I write this up. It will be put with data from other young people to help us understand young people’s experiences. My supervisor, a transcriber and I will be the only people that see all of your data. Some of other work colleagues may see other sections of the transcribed interview, just to help check that I 90 have collected all that I can from the information. They will only see completely anonymised parts of your interview. Your information will be treated confidentially by all of these people. How do I take part or let you know if I do not want to? If you would still like to take part, I would be very grateful if you could email me to inform me of this (r.hunter@surrey.ac.uk) and I will arrange to meet with you, call you or email you to answer any further questions you may have. If you are under 16 years I will need your Guardian’s consent for you to participate. I will ask you to sign a form that you are willing to take part in the study. If you agree to take part I will then arrange to meet with you again to ask you about your experiences. If you are interested then I can contact you again at the end of the study to inform you of what I have found from my research. Could there be any problems for me if I take part? Placement breakdowns can be unpleasant and / or difficult experiences. It is possible that thinking and talking about these experiences may be upsetting. If you feel upset when we are talking about them then you are welcome to stop and have a break and then continue or you can stop all together. I will give everyone written information of people and organisations that are available if you feel there are feelings or experiences you would like support in thinking more about. Your information will be treated confidentially but if you share information that you or someone else is at risk of harm then I will need to share this information with an appropriate individual, but I will discuss this with you first if I need to do this. How will doing the research help? When your answers are put together with all the other participants I hope it will tell us something about young people's views of foster placement breakdowns and how they would like support during such times. I will write a summary of these findings to help those who work with young people to understand what I have found out. Your privacy will be respected and your data will be used anonymously: No one will know that you have taken part aside from me and my supervisor (and if you are 16 or younger, your Guardian). 91 Appendix I: Consent form 92 Consent to participate in research: Young Person Researcher: Ruth Hunter, Trainee Clinical Psychologist Supervisor: Mary John, Registered Clinical Psychologist, HCPC registered Project: What is the experience and impact of foster placement breakdowns from the perspective of the young person? I agree to take part in this study and to be audio recorded in the interview. (If undertaking a telephone or face-to-face interview). I agree for my data (what I say and talk about) to be used in this study and understand that my data will be treated confidentially and stored safely. I understand that my help in this study is my choice and I have the right to withdraw from the study within two weeks of undertaking the interview. I understand that after this it will not be possible to withdraw my information. I understand that the results of this study may be published at a later stage but information that may identify me will not be included. I have read and understand the statements above and agree to take part in this study 93 Signature:……………………………………… Date:…………………… Appendix J: Description of Analysis - Willig’s (2009) Abbreviated Grounded Theory Approach 94 Coding Coding aims to break down the data into component parts. Initial codes were given first, to identify important words or groups of words in the data. Both categories and In vivo codes were labelled using the language of the young people to aim to reduce the risk of the 'child's perspective' becoming a 'child perspective' (Soderback, Coyne & Harder, 2011). Intermediate coding followed later (though the concurrent nature of data collection and analysis means the researcher moves between the initial and intermediate coding stages). This involved connecting sub-categories but also linking categories, exploring the range of properties and dimensions of these. Memos Memos are written records of the thinking of a researcher during the process of undertaking a grounded theory approach that aim to help with data comparison, guide data gathering and enable the exploration of relationships between categories. Short, explicit memos were used here as well as the exploration of more detailed memos encompassing reflections and questions for further exploration. They helped guide further exploration and data collection as well as began to synthesise the comparisons and categories into a more cohesive understanding of the data, moving towards developing a framework. An example of this memo is listed in Appendix J. Theoretical sampling Theoretical sampling helped give focus and direction to the constant comparative actions and analysis of the data by the researcher. It helped identify when more information is needed for categories to become saturated. The Memos were important in helping direct the decisions as to where to obtain this information. Theoretical Saturation and Theory generation Due to limited participants and resources, full theoretical saturation was not achieved and therefore only a developing framework was produced from the data. Visual diagrams were produced of the findings, highlighting the complex relationships between the concepts and categories that emerged through data analysis. 95 Participant validation The findings were presented to participants to obtain their feedback and ensure that they perceived that the model represented their experiences. Following feedback, further analysis occurred and impacted the developing theory. Credibility Analytic credibility in Grounded Theory is improved by the emergent and interactive approach to coding leads to analysis that is influenced but not wholly determined by the researcher. Comparative analysis by the researcher aimed to help ensure the usefulness and robustness of the codes. Theoretical Credibility is dependent on analytical credibility. In itself it is improved by the systematic use of theoretical sampling to explore and develop understanding of properties of tentative categories. Allowing participants to choose the stories they shared before focussing in on more specific lines of questioning by the researcher also helped to ensure neglect of information and a focus on specific information by the researcher (Dey, 1999). Though in this case, it was not possible to return to participants for multiple interviews, systematic follow-up of elements highlighted in previous data analysis aimed to improve theoretical credibility. Yardley (2000) proposed four principles to help improve credibility further in the study. These included the researcher ensuring sensitivity to the context by staying close to the data and reviewing literature to (when appropriate) build theoretically upon the work of previous researchers. Supervision throughout data analysis alongside seeking guidance on the analysis process aimed to improve the competence of the researcher. Within this, supervision ensured and enhanced the transparency of the process, as well as monitored the fit of the theory with the method, key for credibility of the study. The beginnings of a coherent framework of the subject area highlight presence of the fourth key element from Yardley (2000) of impact and importance, important in a credible study. 96 Appendix K: Example of Transcript and Coding 97 Focussed Coding / Intermediate Coding Line by Line coding / Initial coding Transcript Interviewer: Ok. And when you were going to move, kind of, between the two placements… How did that go? What was it like? What happened? Moving placements was horrible Sister chose to leave placement without them Bond with brother was important Couldn’t stand being moved away from brother Bad relationship with foster carer impacted on access to see brother it was horrible my sister, she left that family when she was sixteen to move in with her mates’ mum who fostered her me and my brother had that bond and moving away from him was horrible. I couldn’t stand it. . I was depressed as soon as I moved to [town2]. ]. I weren’t allowed to go to his, to go see him ‘cause I’d fell out with my foster carers it all just happened so quick. Too much to handle in my … in a few days Participant: Oh, it was horrible. It was actually horrible because obviously … my sister, she left that family when she was sixteen to move in with her mates’ mum who fostered her and me and my brother had that bond and moving away from him was horrible. Absolutely horrible. I couldn’t stand it. I was depressed as soon as I moved to [town2]. I weren’t allowed to go to his, to go see him ‘cause I’d fell out with my foster carers. And it all just happened so quick. Too much to handle in my … in a few days. Move happened too quickly too handle Interviewer: And how ... and when 98 did you find out the placement was Found out moving through being told they’d packed her bags They didn’t. They told me they’d packed my bags. ending, relative to when you were then moved? Participant: They didn’t. They told me they’d packed my bags. Moved on the day she found out On the day No warning they’d obviously been planning it for a few weeks they just didn’t tell me. No warning to being moved Interviewer: Ok. So it was on the day? Participant: Mmhhmm Interviewer: There was no warning? Placement move planning was kept a secret from her Stuff was packed without her knowing - secrets Yeah, while I was away for the weekend. Participant: Mmmhhmm. So they’d obviously been planning it for a few weeks and they just didn’t tell me. So I’d come back to find all my stuff packed and then later that day I was being moved to [town]. Interviewer: Ok. And they’d packed your stuff… and things? Participant: Yeah, while I was away Moved same day found out moving No preparation for the weekend. Foster carers told They couldn’t handle my behaviour anymore. cause I was a pretty angry child. Interviewer: Ok. Participant: So I’d come back to find all my stuff packed and then later that day I was being moved to In a way I was glad 99 Told by foster carer they couldn’t handle her behaviour anymore ‘cause the way like I found they treated me obviously unhappy as I counted them as my mum and dad. they was there for everything, helped me like grow up and all that lot I’m back in contact with them now. We still talk occasionally. [town]. Interviewer: And then what did they say to you... was it the foster carers that told you or your social worker or…? Participant: Foster carers. They couldn’t handle my behaviour anymore. Erm…. Yeah and all that, ‘cause I was a pretty angry child. Interviewer: Ok. And how did you Mixed feelings about moving feel …. Participant: Erm… I don’t know Glad to leave because of way treated really. I, like, …. In a way I was glad ‘cause the way like I found they treated me but obviously unhappy as Lost what she viewed as her mum and dad who had been helping her grow up Now back in contact with foster carer Not wanted at foster obviously they didn’t want me staying there anymore I had to get hold of my social worker and the fostering agencies to find me a new place they done pretty much straight away. obviously from 18 I had to sort my own place out but from I counted them as my mum and dad. Like they was there for everything, helped me like grow up and all that lot…… I’m back in contact with them now. We still talk occasionally. Interviewer: Ok. And who moved you? (Rattling in the background). 100 carer home anymore Had to contact SS herself to sort where she was going. going from erm, one foster family to the other, obviously social services had to be involved, , the fostering agency had to be involved I couldn’t say anything about it. Sorry about that noise. Participant: That’s alright. Erm, well obviously they didn’t want me staying there anymore so obviously I had to get hold of my social worker and the fostering agencies to find me SS acted immediately to find a new place a new place… which they done pretty much straight away.… obviously from 18 I had to sort my Had to sort own place from 18 own place out but from going from erm, one foster family to the other, So that was all done behind my back. Next thing I know I’m moving out. SS responsible for her until 18 and had no choice about this Being moved was a secret and a surprise Foster carers talked about (planned) her leaving before they told her. obviously social services had to be involved, the fostering agency had to be involved and I couldn’t say anything about it. Deceit – ‘behind my back’ Interviewer: No? Participant: So that was all done They didn’t even tell me really. They just said ‘you’re going. We can’t have you no more’. behind my back. Next thing I know I’m moving out. Interviewer: Ok, so they’d already joined up and talked about it? Decisions made about her without her 101 Participant: Mmmhhmm. Interviewer:..by the time the foster No detailed explanation for move given. carers told you? Participant: Yeah. Interviewer: Ok. Ok. . My foster carer. Took me They helped me unpack my stuff from the car, gave me a cuddle and then left. Foster carer took to new placement Participant: They didn’t even tell me really. They just said ‘you’re going. We can’t have you no more’. Interviewer: Ok Participant: And then they took me Foster carer helped her to move and then left – (practical support only) It was horrible. . I was crying … felt like all my life was gone between my feet Moving to some house that I didn’t really know. Moving to [town], an area that I didn’t know. to [town2]. Interviewer: And who took you? Participant: My foster carer. They helped me unpack my stuff from the car, gave me a cuddle and then left. Interviewer: And how.. how was Upset about moving that, having them … Felt had lost everything ‘all my life was gone’ Participant: It was horrible. Absolutely horrible. I was crying … everything. I just felt like all my life 102 All new situation at new foster carer’s was gone between my feet. Moving to some house that I didn’t really know. Moving to [town], an area that I didn’t know. 103 Appendix L: Memo Examples 104 Theoretical Memo: Second level – Acceptance of others’ views Rich reports the fact that he is told he is ‘unfosterable’ without question. He reported no emotional response to being told this, no attempt to argue or disagree with social workers appearing to already believe this perspective himself. This was in contrast to his opposition in being moved to particular placements or the boundaries and instructions from foster carers, to which he reports he ‘acted out’. From the way Rich reports being told he is unfosterable it could be surmised that he was aware of this perspective long before it was verbalised to him. He report’s himself as ‘angry’ and ‘bad’ when he was in care and there appears no expectation that others should want him this way. It appears the information the social workers are sharing about the fact that he is unfosterable fits with how Rich views himself as a boy with characteristics that make him not wanted by others. There is a distinct lack of positive attributions of himself in Rich’s account, highlighting only his ability to be independent which is a strength in light of the rejection from adults in a position to offer care to him. Rich does not challenge the view of the social workers, nor does he expect them to search further to find a placement for him. There is a finality and a pervasive hopelessness of the situation that he is an unwanted child because he is naughty, something he also believes. He has no sense of entitlement to be wanted, again a mirror of the apparent attitude of the social workers in giving up on seeking a placement for him. It is unclear with whom this narrative started, whether it emerged from Rich, the social workers or others who cared for him. But it appears that there is now a cycle of this belief between Rich and his social workers. Reflections: In reflecting on this situation for Rich I felt a strong reaction of sadness, both in him being told such cruel information but also that this appeared to be true; that no foster parents would take him in and care for him. I found myself considering what message this must be giving Rich about his worth and his value. I feel some anger at the way this information was given by social workers; in an apparently blunt and consequently almost cruel way without clear consideration or acknowledgement of how this news may impact Rich. Consequently I was aware of feeling quite protective of Rich. His lack of emotional response struck me. I found myself considering how this apparent ‘mature response’ in accepting this piece of information could well be in contrast to the true undercurrent of feelings and whether the sense of hopelessness openly reported in other elements of his narrative was contributing to the lack of emotional expression spoken of here. There was a brief acknowledgement that maybe it was not appropriate for social workers to have shared this information with him in the way they did, but this was quickly followed by a focus on his maturity. I wondered whether previous descriptions of himself as ‘bad’ meant he felt he was responsible for events and could not disagree with social workers’ decisions to tell him what he perceived was the truth. And furthermore, a truth he felt responsible for. 105 106 Appendix M: Example of a finding evidenced across interviews 107 Evidence: Why the need for self-sufficiency response - Lack of support -could not rely on social worker All my Social Workers have been incredibly hard to get hold of. Obviously I do appreciate that they have a huge workload but when you need someone then and now, it is important that you can get hold of them. (Sian) They actually abused my Children’s rights. Because they made decisions without asking me… my opinion first. They did lots of things wrong. I really hated her. She was incompetent. Like she’d come along and say ‘We need to do this, this, this and this and these things’ and then wouldn’t do them herself. (Rich) ‘Cause they try and suit you with the best placement but some Social Workers don’t know their young people well enough to be able to choose that. So they end up putting them in the wrong placement and they just self-destruct. (Lisa) I'm sure Social Workers get all the time 'I want to be moved. I don't like these people' and then it's just like one thing, like they were allowed, I don't know, a bar of chocolate or something. So I think it's, like, understandable that they were, erm, a bit 'We don't want to move them' 'cause it's a lot of hassle. But at the same time, because I'd said it so many times they never listened to me. (Sian) I never ever thought as if they were like seriously considering it. They just said, 'We'll see how it goes in a few weeks.' And I was just at such a low point. (Adam) Like, Ok their Social Workers supposed to…. but my Social Worker's supposed to be on my side. I didn't want it to be sides but like, they were supposed to be like helping me and erm...and I always felt as if it was like reasoning with [Foster carer] and never … it was never like what I wanted to happen. (Rich) I felt really hurt and tricked…cheated, if you know what I mean? I thought ‘Yeah this is it. I’m gonna stay here for a while.’ But no. I felt cheated and that. (Lucy) I think it was because it was just too much effort. It always felt like they were just like 'Oh another problem.' They probably have like so many kids to deal (Jake) I just felt so on my own like no-one would listen to me at all. (Lisa) I would love my choice to be increased but until I hit eighteen everyone's always trying to tell me what to do because in their eyes I'm still a child, whether I have enough maturity to take responsibility for myself or not. So whether I wanted my choice to be heard... sometimes they make decisions that were completely wrong for me, like completely wrong for who I am. Like, not what I would be happy doing but obviously they put me into a box; I'm a child and this is best for children so we're going to do this and it's not (Sian) I was just so angry that my welfare… you know, like a child’s welfare comes second to money. I think that’s disgusting. Like it’s somebody’s life, it’s not just somebody’s job. Like, it might be a job to them, like having to move me and you know, paying for it, but it’s my life, rather than something that they can make decisions about, like half-heartedly. And like, they can cut corners and save money and that’s what’s important rather than my wellbeing. (Adam) Like, it’s my wellbeing but they don’t care about that. All they cared about money. (Jake) 108 Because my... I met my Social … my new Social Worker on the day that I left. I didn't really know her. Like, I couldn't... Like, she was trying to comfort me because I was upset and I felt a bit... I felt a bit like she was intruding... Like, I felt like, 'This is not for you. This is something that is very private for me.' (Lucy) I didn’t feel like I had anybody there for me, like, to talk to about it.I mean I couldn’t talk to my Social Worker; They was always busy with their other cases. (Amy) They don't really listen to you. You're still treated as a child and like, you're not given responsibility for yourself and that you're not... you're not trusted to … to make responsible decisions for yourself. So... they kind of do what they want and put you wherever you are and you can't really control.... (Sian) ‘Cause if I had a choice I don’t think I would have gone there. But you don’t really get a choice in where you end up. That’s Social Services that. (Amy) It's constantly criticising yourself. And like, in Social Services, the children are always being criticised. They always have to have goals to achieve. Like, they have plans to make themselves better. (Rich) They actually told me that that’s why they were trying to move me; because they couldn’t afford it and that they thought it would be better for me to be up near lots of people I knew and my family. But it wasn’t actually at the time. (Rich) I felt safe there and I was getting better and Social Services just pushed me and pushed me to go somewhere that I didn’t want to go. (Lisa) 109 Appendix N: Example of search for triangulation of results 110 Database: Psychinfo Search terms: Foster* AND Surviv* AND Child* OR Young Pe* OR adolescen* OR teen* OR 2005-2015. Peer-reviewed. Results: 111 Relevant results: 34 Key elements Resilience, Adversity, Thriving, trauma-related beliefs, highlighted in Self-reliance results: Future, Psychodynamic adjustment (ego) Destruction (as a means of survival) Reactive attachment disorder, 111 MRP Proposal Impact of disrupted placements on the well-being of children who are accommodated By Ruth Joanne Hunter School of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences University of Surrey August 2012 112 Introduction Background and theoretical rationale In March 2011, just under 60,000 children were in foster care in the UK (British Association of Adoption and Fostering, 2011) and three times more likely to experience mental health difficulties than the general youth and child population. Despite these statistics, research into interventions and support for these fostered individuals is largely based on US populations with limited validation of applicability to individuals in the UK. In 1997, Berridge identified only 13 UK foster care based studies. Although this has increased recently, understanding from studies centres only around how to support foster carers, what effective foster care is and the benefits of 'joined-up' service delivery in supporting foster children. (Sellick, 2006). UK (but also international) research has frequently ignored the experiences and views of fostered children themselves, focusing largely on the perspectives of foster carers and other adults involved in their care and support. This lack of information from the young people themselves is not only unethical when determining what care they received but also risks providing care and resources that are less effective or at worst ineffective at meeting their needs and wishes. (DOH, 2009). 113 On top of the increased risk of mental health difficulties for young people within foster care, breakdown of placements creates further risk for development of emotional, behavioural and mental health problems, (Rubin et al, 2004) highlighting an important area for understanding and appropriate support. The Children's Act (1989) highlights a duty of professionals to facilitate contact between children in care and their biological parents, but it is an area with on-going debates as to the costs and benefits of this. Leathers (2003) identified that some children experienced conflicts of loyalty between biological and foster families and this could lead to problems of adjustment. Brown & Bednar's (2006) found higher conflict loyalty was associated with greater emotional and behavioural problems, difficulties that have been wellestablished as positively associated with greater risk of placement breakdown. It seems clear that the views of foster children in how they manage these relationships is important and currently missing (Leathers, 2003), not just in successful placements but the views of those who have experienced placement breakdowns and are consequently at greater risk of emotional and behavioural and mental health problems. In summary, we lack knowledge and understanding of how young people themselves experience placement breakdowns in the UK and manage their relationships with their biological families during these difficult transitions and experiences and consequently what support they would appreciate at this time. Understanding of resilience and needs must go further than registering percentages of children without problems and measuring behaviour or asking only the opinions of parents and professionals to asking the young people themselves. (Singer et al, 2004). If not, working in the best interests of the child, as encouraged by current 114 policy risks riding on incomplete information and assumptions of what they want and need. (Sempek & Woody, 2010). This study aims to begin to explore this area in order to build an understanding that could help inform professionals involved in supporting young people during this time in the most effective way. Research Question What are the psychological and social impacts of disrupted placements on the well-being of children who are accommodated? Method Participants Between fifteen and twenty participants will be recruited in order to gain enough information to produce a meaningful understanding of the topic in question. 115 Inclusion Criteria 1. 21-30 years: To help ensure they are sufficiently distanced from the experiences of disrupted placements that they are less distressed reflecting on their experiences but recent enough to be able to remember their experiences well. 2. Have been in foster care for at least one year* 3. One placement breakdown minimum* 4. Contact with biological parents for at least one year on entering foster care 5. Grew up in the UK *. These experiences will have been occurring at or after the age of 7, though individuals may have entered foster care earlier. The reasoning for this age cut-off is due to children’s information processing capacity and reliance on verbal recollection in data collection. Piaget (1983) proposed that prior to seven years children have less organised logical thought, are more egocentric and have fewer abstract thinking abilities which will impact on recall of these memories. Cordon et al (2004) highlighted experiences prior to language development will be difficult to recall linguistically. 116 Exclusion criteria Individuals who were in solely kin placements as Messing (2006) proposed that they do not feel the trauma associated with being removed from one's family. Individuals with specific difficulties such as Learning Disabilities or developmental conditions that may make participation in the study difficult due to potential impairments in intellectual disability and communication. Individuals currently seeking help from mental health services or with a diagnosed mental health difficulty due to the possible negative impact of the subject on them, although there is awareness that some individuals may have mental health difficulties that have not been formally diagnosed and / or treated. Feasibility of recruitment It is anticipated that adults who have experienced challenging experiences as children will want to share their experiences for a variety of motivations. Some may want to share their thoughts for altruistic reasons to ensure that other young people have the benefits of services more able to respond effectively whilst others may wish to share their frustrations. Recruitment Strategy 117 Participants will be recruited through advertisements in the British Association for Adoption and Fostering, local newspapers (in the Guildford area) and advertisements on University Supervisors’ website (Ms Mary John, Programme Director PsychD Clinical Psychology). A PhD forum that advertises studies needing participants will also be used alongside using the University of Surrey’s research scheme that advertises studies to students (targeting mature students or those 21-30 years). A brief piece of information about the study will be given and participants will be asked to contact via email, phone or text if interested in participating. More detailed information will then be sent and a meeting will be arranged answer any questions and ensure the participant meets the inclusion criteria. Following this, written consent will be discussed and obtained if the individual wishes to participate in the study, ensuring this is a minimum of 24 hours after receiving the detailed information sheet about the study. If consent is given, a meeting to undertake data collection and the interview will be arranged. Design and proposed data analysis 118 Given the lack of research in this area specifically with individuals who have experienced placement breakdowns and disruptions, a theory of how this is viewed and managed by individuals is lacking. Therefore this study will be a qualitative interview study using a Grounded theory approach in line with Charmaz (2006) in order to enable new theory to emerge in an area that has little current understanding and no current theory specific to this context. The focus on process and change within the Grounded Theory approach acknowledges that what is studied is a dynamic and socially constructed version of events and experiences which sits well in studying the concept of 'impact' in relation to placement breakdowns. Consequently, conclusions and findings will be acknowledged as an interpretation of the concepts explored rather than a 'true' portrayal. IPA is not being used as it has a narrower focus than Grounded Theory, looking at experiences for individuals rather than enabling a wider understanding to be developed. (Willig, 2008). An abbreviated version of Grounded Theory will be used due to time and resource constraints and a likely limited number of participants. If possible however, some initial analysis will be undertaken between interviews to look for emerging categories to help direct further interviews more effectively. (Willig, 2008). 119 It is hoped that further research may be able to explore the theory that will be developed through this study and its application to clinical practise to support children experiencing placement breakdowns and disruptions in foster or adoptive care. A narrative approach will be used in the event that few participants are able to be recruited for the study. Measures Advertisements will be designed with the University of Surrey Co-ordinator of Service User and Carer Involvement. Information sheet. (See Appendix A*). Consent form* Demographics. The following will be collected through brief questions: current age, gender, ethnicity, age at entry to foster care, reason for leaving biological family, number and length of placements, number of disrupted placements / unsuccessful placements and when these were, details of contact with biological parents during care, presence of biological siblings in placements, current contact with biological family The Kinetic Family Drawing. Developed in 1970 by Burns and Kaufman, this test 120 requires the participant to draw a picture of his or her entire family. This picture is meant to elicit the individuals’ attitudes toward his or her family and the overall family dynamics. Introduction to Data collection. (See Appendix B*). Debrief sheet. (See Appendix C*) Audio recording equipment to record the interviews. *These items will all be amended following consultation with service users who have been in foster care previously but may not meet all criteria for the study, arranged in conjunction with the Co-ordinator of Service User and Carer involvement. Procedure The participants will be interviewed alone. It will be undertaken preferably in a room that is quiet and with limited distractions. Interviews will be audio recorded for the purposes of transcription and in line with Grounded Theory are likely to reduce in length as the study progresses, starting at approximately 50 minutes. Telephone interviews will also be offered if preferred or more convenient for the participant. 121 Data collection: 1. Introductions, including reminding the participants of the purpose of the study as well as what to expect from the interview. Confidentiality will also be discussed and the participants will be given the opportunity to ask any questions. They will be reminded that they can stop the interview at any time or not answer questions that they do not wish to. This session will not be recorded to avoid identifiable information being recorded. 2. Demographic data will be collected through short fixed questions. This will not be audio recorded to help protect confidentiality. 3. The Kinetic Family Drawing. Once drawn, the drawing is then discussed with the individual to help ascertain why they have drawn the picture in the way they have. In this scenario the drawing will be extended to include those involved in the individual's childhood – both biological and foster / adoptive family. Administration of this test will also not be audio recorded to avoid recording identifiable information but paper notes will be taken regarding the details of the discussions. Confidentiality will be protected by the allocation of a letter to the participant and only first names will be recorded on the drawing. Alongside eliciting information about the individuals view of those involved in their childhood it will provide a platform from which to develop 122 engagement and rapport and help the participant feel at ease prior to undertaking the interview. 4. Semi-structured interview: In line with abbreviated Grounded Theory, the interview will start with explorative general questions about the research topic and the interview schedule may change as the participants raise topics that may appear valuable to pursue with later participants. This part of the study will be recorded and transcribed in adherence to University policy for the recording, transportation and storage of audio recordings. 5. Debrief / Ending A debrief sheet will be given and the participant will be given the opportunity to ask any questions they have about the study in general or the interview and task they have just completed. 6. Follow-up Participants will be asked if they would like to see and give feedback on the data collected. A follow-up call will also be offered to ensure participants' questions have been answered and they are directed to services for further support if required. 123 Ethical Considerations Application will be made to the Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences Ethics Committee as this study involves recruiting individuals who are potentially vulnerable and discussion of a sensitive and potentially distressing subject. Below is a list of potential ethical considerations in relation to this study: 1. Discussing relationships and potentially distressing experiences of separation and placement breakdowns from the past is a topic to be handled sensitively and empathetically to ensure distress is minimised. Highlighting their right to opt out of answering questions or withdraw their consent at any time aims to help protect the participants from feeling obliged to discuss subjects or share information they do not wish to. Discussion prior to consent will highlight the subject topic and support the participants in identifying whether it is a subject they feel able to discuss without too much distress. The debrief discussion and information sheet and follow-up call aims to help provide a space for the participant to reduce distress if this occurs during the interview. The interview will also take place in a setting that is private and free from disturbances to help reassure the participant that their privacy and feelings discussing potentially difficult subjects are being respected. 2. Confidentiality will be discussed in the first meeting and at the start of the interview. If disclosures emerge through the interview, it will be ascertained whether these have 124 been addressed in the past and advise of the option to discuss the matter with the police. It will also be explored whether any other children may still be at risk and Safeguarding procedures will be followed if appropriate. 3. Disseminating the research is important in ensuring the participants are not subjected to discussing private and potentially distressing issues without purpose. The research aims to help inform support of young people experiencing placement breakdowns rather. . Opportunities to share the findings of the research and publish the work will therefore be sought. 4. Individuals may feel obliged to participate due to the demand characteristics of helping others which may lead to a sense of social compliance, an awareness of the lack of children’s voices in this area and wanting to help other foster children be better supported. They may not want however, to discuss such difficult topics. Discussing the study in detail before and giving time to consent will aim to try and combat this and ensure the participants are as informed as possible before agreeing to do study. Researcher concerns The researcher is aware of lone working policies and a risk assessment will be undertaken with the supervisor. The University lone working policy will be followed with the supervisor being informed of the venues and meeting time commencement and ending. 125 R&D Considerations Not required. Service User and Carer Consultation / Involvement Consultation will be sought from the Co-ordinator of Service User and Carer Involvement at the University of Surrey over the design of the adverts that will be placed on the internet and in the newspapers to ensure appropriate wording and explanation. Further consultation will be sought with two service users as to the questions that will be used in the interview to ensure they are being asked in the most appropriate, sensitive and respectful manner. They will be amended as appropriate following this. Feasibility Issues 126 1. Difficulties in recruiting participants may arise from the reluctance to discuss such a private and potentially distressing topic or accessing them. Individuals may withdraw participation from the study once the interview has started. Benefits of participating in the study will be shared, such as helping inform professionals from the child's perspective of how to more effectively support children experiencing placement breakdowns in the UK will be highlighted. 2. It has been found that foster children often keep their status private to avoid stigma and feelings of difference (Finkelstein et al, 2002) and this may continue into adulthood and lead to a reluctance to express an interest in the study. For those that do express an interest, reassurance will be given in relation to confidentiality and a nonjudgemental stance of the interview, as well as the value of their opinions. 3. Despite the multiple methods, it may be difficult to recruit the number of participants required. If this occurs, narrative evaluation will be employed rather than Grounded Theory to analyse the data collected. 4. Some individuals may find it difficult to remember some of the demographic data required if they had numerous placements from an early age. 127 5. Participants may find it difficult to talk openly about their feelings about both their foster and biological families. Individuals may feel loyal to those that cared for them and struggle to report more negative aspects of their thoughts and feelings about their experiences. Reassurance about the value of all their views and opinions will be given and loyalty issues will be taken into consideration. Dissemination strategy Publication will be sought alongside finding other appropriate arenas such as conferences to share the findings and help potentially inform clinical practise and encourage further exploration of this area. Proposed Study Timeline Ethics Submission: 10/12 Data collection start: 01/13 128 complete: Data analysis start: complete: 08/13 08/13 10/13 Complete draft: Results / Discussion : 12/13 Submit complete draft: 02/14 129 References Berridge, D. (1997). Foster care: A research review. London: HMSO. British Association of Adoption and Fostering. (2011). Retrieved from: http://www.baaf.org.uk/res/stats Brown, J., & Bednar, L. (2006). Foster parent perception of placement breakdowns. Child and. Youth Services Review, 28, 1497–1511. Burns, R. C., & Kaufman, S. H. (1970). Kinetic family drawings (K-F-D): An introduction to understanding children through kinetic drawings. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing Grounded Theory. London: Sage. Cordon, I. M., Pipe, M., Sayfan, L., Melinder, A., & Goodman, G. (2004). Memory for traumatic experiences in early childhood. Developmental Review, 24, 101-132. Department of health. (2000). Children Act 1989. London: HMSO. http://www.legislation.gov.uk Retrieved from: Department of Health. (2009). Promoting the health and wellbeing of looked after children – revised statutory guidance. London. TSO. 130 Finkelstein, M., Wamsley, M., & Miranda, D. (2002). What keeps children in foster care from succeeding in school? Views of early adolescents and the adults in their lives. Vera Institute of Justice. United States. Leathers, S. (2003). Parental visiting, conflicting allegiances, and emotional and behavioural problems among foster children. Family relations, 52, 53-63. Messing, J. (2006). From the Child’s Perspective: A qualitative analysis of kinship care placements. Children and Youth Services Review, 28, 1415-1434. Piaget, J. (1983). "Piaget's Theory". In P. Mussen (Ed.) Handbook of child psychology. Wiley. Rubin, D., Alessandrini, A., Feuddtner, C., Mandell D., Localio, R. & Hadley, T. (2004). Placement stability and mental health costs for children in foster care. Paediatrics, 113, 13361341. Sellick, C. (2006). From famine to feast: A review of the foster care research literature. Children & Society, 20, 67-74. Singer, E., Doornenbel, J. & Okma, K. (2004). Why do children resist or obey their foster parents? The inner logic of children’s behaviour during discipline. Child Welfare league of America, 83, 581-610. Willig, C. (2008). Introducing qualitative research in psychology. Open University Press: England. 131 132 Literature review UK evidence for interventions to improve the psychological wellbeing of foster children: A systematic review Year 1 April 2012 133 Abstract Statistics from the UK highlight that foster children and young people are more than three times more likely than those in the general youth and child population to experience mental health difficulties. (Meltzer et al. 2003, 2004). The majority of interventions to improve foster children's psychological wellbeing have been developed and validated in the US yet some of these are being used in the UK. This systematic literature review looked at the research on the effectiveness of interventions to improve the social, emotional and behavioural wellbeing of foster children in the UK. It was a concern that few papers were found and all had substantial methodological weaknesses that highlighted a large gap for good quality detailed research to be undertaken to ensure professionals are appropriately and effectively supporting this vulnerable group of individuals, directly or indirectly with the interventions they use. Future research needs to address these methodological issues, build on current research, identify interventions for difficulties other than behavioural problems and consider issues of diversity and difference within this vulnerable group. Key words: Foster, children, young people, UK, interventions, review, outcomes 134 Introduction Foster children in the UK Statistics from Department for Education 2011 National Statistics showed that over 48,000 children were in foster care in the UK on March 31st 2011. Children and youth in foster care have higher rates of mental health difficulties than those in the general population. Ford et al (2007) highlighted that the results of a number of studies by Meltzer et al (2000, 2003, 2004a) emphasised that even compared to the most disadvantaged children outside the care system, the mental health of children looked after by local authorities was significantly worse. These studies all involved interviewing carers, teachers and children (between 11-15 years) and using the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Health Related Disorders (ICD-10) to identify a prevalence of a mental health disorder. This was defined by Meltzer et al (2000) as “a clinically recognisable set of symptoms or behaviours associated with considerable distress and substantial interference with personal functions”. Meltzer et al (2003) found that ‘mental disorders’ were present in 40% of the fostered population in England compared to just 13% and 10% for boys and girls between 11 and 15 years respectively and a prevalence of 6% for children between 5-10 years in the general population. (Meltzer et al. 2000). Meltzer et al (2004a, b) found similar prevalence rates for foster children in Scotland and Wales. On top of this, Meltzer et al (2003) highlighted that for looked after children in England, mental health problems (that did not reach criteria for a 135 disorder) was reported for over two fifths of the children included in the study by their carers. A study by Ward et al (2008) found that 70-80% of foster children and young people require specialist emotional and behavioural support. Tapsfield & Collier's (2005) study reported a 37% prevalence of conduct disorder in looked-after children in the UK, which was almost three times higher than that of the general population. Meltzer et al (2004a) identified that after conduct disorders, emotional disorders (specifically anxiety disorders), were the most common mental disorders in looked-after children in Scotland, England and Wales. This was followed by hyperkinetic disorders and depression. Such increased prevalence rates highlight the importance of effective and appropriate psychological support and interventions for foster children. Being healthy, staying safe, enjoying and achieving, making a positive contribution and economic wellbeing were the five outcome aims for children proposed by the government in 2003 (Department for Education and Skills. 2003). If foster children are to be supported to achieve the 5 outcomes then, given their increased risk and vulnerability to mental health difficulties previously detailed then clearly a greater level of mental health support is needed for these individuals. Best practise is to use evidenced based interventions and support when undertaking any type of psychological support, to ensure at worst that no harm is being done, providing the best outcome possible for the intended individuals and that the time and resources of professionals, foster carers and foster children are not being wasted. (National 136 Institute for Clinical Excellence. 2001). Cutbacks and changes in health and social services mean that there are even fewer resources available to promote wellbeing, making it even more essential that the interventions and psychological support being implemented are not only the most effective, but effective for the most needy as well as those with less intense difficulties. Established evidence A basic search of Ebsco, Psychinfo, Medline highlights a wealth of foster care literature and research from the United States (US). Reviews of this research highlight interventions and approaches to mental health care that have the best evidence base so far. Landsverk et al's (2009) work is the most recent published review and they reviewed the evidence for psychosocial interventions for the most prevalent conditions found in foster children in the US. They highlighted that for post-traumatic stress disorder, abuse related trauma, depression, substance abuse and disruptive behaviour disorders, interventions that addressed symptoms, behaviour and functioning had the best (and indeed a 'strong') evidence base. It is beyond the scope of this review to detail all the interventions found by Landsverk et al (2009) to be promising interventions for each of these difficulties. But they found that most interventions were brief and usually more effective when the caregiver was actively involved. Interventions varied between involving multiple professionals, the child and the carer, being community based or clinic based, and of course, the theoretical basis for the approach, highlighting the 137 breadth of approaches that have been found to be effective in the US. UK programmes being implemented Indeed some of these US evidence-based approaches have already begun to be implemented in the UK. McAuley & Davis (2009) identified that ten local authorities have implemented Multi-systemic therapy (MST) programmes for young people aged 11-17 and their families who are on the edge of custody and an additional site piloting MST specifically for children who have experienced abuse or neglect. MST is a US developed intensive home-based family based intervention that acknowledges the role of the family environment on difficulties experienced by the child and the family. Landsverk et al (2009) identified this as an intervention with promise, concluding from critiquing 10 studies into MFT that it was particularly promising for working with foster children to address substance abuse and/or disruptive behaviours. They concluded it was positively associated with a reduction in aggressive behaviours in foster children and fewer placements. McAuley & Davis (2009) also identified 14 local authorities implementing Multi-dimensional Treatment Foster Care (MTFC) programmes for adolescents aged 11-17 years, six programmes for 3-6 year-olds and eight sites for children aged 7-11 years. McAuley & Davis (2009) highlighted initiatives to cascade the programme out to other local authorities. MTFC is a US established programme based on social learning theory that aims to reduce problem behaviours and promote developmentally appropriate and prosocial behaviour through a multifaceted, multiple setting 138 based approach. This includes foster parenting in the home using specific behavioural techniques, skills training and supportive therapy for the foster child, school consultations and family therapy (Moore & Chamberlain. 1994). The other approach identified as being developed when McAuley & Davis (2009) published their research was a cognitivebehavioural programme called the Fostering Changes Programme, which focussed on helping foster carers manage children and was developed by Pallett et al (2002). The Fostering Changes Programme (FCP) was the only intervention to be highlighted by McAuley & Davis (2009) as having been developed and trialled in the UK prior to being rolled out. It is based on 'The Incredible Years' programme (Webster-Stratton, 1998), which was developed in the US, and aimed to promote positive parenting and reduce children's behavioural difficulties and improve their problem solving and social skills. The FCP trial found carers reported improvements in their foster children's behaviour, but results from their other measures were less supportive of this. Through their study, McAuley and Davis (2009) highlighted an apparent drive to pilot interventions in the UK that had been shown to have a strong or emerging evidence-base in the US in relation to effectiveness. They discussed hopes that these pilots would reinforce the value of evidence-based commissioning and gave an acknowledgement of the need to obtain evidence of the effectiveness of these interventions for the UK foster population. No detail however, as to how or why these interventions may or may not be applicable in the UK was addressed in the study: Details that seem fundamental to the predictions of whether these interventions are likely to be effective for the UK foster population. Services in the UK increasingly acknowledge the diversity of the UK population itself and the impact that these differences in culture and background may have on 139 psychological interventions and support. Also, foster children in themselves vary on a multitude of issues: their history, reason for entering care, length of time in care, ethnic background, religion, culture, etc. Identifying and acknowledging some of these differences may provide insight into how and why the effectiveness of interventions with an evidencebase in the US may differ in the UK and guide appropriate adjustments or changes and the appropriateness of implementing or testing these. US and UK foster care In 2007, Thoburn published a study comparing statistics about children in out-of-home care across a number of different countries. She noted a number of similarities and comparisons between the US and the UK in her analysis of the statistics she collated. Thoburn (2007) identified that a fewer percentage of those entering care were 16-17 year olds in the UK than the US, 7% and 20% respectively and the UK had a higher percentage entering at 10-15 years (40% to 25% respectively) but that for those under 12 months the percentages were very similar, (17% and 15%). Thoburn's (2007) data identified that in the USA over 90% of children were taken into out-of-home care as a result of abuse or neglect, whereas this was only 48% for England, with other factors listed as parental disabilities or illness (8%), disability or problems of the child (9%), abandonment (11%) and relationships or other family problems (24%). Wales also had similar percentages to England. Thoburn (2007) also hypothesised that using performance targets to control welfare spending was linked to seeing 140 out-of-home care as something to be avoided; a similarity for the USA and the UK. Differences between the USA and the UK care system were also highlighted in the report through comparing the legal status of children entering care on one particular day: For the USA, over 95% entered into care as a result of a court decision, compared to only 33% in the UK. The USA and UK had similar average lengths of time spent in out-of-home care: 1.8 year and 2.1 years respectively and both countries had a 5% adoption rate. Also, both countries had similar percentages of children placed in un-related foster family care: Between 47% and 57% for the four UK countries and 46% in the USA. As highlighted earlier, Landsverk et al (2009) identified post-traumatic stress disorder, abuse related trauma, depression, substance abuse and disruptive behaviour disorders as the most prevalent mental health conditions of children in foster care in the US. In comparison, Meltzer et al (2003) highlighted conduct disorders as the most common disorder experienced by children in care (not just foster children) in the UK, followed by anxiety disorders and hyperkinetic disorders. Statistics however do not report on the level of impact of the mental health difficulties individually which is also likely to impact what types of support and interventions need to be prioritised by each country. On top of differences in legislation, characteristics and factors related to children entering foster care; in both countries, some ethnic minorities are overrepresented in foster care. In the UK, African Caribbean and mixed African Caribbean and white heritage are overrepresented in foster care with black Africans overrepresented to a lesser extent. (Thoburn. 2007). In the 141 USA, African American and Native American children are overrepresented in out-of-home care statistics, with Hispanic children increasingly so. (Thoburn. 2007). In the UK however, Thoburn (2007) highlight the underrepresentation of those of East and South Asian heritage. Statistics on the percentage of foster carers from ethnic minorities in the UK and the US are difficult to obtain, likely due to the presence of public and private foster-care employment agencies and variations in data governance that create difficulties in obtaining statistics from these. There are a range of other factors such as average number of placements, placements with siblings, payment of foster carers and a number of other factors that may vary between the US and the UK. These could all be issues of diversity and difference that may impact the appropriateness or effectiveness of psychological interventions. These statistics highlight the breadth and depth of similarities and differences between both the foster care systems themselves and the characteristics of the children fostered in these systems and their difficulties. As discussed, there are also likely to be many more. Given these details, it seems complacent to assume that the evidence for effectiveness of interventions used in the USA is automatically relevant to the UK. It highlights the implementation of such evidence-based practises across the UK as a risky and potentially costly trial, in terms of public money and psychosocial impact on the foster children and their families if it is an ineffective intervention. Though US research dominates the literature databases, one would hope that given the extent that programmes such as MST and MTFC have already been implemented in a number of local authorities, that UK based evidence for 142 effectiveness is now guiding practise in the UK more than US evidence. This literature review will now look at what evidence exists for the effectiveness of interventions for foster children in the UK. Method An electronic systematic literature search was undertaken on Psychinfo, Medline, Ebsco, Web of Knowledge and Cochrane databases. It included peer-reviewed papers published from January 2005 to present. The search terms used were “intervention* OR therap*”; “foster* OR looked after OR accommodated OR” and “UK OR United Kingdom OR Brit* OR Welsh OR Wales OR Scot* OR Northern Ireland OR Northern Irish OR Engl*”. Papers were excluded if they did not focus on interventions and outcomes related to children while in foster care (therefore excluding a focus on transition and adult outcomes of foster care). Papers were also excluded if they focussed on policies rather than practise, interventions within education rather than health, or if they focussed specifically on foster children with additional medical health needs such as Learning Disabilities. These exclusion criteria were used as they represent specific areas and groups that are above and beyond the scope of this review. Papers were not excluded based on type of evidence, though consideration as to the quality of each type of evidence is evaluated in the review in line with guidelines from the National Institute of Clinical Excellence (NICE, 2006). Papers that discussed a theoretical approach and used examples such as case studies were included, in order to gain an understanding of potential areas for developments or changes to interventions. 143 The search was performed February 2012 and produced 11 papers that described or evaluated interventions with foster children in the UK. These were divided into categories for evaluation based on who was involved in the intervention; 1) foster parents 2) foster children 3) mixed focus (including combinations of foster parents, foster children, social workers, etc.). The critique of each of the studies was focussed on the methodology, in order to evaluate the strength and validity of the respective results for each, in order to determine the strength of the evidence from each paper, rather than the theoretical approach of the intervention. Results Interventions with Parents of Foster Children Interventions with foster carers are often used as indirect interventions to improve the social, emotional and behavioural outcomes of foster children; most frequently aiming to equip foster carers to manage foster children's difficulties, in order to improve placement stability (a factor that can impact the social, emotional and behavioural outcomes of foster children (Holland, Faulkner & Perez-del-Aguila. 2005)) and help foster children reach their potential. (McDaniel et al. 2011). 144 Two of the papers from the search focussed on CBT based programmes for foster carers. MacDonald et al (2005) looked at the effectiveness of CBT training for parents of foster children with challenging behaviour. They used a randomised controlled trial (RCT) using groups of foster carers. 117 foster carers (all but 2 of whom were white) opted into the study; 67 of these were in the training group and 50 in the control group. Data was collected before and after the training and at 6 months follow-up. Results were that foster-carers in the training group reported increased confidence in ability to deal with behaviour problems and scored higher on knowledge of behavioural principles compared to those in the control group. McDonald et al (2005) queried whether the content and length of the course could have led to disappointing outcomes or a lack of support to help develop the skills learnt. The authors also suggest the limited effectiveness of the CBT training in the results could have been due to using carers of extremely 'difficult' children. They proposed the intervention be refined to deal with the weaknesses identified to ascertain whether in fact the intervention is ineffective or whether this study failed to identify the effectiveness of it due to its limitations. Herbert & Wookey (2007) investigated whether a broadly based CBT programme could indirectly reduce the challenging behaviour of looked-after children and increase placement stability, through improving carers' self-assurance and behaviour management skills. Their study used a randomised controlled study, with 67 foster-carers in the experimental group and 50 in the control group. Those in the experimental group were randomly allocated into six groups, all given the same intervention. Their ages ranged from 32 to 65 and had a mean of 8.68 years of foster experience. The intervention, the Child Wise Programme (CWP) was 145 devised by the authors. It is a programme that aims to be collaborative between facilitators and foster-carers and to empower caregivers by giving them a conceptual framework (CBT and behavioural principles and techniques) that enables them to resolve problems themselves. Herbert & Wookey (2007) reported that CWP has obtained positive results when used with birth parents but had not been validated for use with foster-carers, but this was not referenced in this paper. Results were a significant increase in carers' confidence, a majority of foster carers reporting improvements in children's behaviour (but not to a significant level) and that these were generally attributed to having acquired new behavioural management skills. There was no reduction found in relation to unplanned placement breakdowns where behaviour problems were indicated. Though both these CBT based approaches suggest some promise in supporting foster carers with children with challenging behaviours in the UK, both studies relied only on parent reports of child outcomes and failed to identify detailed demographics of the foster-carers and foster-children involved that could shed light on whom specifically these interventions may work for. Though they used randomised controlled trials, generalisability of the results for both studies is limited by relatively small sample sizes and drop-outs of participants, but also for McDonald et al (2005), by the fact that carers were required to opt into the study. Also, the study that included a six month follow-up (McDonald et al. 2005) found less positive results of the study than Herbert & Wookey (2007), which could suggest a CBT-focussed intervention may have limited long-term effectiveness. Unfortunately a lack of detail about each of the programmes makes it difficult to identify the similarities and differences between 146 the programmes which could be helpful in investigating these approaches further. Two other papers found looked at the effectiveness of the Incredible Years Basic Parent (School Age) Programme (IYP) for improving foster parents ability to manage the complex needs of looked after children by teaching them skills to manage child behaviour and consequently promote placement stability. The IYP is founded on social learning theory and was originally developed for use with biological parents. It's protocol includes detailed session plans and home activities that can be tailored by professionals to the specific needs of families. McDaniel et al (2011) conducted a pilot programme in Northern Ireland, implemented by Barnardo's Professional Fostering Service, using a single-group pre-test and post-test design with 13 foster carers. McDaniel et al (2011) focussed on an age range of 8 and 13, providing some (though limited) information on this. Assessments were carried out before and immediately after the group training and included a self-report parent rating scale to measure the degree of conduct and externalising behaviour management problems are exhibited by children. Results from the study suggested a significant decrease in the intensity of social, emotional and behavioural difficulties reported by foster carers following the IYP, and a smaller but not significant decrease in the extent to which foster carers felt their foster child's behaviour was problematic. Bywater et al (2008) also looked at the effectiveness of IYP. They emphasised the potential of IYP to enhance placement stability and help foster carers understand the needs of lookedafter children. Their study focussed on 46 foster carers of children between 2 and 8 years and 147 employed controlled randomisation to allocate to the intervention (n=29) and control (n=17) groups. Foster carers were approached and requested to participate in the study and received monetary rewards for completing data at baseline and six-month follow-up. The children ranged between 2 and 17 years at the start of the study. Results suggest that the IYP was an effective programme for significantly reducing carer depression and reducing challenging behaviours in the foster children, as rated by the foster carers. Bywater et al (2008) questioned whether their findings had been limited by using a parent-report scale that they felt needed to be more sensitive. They also did not use other measures which reduced the scope of measuring the changes that had occurred. Again, despite promising findings, evidence of the effectiveness of IYP from both these studies is limited by their small sample sizes. Bywater (2008) and McDaniel et al (2011) also relied only on foster-carer reports to identify any impact of IYP on the foster children themselves, and gave very limited information of the foster children and carers, limiting understanding of who exactly the IYP appeared to be effective for. McDaniel et al (2011) also failed to use a control group, which further limits the evidence for whether IYP is a promising intervention. Additionally, a confounding feature in the study was Barnardo's Professional Fostering Service provided ongoing support and training to foster carers outside of the IYP which may have instead resulted, or contributed to the positive changes reported by the carers. Furthermore, McDaniel et al's (2011) study failed to ascertain whether any reports of changes or improvements from the intervention were maintained over time. A strength of these studies was that they used quantitative measures to identify 'significant' improvements. 148 Though both McDaniel et al (2011) and Bywater (2009) also failed to detail many demographics of the foster carers and foster children in their studies that could have aided understanding of for whom the intervention was most effective (backgrounds, placements, etc.). Overall, the study provides promising evidence but yet again another tentative start to identifying the effectiveness of IYP for foster children. Gurney-Smith et al (2010) used a repeated measures design, (pre, post and three month follow-up) design to evaluate the impact of a Fostering Attachments Group (FAG) for a mixed group of foster and adoptive parents. FAG (Golding & Picken. 2004) is based on social learning and attachment theories and aims to develop parenting skills by educating about the influence of the social environment on behaviour, with consideration of the need for responsive and attuned parenting to develop successful attachments. The group comprised of 5 foster carers and 10 adoptive parents and a special guardian; one male and 15 females. Evaluation data was only collected from 13 participants at all three stages. Inclusion criteria involved currently experiencing problem behaviour from a child when the group started and difficulties in attachment relationships for that child. Referrals came from Social workers and were active cases in the Children, Young People and Families Service. Measures used were all parent-report tools. Analysis showed that reported parenting skills and understanding improved to a statistically significant degree which was maintained through to follow-up. A significant decrease in reported child hyperactivity/inattention behaviours was obtained between pre-group and follow-up time points. Post hoc tests also found a significant decrease in overall difficulties of the child reported between pre-group and 3 month follow-up scores. 149 Parenting stress levels were not impacted but children were reported to show significantly less inhibition. Similarly to some of the other studies, no control group was used by Gurney-Smith et al (2010), the sample size was very small, long-term evaluation was limited (only measured at three months after) and measures used only foster-parent reports to identify any positive outcomes for the foster children. The relevance of the findings to foster children is also limited through the study using adoptive parents and a special guardian as well as foster parents as, despite commonalities of background histories, their situations of temporary and permanent placements may well impact the social environment quite differently. Again, as with the other studies, though this study suggests some potential for being effective in working with foster parents, this study fails to identify whether this approach has a truly positive impact for the child. Interventions directly with Foster Children Loxterkamp (2009) aimed to challenge what she described as the ‘predominant conviction’ (p.423) that regular contact of adopted and fostered children with their biological parents is always beneficial. Loxterkamp used three cases studies to illustrate her arguments that contact is not always beneficial (in the short or long-term) and instead can often be the cause of secondary harm. Though this approach highlights well the points argued by Loxterkamp 150 (2009) it does not provide high quality evidence that her hypotheses are valid and that the experiences of those in her case examples are generalisable to those in the wider population. For example, she chose the case studies to include as she felt they were representative of the common attitudes in relation to contact. They were also chosen following therapy, as cases Loxterkamp (2009) that had highlighted issue of biological contact that supported her arguments, bias that cannot be ignored. Consequently, this study begins to identify potential issues of contact but provides poor evidence for these upon which to initiate change in a matter that has well-established beliefs and practises associated with it. In contrast, Sen and Broadhurst's (2011) study was a narrative review of studies into outcomes of foster children as a result of contact with their biological family. However, they gave little detail of the methods of the studies and detailed no quantitative outcomes. They highlighted that evidence for the impact of biological parent contact on children's emotional, behavioural and intellectual outcomes was largely dated and inconsistent with no studies having employed systematic measures to evidence the effect of contact on child outcomes in relation to both UK and International studies. The only two main papers discussed in detail in relation to this were focussed on disruption of attachment formation through contact with biological parents. But again, little detail of the strengths of the study or the conclusions found were presented. Sen & Broadhurst (2011) also looked at the impact of biological parent contact on stability of placements, but failed to link this to identifying psychosocial outcomes for the child and again, the descriptive style provided limited insight into the strength of the evidence found from the studies presented. Sen & Broadhurst (2011) suggest a number of 151 components for future research that would help improve the quality of evidence found, but that these conclusions are valid relies on the reader assuming that these components were not included in the studies discussed by Sen & Broadhurst (2011) based on direct omission of these details from the review rather than detailed critique of the studies, highlighting these omissions. Such suggestions included a need for studies using robust outcomes measures including psychosocial profiling and children's views, sufficiently sized and representative samples of foster children, prospective and longitudinal designs and based on clear definitions of contact. A lack of detail and lack of systematic approach to addressing the literature, alongside the approach used by Loxterkamp (2009) means that evidence for the outcomes of foster children as a result of contact with their biological parents in the UK is currently very unclear. The third study by Wakelyn (2008), looked at the impact of transitional therapy for children in 'short-term' foster care. This work discussed the theories and used one case example to illustrate the approach. It shows a promising consideration of developing and detailing new approaches and ways of working with foster-children that take into account the importance of well evidenced facets of practises such as multi-disciplinary working. It does not however, provide evidence for the effectiveness of this intervention on improving the psychosocial and behavioural outcomes of foster children given the limitations of using a case study, as with Loxterkamp (2009). 152 Interventions with Multiple groups Staines, Farmer & Selwyn (2011) investigated the effectiveness of a Therapeutic Team Parenting approach. A prospective repeated measures design was used, involving children between the ages of 5 and 14 entering new placements organised by one Independent FosterCare Agency. Twenty-three local authorities participated, 450 children were identified and they, their carers and social workers were sent questionnaires at the start of the placement and one year into it. Only eighty placements had data from carers and social workers at both time points which reduced how representative the results are of the placements as a whole. Despite detailing some demographics of the carers, including ethnicity, age and marital status, the sample sizes did not allow reliable detailed analysis of the result in relation to these. The team parenting approach emphasises all team members involved with the child working collaboratively together, assessing the young person in placement rather than in isolation. The therapeutic fostering environment views the placement and activities provided as enabling the fostered individual to learn and develop, based on a systemic approach. Results found high levels of satisfaction from foster-parents for the therapeutic and team parenting approaches and the authors concluded that high-quality placements were provided for many of the challenging children. The authors acknowledged that the approach was not always implemented as efficiently as intended (delays in service provision following assessment, for example). But alongside this, due to the small return rate of questionnaires it is difficult to determine whether this approach positively impacts placement stability or child outcomes or whom it is particularly beneficial to. On top of this, whether this approach could be effective 153 outside the Independent Foster-Care sector could be questioned from debate that Independent Foster Care Agencies often do not provide placements for the most challenging children in the care system. Hibbert & Frankyl (2011) also relied on qualitative analysis and were even more limited by a small sample size in their study into the impact of a psychology consultation service set up to support foster carers and social workers. The approach was a consultation model that aimed to combine clinical expertise and knowledge of psychological interventions for foster children with the experiences and knowledge of social workers and foster carers in order is to help foster carers understand the child’s early experience, the impact of this and then adapt expectations of the child and strategies of working with them accordingly. Social workers (n=7) were randomly selected but foster carers (n=7) opted into the study. They had between 1 and 6 consultation meetings (3.3 and 4.4 average for foster carers and social workers respectively). A semi-structured interview about the consultation service was undertaken and evaluated using thematic analysis. From this analysis, Hibbert & Frankyl (2011) highlighted that all foster carers had reported an increase in psychological understanding of the child and in skills for behaviour management, which in turn foster carers reported had led to improved relationships between carers and their children and more self-confidence from this. Foster carers also reported feeling emotionally supported. Social workers reported feeling supported within the wider professional system, able to obtain different perspectives and reflect on situations, gaining enhanced skills and support with decision making and planning. Unfortunately, again no long-term investigations or child specific outcome measure were 154 used. Given the qualitative nature of the study and the small sample size, the generalisability and reliability of the benefits of this approach appear are also clearly limited, though it suggests a promising potential approach that could be worth investigating further to build on these findings. Golding (2010) investigated a multiagency approach to supporting children in foster care, using an example of an integrated multi-agency service in Worcestershire for Looked-after and adopted children (ISL). The aim is to maximise placement stability by providing mental health and emotional well-being guidance and advice based around a consultation process, supported by additional facets such as network meetings, supporting parenting and providing education and mental health interventions. The author draws upon professional clinical experience and states that 'a range of research and audit-based evaluations has confirmed ISL provides an effective additional level of support for the children, carers and professional network' (p581. Golding, 2010) but does not detail what these are other than detailing that service user views and a pilot evaluation were used. Outcomes are detailed only as foster carers having increased confidence and a changed perception of their children following consultation leaving a vague understanding of the added value of the team. It is also unclear as to the value of this work for the foster children's outcomes specifically, long-term benefits and what components of the approach may be important. Evaluations are needed to identify this, at least to ensure it is not causing harm, if there are other benefits that motivate services to employ such an approach. 155 Discussion Overall, there is currently very little evidence for the effectiveness of any particular intervention to improve the social, emotional and behavioural outcomes for foster children in the UK. Firstly, interventions involving foster-carers appear to have stronger methodologies, using control groups and showing some attempts at identifying long-term outcomes of the approaches used. Results from these studies all appear promising in producing positive outcomes for carers, but unfortunately all child-related outcomes are based on parent reports only, leading to a lack of understanding as to whether these interventions are truly have a positive impact on foster-children. Is it enough to assume positive outcomes for a child if their parents report being more able to cope with difficult behaviour or reduced problem behaviours from the child? Placement stability has been evidenced as having a positive impact on foster children’s social, emotional and behavioural outcomes, but are these studies making assumptions that if foster parents feel more able to cope then the placement will be more stable? Further studies need to take these questions into careful consideration. Demographic details of foster parents and children are also limited in these studies and this therefore limits understanding for whom these interventions may be effective and when. Studies that have tried to consider these have been restrained by their sample sizes. Of the foster-parent based interventions critiqued in this review, all show promise in supporting foster carers and potentially foster children in relation to challenging behaviour particularly, but due to methodological issues it is difficult to identify if any show more promise than 156 others currently. Secondly, interventions that directly involve the foster child are currently lacking in numbers and development: Only Wakelyn (2008) has discussed a specific approach and Loxterkamp (2009) and Sen & Broadhurst (2011) looked at the common practise of contact with biological parents. Methods to evaluate these are also weak, with a lack of controlled trials and use of measures to identify child outcomes. Thirdly, interventions that include a range of individuals including carers, professionals and sometimes fosterchildren themselves seem to be even more difficult to ascertain whether they are having a positive impact on foster children's psychosocial wellbeing: All these studies looked at failed to use control groups and had small sample sizes or relatively high drop-out rates (or incomplete data). Alongside this, they also relied on parent-report of child improvements and changes, rather than multiple perspectives further highlighting difficulties ascertaining whether these approaches truly benefit foster children. The detail of the work undertaken within each study was also lacking, leading to limited understanding about what in the approach may have been beneficial and why. Similarly to the parent-focussed approaches, these studies also had limited demographic data of the foster children and carers involved and the potential impact of issues of difference and diversity on the effectiveness of the interventions. Overall, the approaches that currently appear to have the most established evidence as to their effectiveness in the UK are the foster parent focussed approaches. (McDonald et al (2005); Herbert& Wookey (2007); McDaniel et al (2011); Bywater et al (2008); Gurney-Smith et al (2009)). These however, have much to improve on both in the quality and detail of evidence 157 obtained but also in the fact that they have done more to evidence their positive outcomes for foster carers than foster children themselves and this needs to be addressed as a priority in future research with these approaches. What is also worth highlighting, is that these interventions focus almost solely on issues around challenging behaviours in foster children and though this is the current most common difficulty for foster children in the UK (Tapsfield & Collier. 2005), other difficulties and disorders experienced by foster children need effective and appropriate interventions also. Limitations Limitations of this systematic review are that the inclusion of the search terms around location may have overlooked some papers that have been undertaken in the UK but have not been explicitly listed as such. Despite this, it is important to consider that professionals or researchers interested in using UK evidence-based interventions for foster care are likely to have time pressures that limit them from long database searches to find these. Though this leads to a potential limitation of this review, it highlights the need to label papers in a way that facilitates their use by those whom they are most valuable to. A similar limitation may be the use of peer-reviewed and published articles in this review: It may be that for example, the local authorities implementing MST as discussed by McAuley & Davis (2009) may have evaluated the effectiveness of this intervention but not shared the findings through publishing them, or the findings may have not reached a standard to be published. Again, if such 158 information has not been shared or is not able to be accessed, it limits the ability of other local authorities and services to understand the effectiveness and limitations of these interventions in the UK and make more informed decisions on how to best support foster children and families. Another limitation is the inclusion of papers that have included adoptive parents as well as foster parents as these may have prevented the review drawing on purely foster-related research. (e.g., Gurney-Smith et al. 2010). Despite this drawback, given the limited wealth of research in this area, it was felt including it would enable discussion of a more approaches being used with foster carers in the UK. The same lack of clarity may apply from having used papers that group looked-after children rather than focus on fostered children specifically. (e.g., Golding. 2010) Given that in 2011, 74% of looked after children were in foster placements suggests that this group is likely to constitute a majority of those used in studies including all looked after children and is worth considering. (Department for Education. 2011) Studies have found however, that those in residential care are statistically likely to have greater difficulties (Meltzer et al. 2003, 2004a, b). Future directions Given the limited research and weaknesses in methodology discussed in this review, there are a number of potential directions and considerations for future research. The use of RCT's in 159 some of the research papers critiqued is a promising step towards providing valid and reliable findings on the effectiveness of interventions for foster children in the UK, but greater sample sizes and avoiding using samples where individuals have 'opted in' to the study will help enable stronger conclusions to be drawn from findings. Given the often wide range of individuals involved in a foster-child's care, control groups are very important in validating the impact of an intervention and future studies should consider carefully to what extent their research may be compromised without a control group. Just as the diversity and difference issues between US and UK foster children, carers and systems need to be considered, so does the diversity of foster children and their carers in the UK. Acknowledging and investigating how issues of diversity and difference may impact the effectiveness of interventions may be key in help identifying what interventions are effective to use in the UK. Studies may need to investigate specific ethnic groups, children with specific lengths of time in care or particular histories, etc., or analyse results in more general studies to identify any potential betweengroup differences. Future research looking into supporting foster children with conduct disorders through foster-parent work needs to build on the work discussed in this review and study more carefully the outcomes for the foster children to ensure this work is having the positive impact on the children intended. Studies that look at longitudinal outcomes for foster-parents and children of these programmes will also be important in understanding the value of these interventions for the foster-care population. As emphasised earlier, with a positive and growing focus on evaluating interventions for foster children with conduct disorder in the UK, evaluations and developments of interventions for other difficulties experienced by UK foster children, such as emotional and anxiety disorders must not be 160 neglected. (Tapsfield & Collier. 2005). Depending on the focus, future work in this area may involve developing new approaches, making adjustments to approaches or looking at the validity and relevance of already established approaches but all done in order to ensure interventions used with this vulnerable group are safe and effective. Conclusion From this review, it is clear that the evidence base for the effectiveness of interventions that aim to improve the social, emotional and behavioural wellbeing for UK foster children is limited and weak. Foster-parent focused interventions in the UK show promise in helping foster carers feel more able to manage difficult behaviour but have done little to evidence positive outcomes for the foster children themselves. Evidence of the effectiveness for interventions that address the social and emotional difficulties of foster children in the UK is lacking. Consequently, there is great scope for studies that use stronger methodologies, larger sample studies, carefully defined samples, consider diversity implications and consider longterm effects of interventions, whether these be validating the effectiveness of studies developed elsewhere or developing new ones. What is clear is that with this vulnerable group of individuals, professionals have a responsibility to support these children and young people effectively, safely and use resources wisely to do this, to ensure we help both those with the most need but also as many individuals as possible. In order to do this, we need a greater understanding of what interventions can enable UK foster children to achieve the best 161 outcomes for their current wellbeing and future. 162 References Bywater, T., Hutchings, J., Linck, P., Whitaker, C., Daley, D., Yeo, S., & Edwards, R. (2010). Incredible Years Parent Training Support for Foster Carers in Wales: A Multi-Centre Feasibility Study. Child Care Health and Development. 37, 233-243. Department for Education. (2011). DfE: Children Looked After by Local Authorities in England (including adoption and care leavers) - year ending 31 March 2011. Retrieved from Department of Education website: http://www.education.gov.uk Department for Education and Skills. (2003). Every Child Matters: Presented to Parliament by the Chief Secretary to the Treasury by Command of Her Majesty, September 2003. London: Stationery Office. Retrieved from department of education website: http://www.education.gov.uk Ford, T., Vostanis, P., Meltzer, H. & Goodman, R. (2007). Psychiatric disorder among British children looked after by local authorities: comparison with children living in private households. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 319-325. Golding, K. (2010). Multi-agency and specialist working to meet the mental health needs of children in care and adopted. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 15, 573-587. 163 Gurney-Smith, B., Granger, C., Randle, A., & Fletcher, J. (2010). ‘In time and in tune’ – the Fostering Attachments Group: capturing sustained change in both caregiver and child. Adoption & Fostering, 34, 50–60. Herbert, M., & Wookey, J. (2007). The Child Wise Programme: A course to enhance the selfconfidence and behaviour management skills of foster carers with challenging children Adoption & Fostering Journal, 31, 27-37. Hibbert, G., & Frankl, J. (2011). A psychology consultation service for social workers and foster carers in a child and adolescent mental health service. Educational and Child Psychology, 28, 63-72. Holland, S., Faulkner, A., & Perez-del-Aguila, R. (2005). Promoting stability and continuity of care for looked after children: A survey and critical review. Child & Family Social Work, 10, 29-41. Landsverk, J., Burns, B., Stambaugh, L., & Reutz, J. (2009). Psychosocial interventions for children and adolescents in foster care: Review of research literature. Child Welfare , 88, 4969. Loxterkamp, L. (2009), 'Contact and Truth: The Unfolding Predicament in Adoption and Fostering'. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14, 423-35. 164 Macdonald, G., & Turner, W. (2005). An experiment in helping foster-carers manage challenging behaviour. British Journal of Social Work, 35, 1265-1282. McAuley, C & Davis, T (2009) 'The Emotional Well-Being of Looked After Children in England'. Child and Family Social Work, 14, 147-155. McDaniel, B., Braiden, H. J., Onyekwelu, J., Murphy, M., & Regan, H. (2011). Investigating the effectiveness of the incredible years basic parenting programme for foster carers in northern Ireland. Child Care in Practice, 17, 55-67. Meltzer, H, Gatward, R, Goodman, R and Ford, T. (2000). The mental health of children and adolescents in Great Britain: summary report. London: The Stationery Office. Meltzer, H., Gatward, R., Corbin, T., Goodman, R., Ford, T. (2003). The mental health of young people looked after by local authorities in England. London: The Stationary Office. Meltzer, H., Lader, D., Corbin, T., Goodman, R. and Ford, T. (2004a). The mental health of young people looked after by local authorities in Scotland. Edinburgh: The Stationery Office. Meltzer, H., Lader, D., Corbin, T., Goodman, R., Ford, T. (2004b). The mental health of young people looked after by local authorities in Wales. London: The Stationary Office. 165 Moore, K. J., & Chamberlain, P. (1994). Treatment foster care: Toward development of community-based models for adolescents with severe emotional and behavioral disorders. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 2, 22-30. National Institute of Clinical Excellence. (2001). The guideline development process – Information for national collaborating centres and guideline development groups. London: NICE. Retrieved at: http://www.nice.org.uk Pallett C, Scott S, Blackeby K, Yule W and Weissman R (2002) Fostering changes: a cognitive-behavioural approach to help foster carers manage children. Adoption and fostering, 26, 39-48. Sen, R., & Broadhurst, K. (2011). Contact between children in out of home placements and their family and friends networks: A research review. Child & Family Social Work, 16, 298309. Staines, J., Farmer, E., & Selwyn, J. (2011). Implementing a therapeutic team parenting approach to fostering: The experiences of one independent foster-care agency. British Journal of Social Work, 41, 314-332. Tapsfield, R., & Collier, F. (2005). The cost of foster care: investing in our children's future. London: British Agencies for Adoption and Fostering/ Fostering Network. 166 Thoburn, J. (2007). Globalisation and child welfare: Some lessons from a cross-national study of children in out-of-home care. Norwich: UEA Social Work Monographs. Wakelyn, J. (2008). Transitional psychotherapy for looked-after children in 'short-term' foster care. Journal of Social Work Practice, 22, 27-36. Ward, H., Holmes, L., Soper, J., & Olsen, R. (2008). Costs and consequences of placing children in care. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. World Health Organisation. (1992). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (ICD-10), 10th revision. Geneva: Author. 167 Literature review UK evidence for interventions to improve the psychological wellbeing of foster children: A systematic review By Ruth Joanne Hunter School of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences University of Surrey April 2012 168 Abstract Statistics from the UK highlight that foster children and young people are more than three times more likely than those in the general youth and child population to experience mental health difficulties. (Meltzer et al. 2003, 2004). The majority of interventions to improve foster children's psychological wellbeing have been developed and validated in the US yet some of these are being used in the UK. This systematic literature review looked at the research on the effectiveness of interventions to improve the social, emotional and behavioural wellbeing of foster children in the UK. It was a concern that few papers were found and all had substantial methodological weaknesses that highlighted a large gap for good quality detailed research to be undertaken to ensure professionals are appropriately and effectively supporting this vulnerable group of individuals, directly or indirectly with the interventions they use. Future research needs to address these methodological issues, build on current research, identify interventions for difficulties other than behavioural problems and consider issues of diversity and difference within this vulnerable group. Key words: Foster, children, young people, UK, interventions, review, outcomes Introduction 169 Foster children in the UK Statistics from Department for Education 2011 National Statistics showed that over 48,000 children were in foster care in the UK on March 31st 2011. Children and youth in foster care have higher rates of mental health difficulties than those in the general population. Ford et al (2007) highlighted that the results of a number of studies by Meltzer et al (2000, 2003, 2004a) emphasised that even compared to the most disadvantaged children outside the care system, the mental health of children looked after by local authorities was significantly worse. These studies all involved interviewing carers, teachers and children (between 11-15 years) and using the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Health Related Disorders (ICD-10) to identify a prevalence of a mental health disorder. This was defined by Meltzer et al (2000) as “a clinically recognisable set of symptoms or behaviours associated with considerable distress and substantial interference with personal functions”. Meltzer et al (2003) found that ‘mental disorders’ were present in 40% of the fostered population in England compared to just 13% and 10% for boys and girls between 11 and 15 years respectively and a prevalence of 6% for children between 5-10 years in the general population. (Meltzer et al. 2000). Meltzer et al (2004a, b) found similar prevalence rates for foster children in Scotland and Wales. On top of this, Meltzer et al (2003) highlighted that for looked after children in England, mental health problems (that did not reach criteria for a disorder) was reported for over two fifths of the children included in the study by their carers. 170 A study by Ward et al (2008) found that 70-80% of foster children and young people require specialist emotional and behavioural support. Tapsfield & Collier's (2005) study reported a 37% prevalence of conduct disorder in looked-after children in the UK, which was almost three times higher than that of the general population. Meltzer et al (2004a) identified that after conduct disorders, emotional disorders (specifically anxiety disorders), were the most common mental disorders in looked-after children in Scotland, England and Wales. This was followed by hyperkinetic disorders and depression. Such increased prevalence rates highlight the importance of effective and appropriate psychological support and interventions for foster children. Being healthy, staying safe, enjoying and achieving, making a positive contribution and economic wellbeing were the five outcome aims for children proposed by the government in 2003 (Department for Education and Skills. 2003). If foster children are to be supported to achieve the 5 outcomes then, given their increased risk and vulnerability to mental health difficulties previously detailed then clearly a greater level of mental health support is needed for these individuals. Best practise is to use evidenced based interventions and support when undertaking any type of psychological support, to ensure at worst that no harm is being done, providing the best outcome possible for the intended individuals and that the time and resources of professionals, foster carers and foster children are not being wasted. (National Institute for Clinical Excellence. 2001). Cutbacks and changes in health and social services mean that there are even fewer resources available to promote wellbeing, making it even 171 more essential that the interventions and psychological support being implemented are not only the most effective, but effective for the most needy as well as those with less intense difficulties. Established evidence A basic search of Ebsco, Psychinfo, Medline highlights a wealth of foster care literature and research from the United States (US). Reviews of this research highlight interventions and approaches to mental health care that have the best evidence base so far. Landsverk et al's (2009) work is the most recent published review and they reviewed the evidence for psychosocial interventions for the most prevalent conditions found in foster children in the US. They highlighted that for post-traumatic stress disorder, abuse related trauma, depression, substance abuse and disruptive behaviour disorders, interventions that addressed symptoms, behaviour and functioning had the best (and indeed a 'strong') evidence base. It is beyond the scope of this review to detail all the interventions found by Landsverk et al (2009) to be promising interventions for each of these difficulties. But they found that most interventions were brief and usually more effective when the caregiver was actively involved. Interventions varied between involving multiple professionals, the child and the carer, being community based or clinic based, and of course, the theoretical basis for the approach, highlighting the breadth of approaches that have been found to be effective in the US. 172 UK programmes being implemented Indeed some of these US evidence-based approaches have already begun to be implemented in the UK. McAuley & Davis (2009) identified that ten local authorities have implemented Multi-systemic therapy (MST) programmes for young people aged 11-17 and their families who are on the edge of custody and an additional site piloting MST specifically for children who have experienced abuse or neglect. MST is a US developed intensive home-based family based intervention that acknowledges the role of the family environment on difficulties experienced by the child and the family. Landsverk et al (2009) identified this as an intervention with promise, concluding from critiquing 10 studies into MFT that it was particularly promising for working with foster children to address substance abuse and/or disruptive behaviours. They concluded it was positively associated with a reduction in aggressive behaviours in foster children and fewer placements. McAuley & Davis (2009) also identified 14 local authorities implementing Multi-dimensional Treatment Foster Care (MTFC) programmes for adolescents aged 11-17 years, six programmes for 3-6 year-olds and eight sites for children aged 7-11 years. McAuley & Davis (2009) highlighted initiatives to cascade the programme out to other local authorities. MTFC is a US established programme based on social learning theory that aims to reduce problem behaviours and promote developmentally appropriate and prosocial behaviour through a multifaceted, multiple setting based approach. This includes foster parenting in the home using specific behavioural techniques, skills training and supportive therapy for the foster child, school consultations 173 and family therapy (Moore & Chamberlain. 1994). The other approach identified as being developed when McAuley & Davis (2009) published their research was a cognitivebehavioural programme called the Fostering Changes Programme, which focussed on helping foster carers manage children and was developed by Pallett et al (2002). The Fostering Changes Programme (FCP) was the only intervention to be highlighted by McAuley & Davis (2009) as having been developed and trialled in the UK prior to being rolled out. It is based on 'The Incredible Years' programme (Webster-Stratton, 1998), which was developed in the US, and aimed to promote positive parenting and reduce children's behavioural difficulties and improve their problem solving and social skills. The FCP trial found carers reported improvements in their foster children's behaviour, but results from their other measures were less supportive of this. Through their study, McAuley and Davis (2009) highlighted an apparent drive to pilot interventions in the UK that had been shown to have a strong or emerging evidence-base in the US in relation to effectiveness. They discussed hopes that these pilots would reinforce the value of evidence-based commissioning and gave an acknowledgement of the need to obtain evidence of the effectiveness of these interventions for the UK foster population. No detail however, as to how or why these interventions may or may not be applicable in the UK was addressed in the study: Details that seem fundamental to the predictions of whether these interventions are likely to be effective for the UK foster population. Services in the UK increasingly acknowledge the diversity of the UK population itself and the impact that these differences in culture and background may have on psychological interventions and support. Also, foster children in themselves vary on a multitude of issues: their history, reason for entering care, length of time in care, ethnic 174 background, religion, culture, etc. Identifying and acknowledging some of these differences may provide insight into how and why the effectiveness of interventions with an evidencebase in the US may differ in the UK and guide appropriate adjustments or changes and the appropriateness of implementing or testing these. US and UK foster care In 2007, Thoburn published a study comparing statistics about children in out-of-home care across a number of different countries. She noted a number of similarities and comparisons between the US and the UK in her analysis of the statistics she collated. Thoburn (2007) identified that a fewer percentage of those entering care were 16-17 year olds in the UK than the US, 7% and 20% respectively and the UK had a higher percentage entering at 10-15 years (40% to 25% respectively) but that for those under 12 months the percentages were very similar, (17% and 15%). Thoburn's (2007) data identified that in the USA over 90% of children were taken into out-of-home care as a result of abuse or neglect, whereas this was only 48% for England, with other factors listed as parental disabilities or illness (8%), disability or problems of the child (9%), abandonment (11%) and relationships or other family problems (24%). Wales also had similar percentages to England. Thoburn (2007) also hypothesised that using performance targets to control welfare spending was linked to seeing out-of-home care as something to be avoided; a similarity for the USA and the UK. Differences between the USA and the UK care system were also highlighted in the report 175 through comparing the legal status of children entering care on one particular day: For the USA, over 95% entered into care as a result of a court decision, compared to only 33% in the UK. The USA and UK had similar average lengths of time spent in out-of-home care: 1.8 year and 2.1 years respectively and both countries had a 5% adoption rate. Also, both countries had similar percentages of children placed in un-related foster family care: Between 47% and 57% for the four UK countries and 46% in the USA. As highlighted earlier, Landsverk et al (2009) identified post-traumatic stress disorder, abuse related trauma, depression, substance abuse and disruptive behaviour disorders as the most prevalent mental health conditions of children in foster care in the US. In comparison, Meltzer et al (2003) highlighted conduct disorders as the most common disorder experienced by children in care (not just foster children) in the UK, followed by anxiety disorders and hyperkinetic disorders. Statistics however do not report on the level of impact of the mental health difficulties individually which is also likely to impact what types of support and interventions need to be prioritised by each country. On top of differences in legislation, characteristics and factors related to children entering foster care; in both countries, some ethnic minorities are overrepresented in foster care. In the UK, African Caribbean and mixed African Caribbean and white heritage are overrepresented in foster care with black Africans overrepresented to a lesser extent. (Thoburn. 2007). In the USA, African American and Native American children are overrepresented in out-of-home care statistics, with Hispanic children increasingly so. (Thoburn. 2007). In the UK however, 176 Thoburn (2007) highlight the underrepresentation of those of East and South Asian heritage. Statistics on the percentage of foster carers from ethnic minorities in the UK and the US are difficult to obtain, likely due to the presence of public and private foster-care employment agencies and variations in data governance that create difficulties in obtaining statistics from these. There are a range of other factors such as average number of placements, placements with siblings, payment of foster carers and a number of other factors that may vary between the US and the UK. These could all be issues of diversity and difference that may impact the appropriateness or effectiveness of psychological interventions. These statistics highlight the breadth and depth of similarities and differences between both the foster care systems themselves and the characteristics of the children fostered in these systems and their difficulties. As discussed, there are also likely to be many more. Given these details, it seems complacent to assume that the evidence for effectiveness of interventions used in the USA is automatically relevant to the UK. It highlights the implementation of such evidence-based practises across the UK as a risky and potentially costly trial, in terms of public money and psychosocial impact on the foster children and their families if it is an ineffective intervention. Though US research dominates the literature databases, one would hope that given the extent that programmes such as MST and MTFC have already been implemented in a number of local authorities, that UK based evidence for effectiveness is now guiding practise in the UK more than US evidence. This literature review will now look at what evidence exists for the effectiveness of interventions for foster 177 children in the UK. Method An electronic systematic literature search was undertaken on Psychinfo, Medline, Ebsco, Web of Knowledge and Cochrane databases. It included peer-reviewed papers published from January 2005 to present. The search terms used were “intervention* OR therap*”; “foster* OR looked after OR accommodated OR” and “UK OR United Kingdom OR Brit* OR Welsh OR Wales OR Scot* OR Northern Ireland OR Northern Irish OR Engl*”. Papers were excluded if they did not focus on interventions and outcomes related to children while in foster care (therefore excluding a focus on transition and adult outcomes of foster care). Papers were also excluded if they focussed on policies rather than practise, interventions within education rather than health, or if they focussed specifically on foster children with additional medical health needs such as Learning Disabilities. These exclusion criteria were used as they represent specific areas and groups that are above and beyond the scope of this review. Papers were not excluded based on type of evidence, though consideration as to the quality of each type of evidence is evaluated in the review in line with guidelines from the National Institute of Clinical Excellence (NICE, 2006). Papers that discussed a theoretical approach and used examples such as case studies were included, in order to gain an understanding of potential areas for developments or changes to interventions. The search was performed February 2012 and produced 11 papers that described or evaluated 178 interventions with foster children in the UK. These were divided into categories for evaluation based on who was involved in the intervention; 1) foster parents 2) foster children 3) mixed focus (including combinations of foster parents, foster children, social workers, etc.). The critique of each of the studies was focussed on the methodology, in order to evaluate the strength and validity of the respective results for each, in order to determine the strength of the evidence from each paper, rather than the theoretical approach of the intervention. Results Interventions with Parents of Foster Children Interventions with foster carers are often used as indirect interventions to improve the social, emotional and behavioural outcomes of foster children; most frequently aiming to equip foster carers to manage foster children's difficulties, in order to improve placement stability (a factor that can impact the social, emotional and behavioural outcomes of foster children (Holland, Faulkner & Perez-del-Aguila. 2005)) and help foster children reach their potential. (McDaniel et al. 2011). Two of the papers from the search focussed on CBT based programmes for foster carers. MacDonald et al (2005) looked at the effectiveness of CBT training for parents of foster 179 children with challenging behaviour. They used a randomised controlled trial (RCT) using groups of foster carers. 117 foster carers (all but 2 of whom were white) opted into the study; 67 of these were in the training group and 50 in the control group. Data was collected before and after the training and at 6 months follow-up. Results were that foster-carers in the training group reported increased confidence in ability to deal with behaviour problems and scored higher on knowledge of behavioural principles compared to those in the control group. McDonald et al (2005) queried whether the content and length of the course could have led to disappointing outcomes or a lack of support to help develop the skills learnt. The authors also suggest the limited effectiveness of the CBT training in the results could have been due to using carers of extremely 'difficult' children. They proposed the intervention be refined to deal with the weaknesses identified to ascertain whether in fact the intervention is ineffective or whether this study failed to identify the effectiveness of it due to its limitations. Herbert & Wookey (2007) investigated whether a broadly based CBT programme could indirectly reduce the challenging behaviour of looked-after children and increase placement stability, through improving carers' self-assurance and behaviour management skills. Their study used a randomised controlled study, with 67 foster-carers in the experimental group and 50 in the control group. Those in the experimental group were randomly allocated into six groups, all given the same intervention. Their ages ranged from 32 to 65 and had a mean of 8.68 years of foster experience. The intervention, the Child Wise Programme (CWP) was devised by the authors. It is a programme that aims to be collaborative between facilitators and foster-carers and to empower caregivers by giving them a conceptual framework (CBT 180 and behavioural principles and techniques) that enables them to resolve problems themselves. Herbert & Wookey (2007) reported that CWP has obtained positive results when used with birth parents but had not been validated for use with foster-carers, but this was not referenced in this paper. Results were a significant increase in carers' confidence, a majority of foster carers reporting improvements in children's behaviour (but not to a significant level) and that these were generally attributed to having acquired new behavioural management skills. There was no reduction found in relation to unplanned placement breakdowns where behaviour problems were indicated. Though both these CBT based approaches suggest some promise in supporting foster carers with children with challenging behaviours in the UK, both studies relied only on parent reports of child outcomes and failed to identify detailed demographics of the foster-carers and foster-children involved that could shed light on whom specifically these interventions may work for. Though they used randomised controlled trials, generalisability of the results for both studies is limited by relatively small sample sizes and drop-outs of participants, but also for McDonald et al (2005), by the fact that carers were required to opt into the study. Also, the study that included a six month follow-up (McDonald et al. 2005) found less positive results of the study than Herbert & Wookey (2007), which could suggest a CBT-focussed intervention may have limited long-term effectiveness. Unfortunately a lack of detail about each of the programmes makes it difficult to identify the similarities and differences between the programmes which could be helpful in investigating these approaches further. 181 Two other papers found looked at the effectiveness of the Incredible Years Basic Parent (School Age) Programme (IYP) for improving foster parents ability to manage the complex needs of looked after children by teaching them skills to manage child behaviour and consequently promote placement stability. The IYP is founded on social learning theory and was originally developed for use with biological parents. It's protocol includes detailed session plans and home activities that can be tailored by professionals to the specific needs of families. McDaniel et al (2011) conducted a pilot programme in Northern Ireland, implemented by Barnardo's Professional Fostering Service, using a single-group pre-test and post-test design with 13 foster carers. McDaniel et al (2011) focussed on an age range of 8 and 13, providing some (though limited) information on this. Assessments were carried out before and immediately after the group training and included a self-report parent rating scale to measure the degree of conduct and externalising behaviour management problems are exhibited by children. Results from the study suggested a significant decrease in the intensity of social, emotional and behavioural difficulties reported by foster carers following the IYP, and a smaller but not significant decrease in the extent to which foster carers felt their foster child's behaviour was problematic. Bywater et al (2008) also looked at the effectiveness of IYP. They emphasised the potential of IYP to enhance placement stability and help foster carers understand the needs of lookedafter children. Their study focussed on 46 foster carers of children between 2 and 8 years and employed controlled randomisation to allocate to the intervention (n=29) and control (n=17) groups. Foster carers were approached and requested to participate in the study and received 182 monetary rewards for completing data at baseline and six-month follow-up. The children ranged between 2 and 17 years at the start of the study. Results suggest that the IYP was an effective programme for significantly reducing carer depression and reducing challenging behaviours in the foster children, as rated by the foster carers. Bywater et al (2008) questioned whether their findings had been limited by using a parent-report scale that they felt needed to be more sensitive. They also did not use other measures which reduced the scope of measuring the changes that had occurred. Again, despite promising findings, evidence of the effectiveness of IYP from both these studies is limited by their small sample sizes. Bywater (2008) and McDaniel et al (2011) also relied only on foster-carer reports to identify any impact of IYP on the foster children themselves, and gave very limited information of the foster children and carers, limiting understanding of who exactly the IYP appeared to be effective for. McDaniel et al (2011) also failed to use a control group, which further limits the evidence for whether IYP is a promising intervention. Additionally, a confounding feature in the study was Barnardo's Professional Fostering Service provided ongoing support and training to foster carers outside of the IYP which may have instead resulted, or contributed to the positive changes reported by the carers. Furthermore, McDaniel et al's (2011) study failed to ascertain whether any reports of changes or improvements from the intervention were maintained over time. A strength of these studies was that they used quantitative measures to identify 'significant' improvements. Though both McDaniel et al (2011) and Bywater (2009) also failed to detail many demographics of the foster carers and foster children in their studies that could have aided 183 understanding of for whom the intervention was most effective (backgrounds, placements, etc.). Overall, the study provides promising evidence but yet again another tentative start to identifying the effectiveness of IYP for foster children. Gurney-Smith et al (2010) used a repeated measures design, (pre, post and three month follow-up) design to evaluate the impact of a Fostering Attachments Group (FAG) for a mixed group of foster and adoptive parents. FAG (Golding & Picken. 2004) is based on social learning and attachment theories and aims to develop parenting skills by educating about the influence of the social environment on behaviour, with consideration of the need for responsive and attuned parenting to develop successful attachments. The group comprised of 5 foster carers and 10 adoptive parents and a special guardian; one male and 15 females. Evaluation data was only collected from 13 participants at all three stages. Inclusion criteria involved currently experiencing problem behaviour from a child when the group started and difficulties in attachment relationships for that child. Referrals came from Social workers and were active cases in the Children, Young People and Families Service. Measures used were all parent-report tools. Analysis showed that reported parenting skills and understanding improved to a statistically significant degree which was maintained through to follow-up. A significant decrease in reported child hyperactivity/inattention behaviours was obtained between pre-group and follow-up time points. Post hoc tests also found a significant decrease in overall difficulties of the child reported between pre-group and 3 month follow-up scores. Parenting stress levels were not impacted but children were reported to show significantly less inhibition. 184 Similarly to some of the other studies, no control group was used by Gurney-Smith et al (2010), the sample size was very small, long-term evaluation was limited (only measured at three months after) and measures used only foster-parent reports to identify any positive outcomes for the foster children. The relevance of the findings to foster children is also limited through the study using adoptive parents and a special guardian as well as foster parents as, despite commonalities of background histories, their situations of temporary and permanent placements may well impact the social environment quite differently. Again, as with the other studies, though this study suggests some potential for being effective in working with foster parents, this study fails to identify whether this approach has a truly positive impact for the child. Interventions directly with Foster Children Loxterkamp (2009) aimed to challenge what she described as the ‘predominant conviction’ (p.423) that regular contact of adopted and fostered children with their biological parents is always beneficial. Loxterkamp used three cases studies to illustrate her arguments that contact is not always beneficial (in the short or long-term) and instead can often be the cause of secondary harm. Though this approach highlights well the points argued by Loxterkamp (2009) it does not provide high quality evidence that her hypotheses are valid and that the experiences of those in her case examples are generalisable to those in the wider population. 185 For example, she chose the case studies to include as she felt they were representative of the common attitudes in relation to contact. They were also chosen following therapy, as cases Loxterkamp (2009) that had highlighted issue of biological contact that supported her arguments, bias that cannot be ignored. Consequently, this study begins to identify potential issues of contact but provides poor evidence for these upon which to initiate change in a matter that has well-established beliefs and practises associated with it. In contrast, Sen and Broadhurst's (2011) study was a narrative review of studies into outcomes of foster children as a result of contact with their biological family. However, they gave little detail of the methods of the studies and detailed no quantitative outcomes. They highlighted that evidence for the impact of biological parent contact on children's emotional, behavioural and intellectual outcomes was largely dated and inconsistent with no studies having employed systematic measures to evidence the effect of contact on child outcomes in relation to both UK and International studies. The only two main papers discussed in detail in relation to this were focussed on disruption of attachment formation through contact with biological parents. But again, little detail of the strengths of the study or the conclusions found were presented. Sen & Broadhurst (2011) also looked at the impact of biological parent contact on stability of placements, but failed to link this to identifying psychosocial outcomes for the child and again, the descriptive style provided limited insight into the strength of the evidence found from the studies presented. Sen & Broadhurst (2011) suggest a number of components for future research that would help improve the quality of evidence found, but that these conclusions are valid relies on the reader assuming that these components were not 186 included in the studies discussed by Sen & Broadhurst (2011) based on direct omission of these details from the review rather than detailed critique of the studies, highlighting these omissions. Such suggestions included a need for studies using robust outcomes measures including psychosocial profiling and children's views, sufficiently sized and representative samples of foster children, prospective and longitudinal designs and based on clear definitions of contact. A lack of detail and lack of systematic approach to addressing the literature, alongside the approach used by Loxterkamp (2009) means that evidence for the outcomes of foster children as a result of contact with their biological parents in the UK is currently very unclear. The third study by Wakelyn (2008), looked at the impact of transitional therapy for children in 'short-term' foster care. This work discussed the theories and used one case example to illustrate the approach. It shows a promising consideration of developing and detailing new approaches and ways of working with foster-children that take into account the importance of well evidenced facets of practises such as multi-disciplinary working. It does not however, provide evidence for the effectiveness of this intervention on improving the psychosocial and behavioural outcomes of foster children given the limitations of using a case study, as with Loxterkamp (2009). Interventions with Multiple groups 187 Staines, Farmer & Selwyn (2011) investigated the effectiveness of a Therapeutic Team Parenting approach. A prospective repeated measures design was used, involving children between the ages of 5 and 14 entering new placements organised by one Independent FosterCare Agency. Twenty-three local authorities participated, 450 children were identified and they, their carers and social workers were sent questionnaires at the start of the placement and one year into it. Only eighty placements had data from carers and social workers at both time points which reduced how representative the results are of the placements as a whole. Despite detailing some demographics of the carers, including ethnicity, age and marital status, the sample sizes did not allow reliable detailed analysis of the result in relation to these. The team parenting approach emphasises all team members involved with the child working collaboratively together, assessing the young person in placement rather than in isolation. The therapeutic fostering environment views the placement and activities provided as enabling the fostered individual to learn and develop, based on a systemic approach. Results found high levels of satisfaction from foster-parents for the therapeutic and team parenting approaches and the authors concluded that high-quality placements were provided for many of the challenging children. The authors acknowledged that the approach was not always implemented as efficiently as intended (delays in service provision following assessment, for example). But alongside this, due to the small return rate of questionnaires it is difficult to determine whether this approach positively impacts placement stability or child outcomes or whom it is particularly beneficial to. On top of this, whether this approach could be effective outside the Independent Foster-Care sector could be questioned from debate that Independent Foster Care Agencies often do not provide placements for the most challenging children in 188 the care system. Hibbert & Frankyl (2011) also relied on qualitative analysis and were even more limited by a small sample size in their study into the impact of a psychology consultation service set up to support foster carers and social workers. The approach was a consultation model that aimed to combine clinical expertise and knowledge of psychological interventions for foster children with the experiences and knowledge of social workers and foster carers in order is to help foster carers understand the child’s early experience, the impact of this and then adapt expectations of the child and strategies of working with them accordingly. Social workers (n=7) were randomly selected but foster carers (n=7) opted into the study. They had between 1 and 6 consultation meetings (3.3 and 4.4 average for foster carers and social workers respectively). A semi-structured interview about the consultation service was undertaken and evaluated using thematic analysis. From this analysis, Hibbert & Frankyl (2011) highlighted that all foster carers had reported an increase in psychological understanding of the child and in skills for behaviour management, which in turn foster carers reported had led to improved relationships between carers and their children and more self-confidence from this. Foster carers also reported feeling emotionally supported. Social workers reported feeling supported within the wider professional system, able to obtain different perspectives and reflect on situations, gaining enhanced skills and support with decision making and planning. Unfortunately, again no long-term investigations or child specific outcome measure were used. Given the qualitative nature of the study and the small sample size, the generalisability and reliability of the benefits of this approach appear are also clearly limited, though it 189 suggests a promising potential approach that could be worth investigating further to build on these findings. Golding (2010) investigated a multiagency approach to supporting children in foster care, using an example of an integrated multi-agency service in Worcestershire for Looked-after and adopted children (ISL). The aim is to maximise placement stability by providing mental health and emotional well-being guidance and advice based around a consultation process, supported by additional facets such as network meetings, supporting parenting and providing education and mental health interventions. The author draws upon professional clinical experience and states that 'a range of research and audit-based evaluations has confirmed ISL provides an effective additional level of support for the children, carers and professional network' (p581. Golding, 2010) but does not detail what these are other than detailing that service user views and a pilot evaluation were used. Outcomes are detailed only as foster carers having increased confidence and a changed perception of their children following consultation leaving a vague understanding of the added value of the team. It is also unclear as to the value of this work for the foster children's outcomes specifically, long-term benefits and what components of the approach may be important. Evaluations are needed to identify this, at least to ensure it is not causing harm, if there are other benefits that motivate services to employ such an approach. Discussion 190 Overall, there is currently very little evidence for the effectiveness of any particular intervention to improve the social, emotional and behavioural outcomes for foster children in the UK. Firstly, interventions involving foster-carers appear to have stronger methodologies, using control groups and showing some attempts at identifying long-term outcomes of the approaches used. Results from these studies all appear promising in producing positive outcomes for carers, but unfortunately all child-related outcomes are based on parent reports only, leading to a lack of understanding as to whether these interventions are truly have a positive impact on foster-children. Is it enough to assume positive outcomes for a child if their parents report being more able to cope with difficult behaviour or reduced problem behaviours from the child? Placement stability has been evidenced as having a positive impact on foster children’s social, emotional and behavioural outcomes, but are these studies making assumptions that if foster parents feel more able to cope then the placement will be more stable? Further studies need to take these questions into careful consideration. Demographic details of foster parents and children are also limited in these studies and this therefore limits understanding for whom these interventions may be effective and when. Studies that have tried to consider these have been restrained by their sample sizes. Of the foster-parent based interventions critiqued in this review, all show promise in supporting foster carers and potentially foster children in relation to challenging behaviour particularly, but due to methodological issues it is difficult to identify if any show more promise than others currently. Secondly, interventions that directly involve the foster child are currently lacking in numbers and development: Only Wakelyn (2008) has discussed a specific 191 approach and Loxterkamp (2009) and Sen & Broadhurst (2011) looked at the common practise of contact with biological parents. Methods to evaluate these are also weak, with a lack of controlled trials and use of measures to identify child outcomes. Thirdly, interventions that include a range of individuals including carers, professionals and sometimes fosterchildren themselves seem to be even more difficult to ascertain whether they are having a positive impact on foster children's psychosocial wellbeing: All these studies looked at failed to use control groups and had small sample sizes or relatively high drop-out rates (or incomplete data). Alongside this, they also relied on parent-report of child improvements and changes, rather than multiple perspectives further highlighting difficulties ascertaining whether these approaches truly benefit foster children. The detail of the work undertaken within each study was also lacking, leading to limited understanding about what in the approach may have been beneficial and why. Similarly to the parent-focussed approaches, these studies also had limited demographic data of the foster children and carers involved and the potential impact of issues of difference and diversity on the effectiveness of the interventions. Overall, the approaches that currently appear to have the most established evidence as to their effectiveness in the UK are the foster parent focussed approaches. (McDonald et al (2005); Herbert& Wookey (2007); McDaniel et al (2011); Bywater et al (2008); Gurney-Smith et al (2009)). These however, have much to improve on both in the quality and detail of evidence obtained but also in the fact that they have done more to evidence their positive outcomes for foster carers than foster children themselves and this needs to be addressed as a priority in 192 future research with these approaches. What is also worth highlighting, is that these interventions focus almost solely on issues around challenging behaviours in foster children and though this is the current most common difficulty for foster children in the UK (Tapsfield & Collier. 2005), other difficulties and disorders experienced by foster children need effective and appropriate interventions also. Limitations Limitations of this systematic review are that the inclusion of the search terms around location may have overlooked some papers that have been undertaken in the UK but have not been explicitly listed as such. Despite this, it is important to consider that professionals or researchers interested in using UK evidence-based interventions for foster care are likely to have time pressures that limit them from long database searches to find these. Though this leads to a potential limitation of this review, it highlights the need to label papers in a way that facilitates their use by those whom they are most valuable to. A similar limitation may be the use of peer-reviewed and published articles in this review: It may be that for example, the local authorities implementing MST as discussed by McAuley & Davis (2009) may have evaluated the effectiveness of this intervention but not shared the findings through publishing them, or the findings may have not reached a standard to be published. Again, if such information has not been shared or is not able to be accessed, it limits the ability of other local authorities and services to understand the effectiveness and limitations of these 193 interventions in the UK and make more informed decisions on how to best support foster children and families. Another limitation is the inclusion of papers that have included adoptive parents as well as foster parents as these may have prevented the review drawing on purely foster-related research. (e.g., Gurney-Smith et al. 2010). Despite this drawback, given the limited wealth of research in this area, it was felt including it would enable discussion of a more approaches being used with foster carers in the UK. The same lack of clarity may apply from having used papers that group looked-after children rather than focus on fostered children specifically. (e.g., Golding. 2010) Given that in 2011, 74% of looked after children were in foster placements suggests that this group is likely to constitute a majority of those used in studies including all looked after children and is worth considering. (Department for Education. 2011) Studies have found however, that those in residential care are statistically likely to have greater difficulties (Meltzer et al. 2003, 2004a, b). Future directions Given the limited research and weaknesses in methodology discussed in this review, there are a number of potential directions and considerations for future research. The use of RCT's in some of the research papers critiqued is a promising step towards providing valid and reliable findings on the effectiveness of interventions for foster children in the UK, but greater sample 194 sizes and avoiding using samples where individuals have 'opted in' to the study will help enable stronger conclusions to be drawn from findings. Given the often wide range of individuals involved in a foster-child's care, control groups are very important in validating the impact of an intervention and future studies should consider carefully to what extent their research may be compromised without a control group. Just as the diversity and difference issues between US and UK foster children, carers and systems need to be considered, so does the diversity of foster children and their carers in the UK. Acknowledging and investigating how issues of diversity and difference may impact the effectiveness of interventions may be key in help identifying what interventions are effective to use in the UK. Studies may need to investigate specific ethnic groups, children with specific lengths of time in care or particular histories, etc., or analyse results in more general studies to identify any potential betweengroup differences. Future research looking into supporting foster children with conduct disorders through foster-parent work needs to build on the work discussed in this review and study more carefully the outcomes for the foster children to ensure this work is having the positive impact on the children intended. Studies that look at longitudinal outcomes for foster-parents and children of these programmes will also be important in understanding the value of these interventions for the foster-care population. As emphasised earlier, with a positive and growing focus on evaluating interventions for foster children with conduct disorder in the UK, evaluations and developments of interventions for other difficulties experienced by UK foster children, such as emotional and anxiety disorders must not be neglected. (Tapsfield & Collier. 2005). Depending on the focus, future work in this area may involve developing new approaches, making adjustments to approaches or looking at the 195 validity and relevance of already established approaches but all done in order to ensure interventions used with this vulnerable group are safe and effective. Conclusion From this review, it is clear that the evidence base for the effectiveness of interventions that aim to improve the social, emotional and behavioural wellbeing for UK foster children is limited and weak. Foster-parent focused interventions in the UK show promise in helping foster carers feel more able to manage difficult behaviour but have done little to evidence positive outcomes for the foster children themselves. Evidence of the effectiveness for interventions that address the social and emotional difficulties of foster children in the UK is lacking. Consequently, there is great scope for studies that use stronger methodologies, larger sample studies, carefully defined samples, consider diversity implications and consider longterm effects of interventions, whether these be validating the effectiveness of studies developed elsewhere or developing new ones. What is clear is that with this vulnerable group of individuals, professionals have a responsibility to support these children and young people effectively, safely and use resources wisely to do this, to ensure we help both those with the most need but also as many individuals as possible. In order to do this, we need a greater understanding of what interventions can enable UK foster children to achieve the best outcomes for their current wellbeing and future. 196 References Bywater, T., Hutchings, J., Linck, P., Whitaker, C., Daley, D., Yeo, S., & Edwards, R. (2010). Incredible Years Parent Training Support for Foster Carers in Wales: A Multi-Centre Feasibility Study. Child Care Health and Development. 37, 233-243. Department for Education. (2011). DfE: Children Looked After by Local Authorities in England (including adoption and care leavers) - year ending 31 March 2011. Retrieved from Department of Education website: http://www.education.gov.uk Department for Education and Skills. (2003). Every Child Matters: Presented to Parliament by the Chief Secretary to the Treasury by Command of Her Majesty, September 2003. London: Stationery Office. Retrieved from department of education website: http://www.education.gov.uk Ford, T., Vostanis, P., Meltzer, H. & Goodman, R. (2007). Psychiatric disorder among British children looked after by local authorities: comparison with children living in private households. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 319-325. Golding, K. (2010). Multi-agency and specialist working to meet the mental health needs of children in care and adopted. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 15, 573-587. 197 Gurney-Smith, B., Granger, C., Randle, A., & Fletcher, J. (2010). ‘In time and in tune’ – the Fostering Attachments Group: capturing sustained change in both caregiver and child. Adoption & Fostering, 34, 50–60. Herbert, M., & Wookey, J. (2007). The Child Wise Programme: A course to enhance the selfconfidence and behaviour management skills of foster carers with challenging children Adoption & Fostering Journal, 31, 27-37. Hibbert, G., & Frankl, J. (2011). A psychology consultation service for social workers and foster carers in a child and adolescent mental health service. Educational and Child Psychology, 28, 63-72. Holland, S., Faulkner, A., & Perez-del-Aguila, R. (2005). Promoting stability and continuity of care for looked after children: A survey and critical review. Child & Family Social Work, 10, 29-41. Landsverk, J., Burns, B., Stambaugh, L., & Reutz, J. (2009). Psychosocial interventions for children and adolescents in foster care: Review of research literature. Child Welfare , 88, 4969. Loxterkamp, L. (2009), 'Contact and Truth: The Unfolding Predicament in Adoption and Fostering'. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14, 423-35. 198 Macdonald, G., & Turner, W. (2005). An experiment in helping foster-carers manage challenging behaviour. British Journal of Social Work, 35, 1265-1282. McAuley, C & Davis, T (2009) 'The Emotional Well-Being of Looked After Children in England'. Child and Family Social Work, 14, 147-155. McDaniel, B., Braiden, H. J., Onyekwelu, J., Murphy, M., & Regan, H. (2011). Investigating the effectiveness of the incredible years basic parenting programme for foster carers in northern Ireland. Child Care in Practice, 17, 55-67. Meltzer, H, Gatward, R, Goodman, R and Ford, T. (2000). The mental health of children and adolescents in Great Britain: summary report. London: The Stationery Office. Meltzer, H., Gatward, R., Corbin, T., Goodman, R., Ford, T. (2003). The mental health of young people looked after by local authorities in England. London: The Stationary Office. Meltzer, H., Lader, D., Corbin, T., Goodman, R. and Ford, T. (2004a). The mental health of young people looked after by local authorities in Scotland. Edinburgh: The Stationery Office. Meltzer, H., Lader, D., Corbin, T., Goodman, R., Ford, T. (2004b). The mental health of young people looked after by local authorities in Wales. London: The Stationary Office. 199 Moore, K. J., & Chamberlain, P. (1994). Treatment foster care: Toward development of community-based models for adolescents with severe emotional and behavioral disorders. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 2, 22-30. National Institute of Clinical Excellence. (2001). The guideline development process – Information for national collaborating centres and guideline development groups. London: NICE. Retrieved at: http://www.nice.org.uk Pallett C, Scott S, Blackeby K, Yule W and Weissman R (2002) Fostering changes: a cognitive-behavioural approach to help foster carers manage children. Adoption and fostering, 26, 39-48. Sen, R., & Broadhurst, K. (2011). Contact between children in out of home placements and their family and friends networks: A research review. Child & Family Social Work, 16, 298309. Staines, J., Farmer, E., & Selwyn, J. (2011). Implementing a therapeutic team parenting approach to fostering: The experiences of one independent foster-care agency. British Journal of Social Work, 41, 314-332. Tapsfield, R., & Collier, F. (2005). The cost of foster care: investing in our children's future. London: British Agencies for Adoption and Fostering/ Fostering Network. 200 Thoburn, J. (2007). Globalisation and child welfare: Some lessons from a cross-national study of children in out-of-home care. Norwich: UEA Social Work Monographs. Wakelyn, J. (2008). Transitional psychotherapy for looked-after children in 'short-term' foster care. Journal of Social Work Practice, 22, 27-36. Ward, H., Holmes, L., Soper, J., & Olsen, R. (2008). Costs and consequences of placing children in care. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. World Health Organisation. (1992). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (ICD-10), 10th revision. Geneva: Author. 201 Overview of Clinical Experience Adult Mental Health This placement was based in an Adult Community Mental Health Team. The main model of the placement was Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy (CBT), with some DBT (Dialectical Behavioural therapy) and Schema therapy. I used these approaches to undertake individual work with a range of individuals with a variety of difficulties including depression, bi-polar, personality disorders and a range of anxiety disorders including Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD), hoarding, generalised anxiety, social anxiety and post-natal depression. I also undertook joint work with social workers and community psychiatric nurses (CPNs) doing in-home assessments and interventions as well as joint assessments with the local substance abuse team. I co-facilitated a CBT group for clients with depression and evaluated this to inform service developments. The neuropsychological assessments explored queried Learning Disabilities and brain injuries. Older People's Mental Health This placement was based within an Older People's Community Mental Health Team and Older People's memory service . I undertook cognitive assessments of older adults with suspected memory loss, to help diagnose the presence dementia, including specifying the potential type of dementia. This developed my knowledge of dementia profiles as well as the cognitive changes associated with older age and expanded my skills in neuropsychological assessment tests. I co-ran a support group for individuals newly diagnosed with dementia and 202 their carers and I undertook training for professionals managing behaviours that challenge in individuals with dementia. I undertook individual therapeutic work with older adults with depression, anxiety, bereavement difficulties, and personality disorders, often alongside physical health conditions. Child and Adolescent Mental Health This placement was based in a Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service (CAMHS) where the main approach used by my supervisor was an integrative approach. Due to the nature of the work, there was a high emphasis on a systemic approach, with elements of CBT, narrative and psychodynamic elements. I worked with young people and their families who were experiencing difficulties with depression, generalised anxiety, agoraphobia, PTSD, eating difficulties, histories of sexual assault and sexual abuse, OCD, psychosis, ASD, ADHD and Learning Disabilities. These cases expanded my experience of assessment and managing risk, utilising positive risk management strategies and developed my working knowledge of Child Protection procedures. I also undertook cognitive assessments of young people with queried learning disabilities to inform future support as well as therapeutic input. I also participated in ASD assessments and the family therapy reflecting team. Learning Disabilities This placement was based in a Community Team for Adults with Learning Disabilities. It offered variety in relation to individual work, family group and consultation to staff working in residential homes. Part of this work included functional assessments to inform behaviour 203 plans and staff training as well as CBT for depression, narrative work and complex grief work. I worked closely with staff and families to carry out assessments with clients who were referred to psychology for difficulties living with other residents and challenging behaviour. Whilst on this placement, I was involved in safeguarding meetings and risk assessments, researching specific behaviours and associated risks of these alongside issues of capacity. I also provided consultation to staff in relation to managing safeguarding behaviours in residential homes in the context of limited resources. During this placement I undertook cognitive assessments for dementia with clients with Down Syndrome, as well as with a young lady with a queried Learning Disability and ASD. Advanced competencies This placement was based in a Paediatric Psychology team within a large hospital. This service offers Psychological support to children and their families and medical staff. The predominant models of this placement were systemic, narrative, behavioural models and CBT as assessments and therapy took into account the impact of medical conditions on all the areas of the young people's and their families' lives. This placement expanded my understanding of the contribution of Psychology in a predominantly medical setting. This service exists within a very diverse community of staff and clients and provided me the opportunity to develop my skills in working through interpreters. There was a high level of liaison between medical staff and different medical teams, schools, education services, social services and community medical teams. This expanded my knowledge and skills of care coordinating but also developed my awareness of the roles and functions of a wider range of 204 child focussed services. 205 Academic Assessments Year I Assessments PROGRAMME COMPONENT TITLE OF ASSIGNMENT Fundamentals of Theory and Practice in Clinical Psychology (FTPCP) Short report of WAIS-III data and practice administration Research –SRRP Impact on symptoms of depression of a CBT for depression group Practice case report Cognitive behavioural therapy with a middle-aged man presenting with obsessive-compulsive disorder Problem Based Learning – Reflective Account 'Relationship to change' Research – Literature Review UK evidence for interventions to improve the psychological wellbeing of foster children: A systematic review Adult – case report Assessment, cognitive behavioural formulation and therapy with a man in his thirties presenting with severe depression and post-traumatic stress disorder Adult – case report Cognitive behavioural therapy with a woman in her late twenties presenting with anxiety and depression. Research – Qualitative Research Project Trainees’ perceptions and experiences of selfdisclosure in a therapeutic Setting Research – Major Research Project Proposal Impact of disrupted placements on the well-being of children who are accommodated 206 Year II Assessments PROGRAMME COMPONENT TITLE OF ASSESSMENT Research Research methods and statistics test Professional Issues Essay How do we understand the context of mental health, mental illness and mental wellbeing when people are experiencing material poverty? What psychological frameworks do we have to offer such people? Problem Based Learning – Reflective Account Child protection, domestic abuse, parenting and learning disabilities and kinship care Older People – Case Report Neuropsychological assessment for a gentleman in his eighties experiencing some mild memory difficulties Personal and Professional Learning Discussion Groups – Process Account Personal and Professional Learning Discussion Groups Process Account Child and Family– Oral Presentation of Clinical Activity Positive risk management with a mid-teenage girl Year III Assessments PROGRAMME COMPONENT Research – MRP Portfolio ASSESSMENT TITLE Young people’s perspectives of foster placement instability: A grounded theory approach 207 Personal and Professional Learning – Final Reflective Account On becoming a clinical psychologist: A retrospective, developmental, reflective account of the experience of training Specialist – Case Report Integrative therapy with a young girl with chronic constipation and low self-esteem End of Portfolio. 208