1 Motion-triggered defensive display in a Tephritid fly 2 1,3Samuel Aguilar Argüello, 1Francisco Díaz-Fleischer, 1,2Dinesh Rao 3 4 1Inbioteca 5 Universidad Veracruzana, 6 Av. Culturas Veracruzanas No.101, 7 Col. E. Zapata, CP 91090, 8 Xalapa, Veracruz, México 9 10 3Current address, Instituto de Ecología, A.C, 11 Apartado Postal 63, CP 91000 12 Xalapa, Veracruz, México 13 14 2Author for Correspondence; dinrao@gmail.com 15 1 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 ABSTRACT Interactions between prey and predators are often mediated by signals sent by the prey. Passive signals such as aposematic colouration, and active signals such as pursuit deterrence signals are thought to prevent attack from predators. In true fruit flies (Diptera: Tephritidae), the defensive wing display is called supination, and studies have shown that supination effectively reduces the chance of being attacked by salticid predators. In this study, we investigated the proximal causes of supination in staged interactions in an arena. We asked whether the movement of the display target influences the likelihood of triggering supination in the Mexican fruit fly Anastrepha ludens. We tested the effect of motion on fly display in three different ways using 1. a manually moved dead spider or beetle, 2. live bouts with a spider and a katydid, and 3. video playback experiments where movement of the display target was controlled. Our results show that flies are more likely to perform supination when the display target moves. The identity of the display target did not influence display propensity suggesting that the supination of flies is a generalized display behaviour against any possible threat. 32 33 Keywords: supination, jumping spiders, aggressive behaviour, Anastrepha ludens 34 35 2 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 INTRODUCTION Interactions between prey and predators are often mediated by signals sent by the prey. These signals can be passive (e.g., aposematic colouration, (Bond 2007)) or active (e.g., honest signals, (Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997)). In active signalling, there may also be an element of deception, wherein a prey can send misleading information to the predator. Prey can also signal to predators to interrupt an attack (e.g., startle displays, (Ruxton et al. 2004)) or to prevent the launch of an attack (e.g., pursuit deterrence, (Caro, 1995)). By signalling the relative difficulty in prey capture, the prey benefit by thwarting an attack, and the predator can benefit by conserving energy and redirecting its attack towards a facile prey. Active signalling to avoid predation has been documented in a taxonomically wide variety of animals such as tail-wagging in mot mots (Murphy 2006), tail flagging in ground squirrels (Rundus et al. 2007), inspection behaviour in guppies (Godin and Davis 1995) and the shimmering behaviour of the Asian hive bee (Tan et al. 2012). Interactions between jumping spiders (Araneae: Salticidae) and the true fruit flies (Diptera: Tephritidae) are an example of a system where a prey signals to the predator and successfully avoids attack. In this system, flies perform a wing display called supination, where the fly brings the wings forward, perpendicular to the long axis of the body, while the ventral surface of the wing is turned to face anterior of the fly such that the costal margin of the wing is dorsal (Headrick and Goeden 1994, Supplementary Video S1). Supination can be asynchronous or synchronous, i.e., it can occur with both wings simultaneously or sequentially (Headrick and Goeden 1994). This display is common in both male and female flies and has been observed during conspecific interactions (Briceno et al. 1999; Headrick and Goeden 1994, Benelli 2013, 2015, Benelli et al. 2014). Supination has been found effective in preventing an attack in 4 species of flies against up to 25 species of salticids (Greene et al. 1987; Hasson 1995; Mather and Roitberg 1987; Rao and Díaz-Fleischer 2012). The signalling is thought to be deceptive in function, since the flies have bands on their wings which, when viewed from a certain angle, mimics the leg patterns of the spiders (Eisner 1985). The display of the flies may mimic the courtship or aggressive displays of jumping spiders. This hypothesis, termed the predator mimicry hypothesis because the flies purportedly mimic their predators, has been invoked to explain the functional significance of these displays (Greene et al. 1987; Mather and Roitberg 1987). However, there are unresolved questions with respect to the hypothesis. Firstly, salticids are known to kill other salticids (Jackson 1977), 3 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 and the mere spider-like appearance of a fly is not enough to grant it complete immunity from attack. However, the spider-like appearance may contribute to confusion on the part of the spider and thus allow sufficient time for the fly to escape, akin to evasive mimicry or imperfect mimicry (Ruxton et al. 2004). Secondly, the display is successful against a range of salticid species, even though salticid displays are highly species specific (Elias, Land, Mason, and Hoy 2006). Thirdly, though most true fruit flies have banded wings (Sivinski and Pereira 2005), the display is seen even in species that are lightly banded. In previous experiments, we established that the appearance of the bands was not as important as the display itself in order to prevent an attack (Rao and Díaz-Fleischer 2012). Why do the flies display to predators? The display was found to be ineffective against non-salticid predators such as preying mantids, lizards and assassin bugs (Greene et al. 1987). The display is also used in aggressive interactions with conspecifics. Therefore this display may be a reaction to the presence of any potential aggressor, and not necessarily directed to a specific predator such as a spider. In this study, we sought to investigate the proximate causes of supination in the tephritid fly Anastrepha ludens. In particular, we hypothesised that the display is triggered by the motion of the display-target rather than its identity. If motion is the proximate trigger for the supination display, then the fly should display regardless of the source (spider or control) or type of the motion (manually moving objects, live animals or video playback). 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 Methods In this study we used the large jumping spider, Phidippus audax (Araneae: Salticidae), which is distributed all across North America (Edwards 2004), and frequently found in citrus orchards, where it is likely to encounter tephritid fruit flies. Female spiders (mean body length: 10.93 mm) are bigger than males (mean body length: 8.39 mm) (Edwards 2004). The abdomen is generally black with a white spot, though there is some variation in colour in this species (Edwards 2004). Spiders were collected from an abandoned maize plantation on the outskirts of Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico. They were brought to the laboratory of the Inbioteca campus of the Universidad Veracruzana in Xalapa and housed individually in small plastic containers (7 cm diameter x 5 cm height). Spiders were fed grasshoppers weekly and watered every three days. Mass-reared Anastrepha ludens flies were obtained from the MoscaFrut plant in Metapa de Dominguez, Chiapas. Flies were 4 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 acquired as pupae and were allowed to emerge in wooden cages (30 x 30 x 30 cm) covered in mesh cloth within the laboratory. Flies were fed yeast hydrolysate and sugar (proportion 1:3) ad libitum. There is sexual dimorphism in Anastrepha ludens flies since female flies (total body length; mean ± std. dev.: 9.99 ± 0.46 mm) are larger than male flies (total body length; mean ± std. dev.: 7.40 ± 0.49 mm), and can be distinguished by the presence of an ovipositor (Tejeda et al. 2014). Experimental Design All experiments were carried out in the laboratory under natural light conditions from 10 am to 4 pm. Flies were chosen randomly from a holding cage for each experiment and each fly was used only once. Flies were not mated. In all experiments, flies were introduced into the test arena first and allowed to acclimatise for 1 min. All experiments were recorded with a Sony HDR-XR260 video camera from above. Experiment 1: Manual movement In this experiment, we tested whether male and female flies (n = 60) would respond to movement. One spider and one beetle were used for this experiment. The animals were anaesthetized with CO2 and subsequently frozen. We used a dead female spider and mounted it with wax on a small circular disc on a wooden platform. The disc was moved by means of a lever placed at the base of the platform. The arena consisted of a petri dish (9 cm diameter and 1.3 cm height). As a control, we used a dead beetle (Calosoma sp.; Coleoptera: Carabidae) of similar size and colour as the spider. Each interaction trial lasted for 3 min. There were two treatments: Moving and Still Treatments. For the Moving treatments, the disc was rapidly rotated when the fly was facing the spider or the beetle. During the Still treatment, the disc was not moved. The models were rotated only when the fly was facing it, extending to an angle approximately 45 degrees on either side. We imported the video clips to an Apple iMac computer and recorded the following variables: presence or absence of displays, rate of display, bout duration (time from initiation of interaction to outcome) and proportion of flies that displayed. We analysed the data with a full factorial Generalized Linear Models, with link functions according to the distribution of the response variable. The p values of the whole model correspond to the comparison between the model to the model that contains only the intercept parameter. Analysis was carried out in JMP v9. Experiment 2: Live bouts 5 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 In this experiment, male (n = 62) and female (n = 58) flies were placed into a plastic petri dish (14 cm diameter, 2.5 cm height). The petri dish was divided into 2 partitions, separated by an opaque divider. Flies were introduced first into one of the partitions and the order of introduction (i.e. left or right) was randomised. Spiders or a control (a katydid) were introduced into the opposite partition (Supplementary Video S1,2). The katydid was chosen as a control due to its easily quantifiable sudden movements (pers. obs, Rao. D.). Both animals were allowed to acclimatize for a minute. Once the trial started, the divider was removed, and the interaction was filmed from above. The following variables were recorded: presence or absence of displays, number of displays, presence or absence of movement of the animals (we considered only movements that involved a change in position, i.e. we did not include movement of body parts). From the video clips we recorded: bout duration, the distance at which the fly initiated display (Display Initiation Distance) and the distance at which the fly escaped or walked away (Flight Initiation Distance). Distances were measured using the software GraphClick Ver 3.0. We analysed the data with full factorial Generalized Linear Models, with link functions according to the distribution of the response variable. The p values of the whole model correspond to the comparison between the model to the model that contains only the intercept parameter. Distance data were analysed using a standard least squares fit model. We recorded the onset of spider or katydid movement (after approximately 5 seconds of inactivity), fly attention (defined as the moment when the fly detected the presence of the spider or katydid and moved to face the spider by positioning its body directly at the animal), display (when the fly finished a display cycle, defined as from one outstretched wing pose to another outstretched wing pose), end of fly attention and end of movement. These variables were analysed using the event recording software JWatcher Ver 1.0 (Blumstein and Daniel, 2007). We performed an analysis of non-repeating sequences to test whether a given behaviour was more likely to be followed by another. We computed transitional probabilities for two key behaviour sequences: movement followed by fly attention, and fly attention followed by display. To test whether these probabilities were significant, we obtained the Z-scores (adjusted residuals) of the sequence analysis. These Z-scores were calculated using a log-linear analysis for data with structural zeros using the software iLog ver 4.0 (Bakeman and Robinson 1994). Z-scores greater than 1.96 are considered significant at the 0.05 level (Bakeman and Gottman 1986). 6 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 Experiment 3: Video playback We recorded short clips of spider and katydid movement and configured these clips to play on an Apple iPod Touch (Supplementary Video S3). As a control we used a still image of a spider or a katydid. The iPod was integrated into one wall of an arena (15 x 15 x 7 cm). Male and female flies (n = 60) were introduced into the arena and the interactions were filmed from above. The trials lasted 3 min. We recorded the number of displays, bout duration, fly attention (while facing the screen) and the onset of these behaviours. Data Analysis We analysed the data with a full factorial Generalized Linear Models, with link functions according to the distribution of the response variable (Crawley, 1993; Agresti, 2007). Count data were analysed using a generalised linear model (GLM) with Poisson errors, a log-link function and type III significance tests. For binary data, we used binomial errors and logit-link function and type III significance tests. The p values of the whole model correspond to the comparison between the whole model and the model that contains only the intercept parameter. Contrasts were used to test for differences in levels within variables. 192 193 194 RESULTS 195 Experiment 1: Manual movement 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 Fly display (presence or absence) was significantly triggered by the factors (i.e., movement, sex of the fly or the display target) in the model (GLM, binomial distribution, Logit Link: 2 = 20.32, df = 7, p = 0.005; Fig 1A). Of these factors, sex of the fly, movement and the interaction between display target and movement were significant (Table 1). Female flies (probability 0.90) displayed more than male flies (probability 0.79). Flies were more likely to display to a still beetle (mean probability of display: 0.85 ± 0.05) than to a moving beetle (mean probability of display: 0.59 ± 0.14), but this was not seen in the case of the spider (still: 0.66 ± 0.09; moving: 0.63 ± 0.13). Post hoc contrasts revealed that there were significant differences between movement and still treatments (2 = 6.6, p< 0.001) and no significant differences between beetle and spider (2 = 2.4, p = 0.12). There was a significant effect of the factors on bout duration (GLM, Poisson distribution, Log link, 2 = 35.73, df = 7, p < 0.0001). Of these factors, movement and 7 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 sex of the fly were significant (Table 1). There was also a significant effect of the interaction between the display target and the sex of the fly, as well as between movement and display target (Table 1). Female flies displayed for longer against beetles (Mean ± S.D.; 6.1 ± 3.5 s;) than against spiders (Mean ± S.D.; 4.15 ± 1.08 s), Male flies displayed for longer against spiders (Mean ± S.D.; 7.01 ± 0.08 s) than against beetles (Mean ± S.D.; 6.45 ± 2.25 s). However, neither was significant in post-hoc contrast analysis. Post-hoc contrast analysis showed that flies displayed for significantly longer to beetles when there was no movement (Mean ± S.D.; Still: 8.3 ± 0.38 s; Moving: 4.23 ± 0.86s, 2 = 18.50, p< 0.0001), while they displayed for similar durations to spiders irrespective of movement (Mean ± S.D.; Still: 5.99 ± 1.52 s; Moving: 5.17 ± 2.52s, 2 = 1.97, p = 0.16). There was no significant effect of the three factors on display rate (GLM, Poisson distribution, Log link, Χ2= 1.1, df = 7, p = 0.99). Experiment 2: Live bouts During live bouts, flies did not significantly differ in their propensity to display (GLM, binomial distribution, Logit Link: 2 = 2.56, df = 3, p = 0.463); and this pattern was seen whether the display target was a spider or a katydid or whether the fly was male or female (Table 2). There was a significant effect of the factors on bout duration (GLM, Poisson distribution, Identity Link: 2 = 29.01, df = 3, p < 0.0001). For bout duration, both the display target (Katydids: Mean ± S.D. = 14.14s ± 12.01; Spiders: Mean ± S.D. = 11.68s ± 8.66) and the sex of the fly (Males: Mean ± S.D. = 14.61s ± 11.79; Females: Mean ± S.D. = 11.27s ± 8.79; Table 2) were significant. There was no significant effect of display target or sex of the fly on the rate of display (GLM, poisson distribution, Log Link: 2 = 0.908, df = 3, p = 0.823; Table 2). Display Initiation Distance (Least squares fit, R2 = 0.06, F 3, 76, p = 0.202) and the Flight Initiation Distance (Least squares fit, R2 = 0.06, F 3, 76, p = 0.158) were not significantly different according to whether the flies displayed to a spider or a katydid. Flies were significantly more likely to display when the spider or katydid moved. The relevant transitions of behaviour along with the Z-scores are presented in Fig. 2. Experiment 3: Video playback 8 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 The likelihood of display was influenced by the factors (GLM, binomial distribution, Logit Link: 2 = 54.71, df = 7, p < 0.0001). Of the three factors, i.e. sex of the fly, movement or identity of display target, only movement significantly influenced presence of display (Table 3). Flies were more likely to display to a moving video clip (Fig. 1B). There was no significant effect of sex of the fly or the display target or the interactions (Table 3). Bout duration was significantly influenced by the factors (GLM, Poisson distribution, Log link, 2 = 359, df = 7, p < 0.0001). Of the factors, both the display target and movement influenced the length of the bouts (Table 3). All interactions were significant (Table 3). Post hoc contrasts revealed that there were significant differences between movement and still treatments (2 = 114.56, p< 0.001). Bouts by female flies were longer against katydids and spiders when there was movement (predicted value: 34.27 s). On the other hand, male flies displayed for longer against katydids when there was movement (predicted value: 31.06 s), but not against spiders (predicted value: 15.33 s). The rate of display was not significantly influenced by the factors (GLM, Gaussian distribution, Identity Link: 2 = 1.98, df = 7, p = 0.961; Table 3). 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 DISCUSSION In all three experiments, fly displays were triggered by the motion of the display target, irrespective of the type of motion — manual, live or video playback. Our results suggest that rather than being a predator-directed display (Greene et al. 1987), supination in A. ludens is a generalized display against any potential threat and is primarily triggered by motion. A potential drawback of our methodology lies in the fact that we did not use multiple examples of the target in experiments 1 and 3. There is the possibility that the spiders were responding to the target individual in question and not the movement per se. However, since the treatments are very distinct (i.e. still and movement), we can consider the response of the fly as representative. Further studies should incorporate several exemplars in order to resolve this issue. Of all the display parameters measured, the likelihood to perform display was influenced by movement across the three experiments. However, other parameters 9 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 such as bout duration and rate of display were also significant. In terms of interactions between factors, from the manual movement and video playback experiments, it was apparent that male and female flies do not respond similarly to the stimulus, which is understandable since they have different needs and motivations. In A. ludens, there is a lek mating system where courting males guard non-resource territories and wait for the approach of females. Aggression between males is more likely at this stage; whereas between females aggression may be more likely over oviposition locations (Benelli 2015). From the fly point of view, the display seems to have various functions. The primary use is against conspecifics in aggressive encounters (Headrick and Goeden 1994). Male flies could use this encounter to evaluate opponents in contests, as seen in the displays of the stalk-eyed flies (Worthington and Swallow 2010). Furthermore, the looping side-to-side movement (described in Rao and DíazFleischer 2012) could also serve as a mechanism where the fly integrates sufficient visual information in order to make a decision regarding the threat level of the opponent. By observing the target from various angles, the fly could increase the visual information available to it. It is conceivable that the fly fails to recognise the jumping spider as a threat until it accumulates sufficient visual input. In the manual movement experiment, flies displayed even to spiders in the non-moving treatment, suggesting that movement is not the sole trigger for performing a display, but perhaps is the dominant component. If we consider that bout duration is a proxy for time needed to ‘recognize’ the identity of the opponent, flies vary in detection ability based on the stimulus and the context. These ideas need to be tested further in more detail. A multi-stage anti-predator behaviour would help flies to reduce the risk of predation but also to increase the quality of information with regards to the actual risk as seen in fiddler crabs Uca vomeris McNeil (Hemmi and Pfeil 2013). When displaying, flies may collect qualitatively different visual information during successive stages of their display sequence. Furthermore, since displays are usually linked to aggression in various species of Tephritidae (Benelli 2015), they may contain an additional function of deterrence. In this scenario, the trigger for acquisition of information is the movement of the opposing organism. In earlier experiments with this species, we established that the deterrence effect of supination against salticid predators was largely due to the display itself rather 10 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 than the appearance of the wings or the body condition of the fly (Rao and DíazFleischer 2012). This result calls into doubt the predator mimicry hypotheses as an explanation for the interaction between salticids and tephritid flies. If the aim of the display is to ‘confuse’ the salticid into misidentifying the fly as another spider, then the fly by using the same display against various non-salticid predators (Greene et al. 1987), is hedging its bets that the main predator it will encounter will be salticids. While there is some evidence that salticids could be one of the main predators of another tephritid fly, Zonosemata vittigera (Greene et al. 1987), they are not the only predators of tephritid flies. On the other hand, by mimicking a salticid, flies could deter attack from potential salticid-averse predators. We suggest that the supination behaviour primarily arose from intraspecific aggressive interactions and the deterrence effect on salticids is a side effect of the visual biases of jumping spiders. 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank Diana Pérez-Staples, Ajay Narendra and anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. Thanks to Pablo Nuñez Berea for help in rearing the spiders and Dina Orozco (Subdirector of Production, MoscaFrut) for flies. We thank Quiyari Santiago for identifying the beetle. This project was funded by a Ciencia-Basica CONACyT grant (No. 168746) to DR. 330 331 11 332 333 334 TABLES Table 1: Summary of the statistics for experiment 1 with manual movement. Significant p values are in bold. Display Bout Duration Display Rate Χ² p Χ² p Χ² p Display Target 2.26 0.13 0.81 0.37 0.21 0.64 Sex of the fly 6.6 0.01 9.38 0.002 0.2 0.65 Movement 6.56 0.01 16.24 < 0.0001 0.02 0.89 Display Target * Sex of the fly 0.01 0.91 3.91 0.04 0.001 0.97 Sex of the fly * Movement 0.09 0.75 2.74 0.09 0.15 0.69 Display Target * Movement 5.08 0.02 5.21 0.02 0.02 0.86 Display Target *Sex * Movement 0.68 0.41 0.01 0.91 0.64 0.42 Variable 335 336 337 Table 2: Summary of the statistics for experiment 2 with live bouts. Significant p values are in bold. Display Variable Χ² Bout Duration p Display Target 0.46 0.49 Sex of the fly 2.06 0.15 Display Target * Sex of the fly 1.63 0.99 Χ² p 8.00 0.0047 15.95 < 0.001 3.58 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 12 0.058 Display Rate Χ² p 0.11 0.74 0.39 0.53 0.57 0.44 347 348 Table 3: Summary of the statistics for experiment 3 with video playback. Significant p values are in bold. 349 Display Bout Duration Χ² p Χ² Display Target 0.36 0.54 40.53 Sex of the fly 1.15 0.28 2.72 Movement 52.94 < 0.001 Display Target * Sex of the fly 0.12 Sex of the fly * Movement Variable p Display Rate Χ² < 0.0001 0.00008 0.99 0.06 0.79 114.56 < 0.0001 1.19 0.27 0.72 12.05 0.0005 0.02 0.88 0.76 0.38 11.28 0.0008 0.04 0.83 Display Target * Movement 0.16 0.68 9.33 0.0022 0.12 0.72 Display Target *Sex * Movement 0.37 0.54 70.24 < 0.0001 0.34 0.55 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 13 0.09 p 364 FIGURES 365 366 367 368 Figure 1A: The numbers of flies displaying were significantly different according to movement. See Table 1 for details. 369 370 371 Figure 1B: The number of displays of the flies was significantly different according to movement, but not between the spider and the katydid. See Table 3 for details. 372 373 374 375 Figure 2: Ethograms of fly response to (A) spiders and (B) katydids during live bouts. Numbers on the arrows represent the transitional probabilities with Z-scores in parentheses. Negative Z-scores imply that the values are below the mean. 376 377 14 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 Supplementary Material S1 Video demonstrating the defensive display of the tephritid fly Anastrepha ludens against a jumping spider Phidippus audax S2 Video demonstrating the display of the tephritid fly Anastrepha ludens against the katydid. Note the initiation of the display when the katydid moves. S3 Video demonstrating the display of the fly against a moving spider on the playback video screen. 15 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 REFERENCES Agresti, A. (2007) An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis. New Jersey:Wiley– Interscience. Bakeman R, Gottman JM (1986) Observing interaction. An introduction to sequential analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Bakeman R, Robinson BF (1994) Understanding log-linear analysis with ILOG: An interactive approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Benelli G (2013) Aggressive behavior and territoriality in the Olive Fruit Fly, Bactrocera oleae (Rossi) (Diptera: Tephritidae): Role of residence and time of day. J Insect Behav 27:145-161 Benelli, G. (2015) Aggression in Tephritidae flies: Where, when, why? Future directions for research in Integrated Pest Management." Insects 6:38-53. Benelli G, Daane KM, Canale A, Niu C-Y, Messing RH, Vargas, RI (2014) Sexual communication and related behaviours in Tephritidae: current knowledge and potential applications for Integrated Pest Management. J Pest Sci 87:385405. Blumstein DT, Daniel JC (2007) Quantifying behavior the JWatcher way. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. Bond AB (2007) The evolution of color polymorphism: Crypticity, searching images, and apostatic selection. Ann Rev Ecol Sys 38:489-514 Briceno R, Ramos D, Eberhard W (1999) Aggressive behavior in medflies (Ceratitis capitata) and its modification by mass rearing (Diptera: Tephritidae). J Kans Ent Soc 1:17-27 Caro T (1995) Pursuit-deterrence revisited. TREE 10:500-503 Crawley R (1993) GLIM for ecologists. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Edwards GB (2004) Revision of the jumping spiders of the Genus Phidippus (Araneae: Salticidae). Occ Pap Fla Sta Coll Arth 11: 1-118 Eisner T (1985) A fly that mimics jumping spiders. Psyche 92: 103-104 Elias DO, Land BR, Mason AC, Hoy RR (2006) Measuring and quantifying dynamic visual signals in jumping spiders. J Comp Physiol A 192:785-797 Godin J, Davis S (1995) Who dares, benefits: Predator approach behaviour in the Guppy (Poecilia reticulata) deters predator pursuit. Proc Roy Soc B 259:193200. Greene E, Orsak L, Whitman D (1987) A Tephritid fly mimics the territorial displays of its jumping spider predators. Science 236:310-312. Hasson O (1995) A Fly in Spider's Clothing: What size the spider? Proc Roy Soc B 261:223-226. Headrick DH, Goeden RD (1994) Reproductive behavior of California fruit flies and the classification and evolution of Tephritidae (Diptera) mating systems. Stud Dipterol 1:194-252. Hemmi JH, Pfeil A (2010) A multi-stage anti-predator response increases information on predation risk. J Exp Biol 213: 1484-1489 16 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 Jackson RR (1977) Prey of the jumping spider Phidippus johnsoni (Araneae: Salticidae). J Arachnol 5:145-149. Jackson RR, Pollard SD (1996) Predatory behavior of jumping spiders. Ann Rev Entomol 41:287-308. Mather MH, Roitberg BD (1987) A sheep in wolf's clothing: Tephritid flies mimic spider predators. Science 236:308-310. Rao D, Díaz-Fleischer F (2012) Characterisation of predator-directed displays in Tephritid flies. Ethology 118:1165-1172. Rundus A, Owings D, Joshi S, Chinn E, Giannini N (2007) Ground squirrels use an infrared signal to deter rattlesnake predation. PNAS 104:14372-14376. Ruxton G, Sherratt TN, Speed MP (2004) Avoiding attack: The evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signs and mimicry. New York: Oxford University Press. Sivinski J, Pereira R (2005) Do wing markings in fruit flies (Diptera: Tephritidae) have sexual significance? Fla Entomol 88:321-324. Spano L, Long SM, Jakob EM (2012) Secondary eyes mediate the response to looming objects in jumping spiders (Phidippus audax, Salticidae). Biol Lett 8:949-951. Tan K, Wang Z, Li H, Yang S, Hu Z, et al. (2012) An ‘I see you’ prey–predator signal between the Asian honeybee, Apis cerana, and the hornet, Vespa velutina. Anim Behav 83:879-882. Tejeda MT, Arredondo J, Pérez-Staples DF, Ramos-Morales P, Liedo P, Díaz-Fleischer F. (2014) Effects of size, sex and teneral resources on the resistance to hydric stress in the tephritid fruit fly Anastrepha ludens. J Ins Physiol 70: 73-80 Worthington AM, Swallow JG (2010) Gender differences in survival and antipredatory behavior in stalk-eyed flies. Behav Ecol 21:759-766. Zahavi A, Zahavi A (1997) The handicap principle. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 453 17