The Independent Review of Early Intervention Delivery Graham Allen MP is chairing an independent review of how early intervention projects can improve the lives of our most vulnerable children. We have responded from the BCRP. The questions and our responses are set out below About you This response is from the research team leading the Better Communication Research Programme (BCRP) funded by the Department for Education (2009-12) as part of the Better Communication Action Plan, the government’s response to the Bercow Review of services for children with speech, language and communication needs (SLCN) 1 published in 2008. The BCRP team is led Professors Geoff Lindsay, University of Warwick; Julie Dockrell, Institute of Education, University of London; James Law University of Newcastle and Sue Roulstone University of the West of England. Who has SLCN? SLCN refers to a wide range of conditions affecting any aspect of communication from children with poor speech at one end, to children with specific learning difficulties, those with more general learning disabilities and on to those with complex needs of which SLCN is a part. Children with autism also fall within this group because communication is a feature of their profile. The BCRP focuses on the child with SLCN and the additional problems experienced by some of the children including literacy, behaviour and academic performance. We would argue that the needs of the child with SLCN fall fairly and squarely within the remit of this review given the impact of poor communication on learning and socio-emotional development. The BCRP included five specific projects in its first year of which one focuses on the evidence for best practice both at a research and a practitioner level and a second will be making recommendations about economic evaluations. Questions - Early Intervention 1. What are the likely causes of impairments to children’s social and emotional capability? And how common are they across the population? Of the order of 10% of children across the population have some sort of SLCN. This is higher in early childhood than it is in adolescence and much higher in children from areas of high social disadvantage. Some forms of SLCN may be hereditary although the precise mechanisms are complex and underspecified in many cases. They may be acquired in the sense that they can result from medical conditions such a cleft palate or from trauma such as road traffic accidents or illness such as meningitis. They may also be exacerbated by and in extreme cases caused by environmental factors. The key issue is that they co-occur with other difficulties that children experience and can affect the child’s ability to respond to interventions and services: for example children with poor language skills are less likely to respond to reading interventions. The developmental pathways are complex. There are some children who start at a disadvantage and manage to achieve well and similarly there are those who become more disadvantaged over time. We know that good parenting and supportive neighbourhoods can make a huge difference and likewise schools can play a considerable part in raising children’s aspirations and changing predicted trajectories. 1 Bercow, J. (2008) A review of services for children and young people (0-19) with speech, language and communication needs. 2. Do we know how to improve children’s social and emotional capabilities in a cost-effect way? At least 58 relatively high quality effectiveness studies have been carried out with regard to children with SLCN. These have tended to be carried out with younger children and often show positive responses for speech and expressive language. Six studies have examined the cost effectiveness of intervention related to SLCN. Both of these sets of studies have been reviewed as a part of the BCRP (details are available). Findings suggest that the costs of intervention as currently delivered can be quite low and given the outcomes from many studies it is likely that many interventions will be cost effective. The strongest message is that parents play a considerable role in both improving outcomes and reducing costs for younger children. We are currently examining the relationship between such studies and what actually goes on in practice. 3. If we know how to improve children’s capabilities in a cost effective way, why are we not doing so? What is the split between universal schemes and specific schemes? We don’t know that we are not doing so. In fact the demand for such services appears to be increasing suggesting that consumers, at least, favour intervention. That said, studies which show that an intervention works are often carried out in relatively favourable conditions – ie. with enough staff and over a long enough period of time (this shows efficacy). In practice children often receive fewer services than those available in the intervention studies (effectiveness). Again we are exploring the issue of dosage in the BCRP but it is by no means clear that children necessarily need more support services. What they need is the right services at the right time. A single universal programme will not necessarily be the most appropriate answer; we need to understand what are the key mechanisms of effective programmes in order to be able to replicate effectively in different contexts. A programme of research closely related to the BCRP has been funded by the National Institute of Health Research to investigate this and begins in January 2011. More information about this is available from Sue Roulstone. 4. What are the patterns of income and expenditure for late versus early intervention in general and are there proven rates of return for specific schemes? It is often assumed that early intervention is better than later intervention. It is an appealing claim and it is certainly the case that early intervention is effective in ameliorating the potentially negative consequences of a number of adverse factors, whether within-child (e.g. certain developmental conditions) or environmental. But considerable cognitive changes occur during adolescence and interventions often aim to address these factors. Also, whereas there have been studies arising out of the US Headstart programme that have demonstrated substantial long term outcomes in terms of societal outcomes, these interventions reflect a model and intensity of early intervention which has never been available in the UK. We simply do not know that early intervention prevents later difficulties: true in some cases but in others this is best viewed as amelioration – important in itself – whereas in other cases the continuing adverse life circumstances of the child may lead to ‘wash out’ of the earlier gains. Indeed the equivocal nature of the evaluation of Sure Start demonstrates this complexity. We would argue that there is an important role for early intervention but also that services need to be responsive to the needs of the children throughout the age range, particularly for those with continuing high level needs. It is also important to consider the method of identification of those in need of early intervention. Substantial evidence indicates that this is more problematic than may be assumed. Briefly, there are highly effective screening procedures for some conditions which allow early identification to be targeted efficiently. However, as we move away from conditions with clear biological causes (e.g. profound hearing loss) to less severe developmental difficulties, the accuracy of screening methods for identifying children is considerably reduced. This is particularly the case with children with lower levels of language difficulties. The important message here is that early identification should be conceptualised as a more elaborate system and not simply a series of screening procedures. Again we can provide more information. 5. What lessons can previous experience teach us about what doesn’t work? What programmes have proved ineffective? What characteristics associated with previous programmes are ineffective? What other aspects of early intervention are ineffective? Professional development provided in inset alone, that is without appropriate development of skills and mentoring is minimally effective Evidence based universal pre-school or school provision reduces the need for specialist services, although there is currently limited evidence of what works in the educational context. This leaves those children with significant difficulties who require additional interventions and continuing targeted or specialist interventions. We do not know how little intervention is needed to have an effect (the ‘dosage’ issue) and clearly further work is needed on this type of threshold. These issues are important as the less intervention an individual or family receives the less they are likely to respond but, on the other hand, there may be diminishing returns - more is not necessarily better beyond a certain amount. But there is also the question of delivery not just absolute quantity. There is a question whether intervention is better for children if provided intensively or in small doses less frequently. There is strong research evidence that distributed practice is more effective than the same amount in larger and less frequent sessions. This will work more readily in schools where such a model is feasible but is less easy to deliver in “clinical” contexts where practitioners see the children, for example, on a weekly basis This speaks to the benefit of speech and language therapists working with school, staff. 6. Are there interventions with a robust international evidence base that have been effectively applied in the UK? We are particularly interested in evidence which demonstrates both the effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of interventions. Programmes which can clearly and unambiguously demonstrate measurable benefits, which have a cashable value, will be particularly helpful. There are a number of interventions which would appear to have robust evidence but these have not been replicated either nationally or internationally. We can forward these to the panel if required 7. What are the common characteristics and processes which facilitate effective and cost-effective early intervention policy? To date our sense is that some of the policies in this field are neither evidence-informed nor evidence-based – Every child a talker is an exception. It would be helpful if policy makers were more engaged in the evidence. This consultation may be a step in the right direction. 8. Are there promising programmes that have yet to be properly evaluated? What are the future evaluation plans and over what timescale might we expect results? There are a number of interventions which look promising and have been piloted but which have yet to be evaluated for their effectiveness, in sizeable populations, as opposed to their efficacy under optimal conditions. Some of these are documented on the What Works Clearing House. For example, there are a number of high quality intervention studies being out in Melbourne Australia which are likely to have an impact in the future (3-5 years). Similarly the Nuffield Foundation has funded three trials of relevant interventions which are currently being written up. As far as we are aware there are no plans to roll these out. It cannot simply be assumed that an intervention carried out in optimal conditions of a research trial, which is shown to have high efficacy, will be effective when rolled out on a large scale, implemented by staff less directly involved in the original trial(s). For example, a recent meta analysis of the effect of book reading on vocabulary learning highlighted the fact that the robust findings with experimental studies were simply not replicable in a natural classroom setting. The trials are essential to provide basic evidence but we also need studies of the subsequent roll out on a medium to large scale. A good example of this is the roll out across all English LAs of evidence-based parenting programmes for children at risk of behavioural problems (The Parenting Early Intervention Programme). This is currently being evaluated by one of this team (Lindsay) in a separate DfE funded study – interim findings are positive, indicating that large scale roll out of evidence-based programmes can produce positive results. Funding to promote translational research or knowledge transfer in this field could facilitate this important stage in the development of evidence based practice. 9. What could be done to test and promote these ideas nationally? How should we best communicate best practice? While it would be possible for central government to try to regulate current services care needs to be taken not to assume that there is a simple solution. Intervention of this type does not work like a pill and is only likely to function effectively in the right policy environment. As noted in 8, it is necessary i) to run trials of theoretically driven, practical and acceptable interventions to identify efficacious interventions under optimal conditions; ii) then to evaluate the large scale roll out of interventions which show efficacy in order to ensure they are effective when delivered on this large scale, and the conditions under which this occurs, e.g. the necessary organisational factors at local level. 10. What could be done to nurture and develop ideas in the field of early intervention? Is there a role for a central body to test, approve and promote policy in this field? Are there organisations that have some or all of these functions already? Ideas about early intervention across the public, charitable and research contexts have a long history. Many of these rely heavily on untested assumptions. The key issue is the proper controlled evaluation of programmes. There is a stronger case for government funding for the evaluation of such programmes and promulgating good practice rather than acting as a regulatory authority in this field. 11. What new models of financing early intervention, or wider social policy, exist? This does not fall within the remit of the BCRP 12. What other instruments could be introduced to diversify funding of early intervention? This does not fall within the remit of the BCRP 13. What could be done to nurture and develop these financing ideas? This does not fall within the remit of the BCRP 14. What must government do and not do to enable non-government financing to assist Early Intervention? This does not fall within the remit of the BCRP