The Contribution of Hot and Cool EF to Pre-Readers’ Narrative Comprehension and Later Reading Comprehension in Elementary School Nicole R. Guajardo and Kelly B. Cartwright (Christopher Newport University) Paper presented December 4, 2014 at the 64th annual conference of the Literacy Research Association, Marco Island, FL Theoretical Framework: Skilled reading comprehension involves being aware of one’s own reading processes (see Cartwright, 2009, 2010, and Pressley, 2002, for reviews) and being able to infer critical information to make sense of the text being read (Elbro & Buch-Iversen, 2013; Oakhill & Cain, 2012). In fact, children who have poor metacognitive skills struggle with comprehension (Paris & Jacobs, 1984), and those with poor comprehension are less able to generate inferences to support their reading comprehension (Cain & Oakhill, 1999; Laing & Kamhi, 2002; Rapp, Van den Broek, McMaster, & Kendeou, 2007). Executive function (EF) skills likely account, at least partially, for individual differences in both reading comprehension and reading awareness. The importance of executive function, broadly speaking, for reading comprehension is well documented (e.g., Cartwright, 2002, 2007, 2012; Oakhill & Cain, 2012; Sesma, Mahone, Levine, Eason, & Cutting, 2009). Recently, theorists have made a distinction between cool and hot executive function. Cool executive function refers to processes involved in abstract, decontextualized problems (e.g., working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility), whereas hot executive function pertains to problems with affective and motivational components (see Zelazo & Mȕller, 2002; Zelazo & Carlson, 2012). Zelazo and Mȕller (2002) include the understanding that others can have false beliefs, an important component of theory of mind (ToM), as an aspect of hot executive function. We propose counterfactual reasoning as another aspect of hot executive function. Counterfactual reasoning involves comparing an outcome to possible alternatives (Kahneman & Miller, 1986; Kahneman & Tversky, 1982) and serves a motivational function (Roese, 1994). By considering alternative antecedents that could have changed an outcome, individuals can motivate changes in behavior to prepare for future events or modulate their emotions (Roese, 1994). The established relationship between false belief understanding and counterfactual reasoning (e.g., Guajardo, Parker, & Turley-Ames, 2008; Guajardo & Turley-Ames, 2004; Perner, Sprung, & Steinkogler, 2004; Riggs, Peterson, Robinson, & Mitchell, 1998) also supports the proposition that they are two aspects of hot executive function. It follows that not only cool EF (Borella, Carretti, & Pelegrina, 2010; Sesma et al., 2009), but also hot EF that involves making inferences, such as ToM and counterfactual reasoning, would predict reading comprehension. Theory of mind involves attributing mental and emotional states to others as well as an awareness of one’s own and others’ thoughts (Astington, Harris, & Olson, 1988). Thus, it is likely that theory of mind understanding would facilitate reading comprehension because it would help readers better understand characters’ motivations. Feldman, Bruner, Renderer, and Spitzer (1996) proposed two landscapes within a story: landscape of consciousness and landscape of action. Reading comprehension relies on one’s abilities to make inferences from narratives regarding both landscapes. To fully comprehend a story, readers must be able to understand the events in the story as well as the characters’ thoughts, perceptions, and motives (Emery, 1996). Evidence for the importance of mental and emotional state inferences for narrative and reading comprehension has been shown with both pre-readers (Pelletier & Astington, 2004; Kim & Phillips, 2014; Tompkins, Guo, & Justice, 2013) and elementary aged readers (Emery, 1996; Shanahan & Shanahan, 1997). Thus, skilled comprehenders are processing the actions of the story as well as the thoughts and intentions of the characters. However, across the elementary grades, children typically do not include references to characters’ internal states in their retellings of stories unless they are stated explicitly (Shannon, Kameenui, & Baumann, 1988), and their inferences from text are most often about characters’ actions rather than characters’ internal states (McConaughy, 1985). Counterfactual reasoning, which involves inferring potential alternative outcomes, is also related to reading comprehension in adults (Trabasso & Bartolone, 2003). Previous work has not examined whether counterfactual reasoning is related to pre-reader and early reading comprehension, yet it is plausible that early counterfactual reasoning lays an important foundation for considering causal links in a story and making inferences that enhance comprehension. Finally, as noted above, knowledge of one’s own thoughts is an important part of theory of mind understanding, and metacognitive skills are important for reading comprehension, which further suggests a connection between theory of mind understanding and reading. Additionally, metacognition is considered an executive skill that supports educational success (Meltzer, 2007). Skilled readers are metacognitively aware, engaging actively and strategically while reading (Jacobs & Paris, 1987; Pressley, 2002). Mental state understanding is an important aspect of metacognition (Lecce, Zocchi, Pagnin, Palladino, & Taumoepeau, 2010). Indeed, researchers have supported a connection between mental state language and metaknowledge about reading (Lecce et al., 2010) and between metacognition (i.e., metacognitive language and second order theory of mind) and later reading ability and story comprehension (Pelletier, 2007). Theory of mind understanding leads to awareness of the author’s intent (Lecce et al., 2010) as well as perhaps awareness of one’s own mental processes while reading. Purpose: Previous work has demonstrated associations between both cool and hot executive function (i.e., theory of mind) and emergent comprehension skills. Less work has examined associations with reading comprehension and metacognitive awareness in elementary school. The present longitudinal study adds to this work by examining the role of cool and hot executive function in pre-reading comprehension in preschool and reading comprehension in elementary school. We also examined the role of counterfactual reasoning in reading comprehension, which has not yet been studied in childhood. We hypothesized that false belief performance and counterfactual reasoning in preschool would predict concurrent narrative comprehension as well as reading comprehension and metacognitive awareness in elementary school. We also hypothesized that concurrent false belief understanding would be related to these aspects of reading skill. Methods and Data Sources: Thirty-one preschoolers (19 girls, 12 boys; 32.3% minority students) completed measures of working memory, color-shape cognitive flexibility (i.e., cool EF skills), first-order ToM, counterfactual reasoning (i.e, hot EF skills), vocabulary, and sentence and phrase comprehension. These same children completed second-order ToM, reading-specific cognitive flexibility, decoding ability, reading comprehension, and reading awareness tasks 3.75 years later. Results: During the preschool years, cognitive flexibility, working memory, and ToM contributed unique variance to children’s sentence and phrase comprehension. Longitudinal data indicated preschool ToM was related to reading awareness in elementary school, whereas preschool counterfactual thinking correlated with reading comprehension in elementary school. Regression analyses indicated preschool counterfactual thinking and general cognitive flexibility uniquely predicted later reading comprehension. Also, in elementary school, reading-specific cognitive flexibility (assessed with sorting speed), contributed unique variance to reading comprehension. Finally, second order ToM predicted elementary students’ reading awareness. The contributions of EFs to elementary school reading comprehension remained significant, even when decoding was controlled. Time 1 narrative comprehension and vocabulary (34% of variance in reading comprehension), Time 2 decoding (20% of variance), and Time 1 and Time 2 hot and cool EF measures including reading awareness (30% of variance), predicted 84% of the total variance in reading comprehension. Furthermore, after controlling for Time 1 vocabulary and narrative comprehension (language comprehension) and Time 2 decoding, preschool EFs (17%) and elementary school EFs (13%) each accounted for significant, unique variance in reading comprehension. Conclusions, Importance, and Interest: Our findings indicate both hot and cool executive functions contribute to children’s developing comprehension skill: narrative comprehension in preschool and reading comprehension in elementary school. Such skills allow children to think flexibly about the actions within a story and about characters’ thoughts. These findings have implications for ways to enhance young children’s comprehension skills. Training either, or both, cool and hot executive function skills may prove to be an effective way to enhance children’s reading comprehension and/or awareness. Additionally, these findings have implications for theoretical conceptions of reading comprehension processes, such as the popular simple view of reading (Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990). Hierarchical regression indicates hot and cool executive skills contribute a practically and statistically significant amount of variance (30%) to reading comprehension beyond language comprehension and decoding processes, which account for just over half (54%) of variance in reading comprehension. Thus, our findings suggest that cognitive processes beyond those traditionally associated with reading comprehension (i.e., executive functions) are critical to successful comprehension development. References: Astington, J., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (Eds.) (1988). Developing theories of mind. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Borella, E., Carretti, B., & Pelegrina, S. (2010). The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 541-552. Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (1999). Inference making ability and its relation to comprehension failure. Reading and Writing, 11, 489-503. Cartwright, K. B. (2002). Cognitive development and reading: The relation of reading-specific multiple classification skill to reading comprehension in elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 56-63. Cartwright, K. B. (2007). The contribution of graphophonological-semantic flexibility to reading comprehension in college students: Implications for a less simple view of reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 39, 173-193. Cartwright, K. B. (2009). The role of cognitive flexibility in reading comprehension: Past, present, and future. In S. E. Israel & G. Duffy (Eds.) Handbook of Research on Reading Comprehension (pp. 115-139). NY: Routledge. Cartwright, K. B. (2010). Word callers: Small-group and one-to-one interventions for children who “read” but don’t comprehend. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Elbro, C., & Buch-Iversen, I. (2013). Activation of background knowledge for inference making: effects on reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(6), 435-452. Emery, D. W. (1996). Helping readers comprehend stories from the characters’ perspective. The Reading Teacher, 49, 534-541. Feldman, C. F., Bruner, J., Renderer, B., & Spitzer, S. (1990). Narrative comprehension. In B. K. Britton & A. D. Pellegrini (Eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language (pp. 1 - 78). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6-10. Guajardo, N., Parker, J., & Turley-Ames, K. J. (2009). Associations among false belief understanding, counterfactual reasoning, and executive function. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 681–702. Guajardo, N. R., & Turley-Ames, K. J. (2004). Preschoolers’ generation of different types of counterfactual statements and theory of mind understanding. Cognitive Development, 19, 53-80. Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127-160. Jacobs, J. E., & Paris, S. G. (1987). Children's metacognition about reading: Issues in definition, measurement, and instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22(3-4), 255-278. Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136-153. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In. D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201-208). New York: Cambridge University Press. Kim, Y. S., & Phillips, B. (2014). Cognitive correlates of listening comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly (just accepted) doi:10.1002/rrq.74 Laing, S. P., & Kamhi, A. G. (2002). The use of think-aloud protocols to compare inferencing abilities in average and below-average readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 5, 436-447. Lecce, S., Zocchi, S., Pagnin, A., Palladino, P., & Taumoepeau, M. (2010). Reading minds: The relation between children’s mental state knowledge and their metaknowledge about reading. Child Development, 81, 1876-1893. McConaughy, S. H. (1985). Good and poor readers' comprehension of story structure across different input and output modalities. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 219-232. Meltzer, L. (Ed.). (2011). Executive function in education: From theory to practice. Guilford Press. Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 91-121. Paris, S. G., & Jacobs, J. E. (1984). The benefits of informed instruction for children’s reading awareness and comprehension skills. Child Development, 55, 2083-2093. Pelletier, J. (2006). Relations among theory of mind, metacognitive language, reading skills and story comprehension in L1 and L2 learners. In A. Antonietti, O. Sempio-Liverta, & A. Marchetti (Eds.), Theory of mind and language in developmental contexts (pp. 77-92). New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media. Pelletier, J., & Astington, J. W. (2004). Action, consciousness, and theory of mind: Children’s ability to coordinate story characters’ actions and thoughts. Early Education & Development, 15, 5-22. Perner, J., Sprung, M., & Steinkogler, B. (2004). Counterfactual conditionals and false belief: a devlopmental dissociation. Cognitive Development, 19, 179-201. Pressley, M. (2002). Metacognition and self-regulated comprehension. In A. E. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Rapp, D. N., Broek, P. V. D., McMaster, K. L., Kendeou, P., & Espin, C. A. (2007). Higher-order comprehension processes in struggling readers: A perspective for research and intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 289-312. Riggs, K. J., Peterson, D. M., Robinson, E. J., & Mitchell, P. (1998). Are errors in false belief tasks symptomatic of a broader difficulty with counterfactuality? Cognitive Development, 13, 73-90. Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805-818. Sesma, H. W., Mahone, E. M., Levine, T., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 232-246. Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, S. (1997). Character perspective charting: Helping children to develop a more complete conception of a story. The Reading Teacher, 50, 668-677. Shannon, P., Kameenui, E. J., & Baumann, J. F. (1988). An investigation of children’s ability to comprehend character motives. American Educational Research Journal, 25(3), 441-462. Tompkins, V., Guo, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2013). Inference generation, story comprehension, and language skills in the preschool years. Reading and Writing, 26, 403-429. Trabasso, T., & Bartolone, J. (2003). Story understanding and counterfactual reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 904-923. Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354-360. Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed), Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, pp. 445-469. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.