1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION According to the U.S. Census Bureau (2000), ethnic minorities will make up one-third of the United States population by the year 2015. By 2050, that number will rise to nearly 50 percent (Wang et al., 2003). Given the changing demographics, as well as recent rise in liberation movements, minority assertion, and identity politics in the United States, there has been an increase in research on ethnicity and related issues such as ethnic group relations and ethnic identity (Lee, 2003; Negy, Shreve, Jensen, & Uddin, 2003; Sellers, Smith, Shelton, Roley, & Chavous, 1997; Simons et al., 2002). In the social sciences, scholars have been investigating the broader social context and the ways in which it shapes and influences people’s understanding of ethnicity (Nagel, 1994; Waters, 1990). For example, they have focused on the larger economic, social, political and cultural conditions associated with ethnic group relations to understand the ways in which ethnicity and ethnic identity are played out at the individual and interpersonal levels. In the psychological literature, there has been an increase in research examining ethnic identity and its relationship to psychological correlates such as self-esteem and at-risk behaviors with youth and young adults (Phinney, 1992; Phinney & Alipura, 1996; Verkuyten & Martinovic, 2006). Researchers have also been examining the development and shifts in multicultural awareness and competence for students living in ethnically diverse communities as well as for counselors, teachers, and practitioners working with diverse populations (Wang et 2 al., 2003). This study examines the following questions: a) How might multicultural attitudes vary by ethnic composition of social context for White Americans and ethnic minority group members, b) How might multicultural attitudes vary as a function of ethnic composition of social context and ethnic identity for ethnic minority group members, and c) What differences are there in predictors of multicultural awareness and competency for White Americans and ethnic minority group members? The following is a review of the literatures on ethnicity, ethnic identity, and multicultural awareness and competency. 3 Chapter 2 SOCIAL CONTEXT Bronfenbrenner's ecological perspective (1979) represents one of the first systematic endeavors to contextualize human behavior. In this framework, Bronfenbrenner (1979) examined how both psychological processes (e.g., perception and motivation) and content (e.g., what is being thought about, perceived, desired or acquired as information) change as a result of one's exposure to and interaction with the environment. Briefly, his theoretical perspective views human development as a function of four systems: a) microsystem, b) mesosystem, c) exosystem and d) macrosystem. Microsystem refers to the individual and that person’s links to other individuals in an immediate setting. Mesosystem refers to the system of relations between two or more settings (e.g., school, home) in which an individual is an active participant. Exosystem refers to settings in which the individual does not participate but nevertheless affect events in that person’s immediate environment (i.e., mesosystem). Macrosystem refers to the organization of broad social structures and processes. Viewing ethnicity from Bronfenbrenner's ecological model (1979), one adopts the perspective that multiple levels of social organization influence the ways in which ethnicity is experienced by the individual. At the structural level, researchers maintain that the larger social, cultural, and political periods ethnic groups are situated in shape the form and content of ethnicity (Nagel, 1994; Waters, 1990). From this perspective, ethnicity is shaped by the continuous shifts 4 in ethnic boundaries that are based on factors such as language, region, culture, physical appearance, and religion. These boundaries are constantly negotiated, redefined, and transformed by both in-group members and out-group actors. Instead of a model of ethnicity that is stable and fixed, the result is a dynamic, situational, and fluid model of ethnicity (Nagel, 1994; Waters, 1990). At the micro-level, researchers view ethnicity as an aspect of one's social identity that plays a central role in the development of the self (Phinney, 1990; Tajfel, 1979). According to this view, one's identification with ethnicity plays a crucial role in the formation of a stable and positive self-concept. An understanding of one's ethnicity also has important implications for attitudes and emotions ethnic minority group members have toward in-group and out-group members (Helms, 1990; Tajfel, 1979). By and large, researchers have associated various attitudes and emotions with specific developmental stages of ethnic identity. For example, some people may have more favorable attitudes and emotions for own ethnic group members in certain stages while others may have more negative attitudes and emotions for own ethnic group members in other stages (Phinney, 1990). In the spirit of Bronfenbrenner's ecological perspective, then, we can see that the social context and various levels of social organization, in theory, influence how individuals perceive and give meaning to their ethnicity. Although the current study does not focus directly on the macro-level, it examines the more immediate settings such as the cities and towns people have lived in which are 5 understood to be influenced by the broader social environment and give meaning to their understanding of ethnicity. A discussion on one’s identification with ethnicity follows in the next section. 6 Chapter 3 ETHNIC IDENTITY In the psychological literature, ethnic identity is seen as a complex, multidimensional construct that includes commitment and a sense of belonging to one’s ethnic group, interest and knowledge about the group, and involvement in activities and cultural traditions of the group. Ethnic identity is considered an important psychological construct for individuals, especially for those who belong to an ethnic minority group (Phinney, 1990, 1996a, 1996b; Christian, Gadfield, Giles, & Taylor, 1976). Ethnic identity becomes especially important in childhood and adolescence and increases as a function of color, language differences, physical features, and racial and ethnic stereotypes (Spencer & Markstrom-Adams, 1990). Children often confront conflicting societal and familial values from which they must choose when forming an ethnic identity. The study of ethnic identity emphasizes how ethnic group members understand and interpret their own ethnicity, with individuals potentially differing in how they identify with their own ethnic group (Phinney, 1996a). While one individual of a particular ethnic group may feel a strong sense of commitment to one’s group, another individual of the same ethnic group may be confused and have conflicting emotions regarding one’s ethnicity. Some people may have high participation in their cultural heritage and customs while others may feel that their ethnicity is not an important aspect in their lives. These differences can be associated with developmental changes over time as individuals explore and 7 evaluate the meaning and implications of their own group membership. The formation of ethnic identity depends on the exploration of questions regarding prior ethnic attitudes and can lead to the construction of a secure and positive sense of one’s identity as a member of an ethnic group that overlaps with the acceptance of other groups. A discussion on approaches researchers use to understand the ways in which ethnic group members understand their ethnicity follows next. Psychological Models of Ethnic Identity Most researchers in psychology use one of two general frameworks to guide their study of ethnic identity: social identity and developmental theory. Although these perspectives share the basic notion that ethnic identity implies some form of psychological attachment with one’s ethnic group, they differ in their points of emphases. This section begins with an analysis of the social identity framework, the first systematic attempt to account for the psychological relationship to one’s ethnic group in a racially oppressive society, followed by an examination of the developmental approach to the study of ethnic identity. Social Identity Framework. A large body of work on ethnic identity has been influenced by the social identity perspective (Cameron & Lalonde, 1994; Gurin, Hurtado, & Peng, 1994; Tajfel, 1981; Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994). In response to the reductionistic tendencies of psychology that focused solely on human behavior produced by individual dynamics, the social identity approach was developed with the aim to deal with the dynamic relationship 8 between the individual and society (Tajfel, 1981). Social identity is defined as “that part of the individual’s self concept which derives from his knowledge of his membership of a social group together with the value and emotional significance attached to that membership” (Tajfel, 1981, p. 255). Typically, the identification process involves social comparisons in which there is a claim of membership to one group and a contrast of that group with another group (Oaker & Brown, 1986). Value-loaded comparisons with other groups or their individual members that present one’s own group(s) favorably enhance the self-concept of the individual. Social psychologists have extensively examined the psychological relationship between the individual and one’s own ethnic group and have argued that ethnic minorities present unique dilemmas for social identity theories (Tajfel, 1981; Turner & Tajfel, 1979). In particular, they have argued that if the dominant group in society holds the characteristics of an ethnic group in low regard, then ethnic group members may potentially be faced with a negative social identity. This, in turn, may have negative psychological effects, such as one "leaning-overbackwards" in accepting derogatory views about one’s ethnic group by the majority group. This view is an important point because it suggests that ethnic minority group members face obstacles in developing a positive ethnic identity. Tajfel and his colleagues (Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel, 1981, Tajfel & Turner, 1979) suggested that minority group members have several ways to resolve their negative ethnic identity. The first, known as passing, is a form of complete assimilation that requires one to identify with the dominant group and reject one’s 9 own ethnic group. One often proclaims disdain for one’s “inferior” ethnic group; however, this course of action may have negative psychological consequences. Furthermore, this is a limited option for ethnic group members whose physical features are distinct. The second solution, cultural and psychological insulation, allows one to isolate oneself from the cultural and social influences of the dominant group. For example, rather than integrating oneself into mainstream society, one chooses to interact only with members of one’s ethnic group and retain cultural values and behavior that reflect this membership. In the third solution, social change or social creativity, one seeks to change the social situation of the ethnic minority group and enhance its self-respect. This solution requires that one not act independently but as an identified member of one’s ethnic group such as reframing existing group characteristics viewed unfavorably and make them appear favorable (e.g., “Black is Beautiful”) or revitalizing old traditions to give them a new and positive significance (e.g., national language; Christian, Gadfield, Giles, & Taylor, 1976; Giles, 1977; Tajfel, 1982). It is evident that social groups are important to the psychological functioning of individuals, particularly ethnic minority group members, when viewed from the social identity perspective. This is because the social identity perspective assumes that society is comprised by social groups that vary in power and status. Power is considered to be a scarce resource, whereas status reflects the relative position of a group along some evaluative dimension. These social groups and their positions relative to one another define the social structure of society. 10 Individuals are assumed to identify with these pre-existing social groups (Tajfel, 1982). Viewed in this way, there is some regard for external forces such that ethnic identity is defined by the power and status position given to one’s ethnic group relative to other groups. However, these external forces such as power and status are not examined or articulated much in the social identity literature. Developmental Framework. Phinney (1989; 1993) proposed a three-stage ethnic identity development model based on the theoretical contributions of Erikson (1968), Marcia (1966; 1980), and Cross (1971), designed to be applicable across ethnic groups. Phinney focuses on the exploration of ethnicity in light of experiences of one’s group, which may or may not include oppressive, racist treatment by the dominant group in the larger society. Although individuals may experience discrimination or racism, these experiences are not the central foci in rethinking of issues related to one’s ethnicity. Rather, individuals’ experience of the more general phenomenon of dissonance--an increasing “awareness that not all cultural values of the dominant group are beneficial to ethnic minorities”-motivates them to explore their ethnicity (Phinney, 1993, p. 69). In Phinney’s three-stage model, the first stage, unexamined ethnic identity, is characterized by lack of active exploration of one's own ethnicity. In this stage, individuals internalize societal or parental views of their own ethnicity, though this does not necessarily imply preference for the dominant culture. In cases where individuals have been exposed to positive valuing of their ethnicity from their parents, individuals may have positive views towards their own ethnic group. 11 The key feature of this stage is that people have not critically explored their own ethnicity which suggests that people’s investment in defining their ethnic identity is an important aspect of ethnic identity development. This initial stage continues until people encounter a situation that initiates an ethnic identity search. The second stage, ethnic identity search, is characterized by an exploration into one’s culture of origin. Usually, people are motivated to explore their ethnicity due to a growing awareness that not all values of the dominant group are beneficial to ethnic minority group members. The growing awareness is said to be cumulative and becomes the basis for initiating a search or exploration of ethnicity where one questions and develops a personal sense of ethnic identification. In the ethnic identity search stage, people begin to learn more about their culture of origin by actively participating in cultural activities and talking to family or friends about issues related to ethnicity. Furthermore, they think more about the implications of their own ethnicity and minority status for their present and future life. The final stage, ethnic identity achievement, is characterized by an appreciation for one's ethnicity and resolution of conflicts with the dominant group. As individuals actively learn about their ethnicity, they come to a deeper understanding and appreciation of their ethnicity through a resolution of two issues—cultural differences between own ethnic group and the dominant group and the low status of that ethnic group in society (Phinney, 1990). At this point, 12 people have a clear and confident relationship with their ethnicity, though they may not be highly involved in activities related to their ethnic group. The conceptual and theoretical work on ethnic identity within psychology offer different definitions about ethnic identity and carry different assumptions concerning human behavior and processes that are influential in ethnic identity development. Although none are comprehensive, these two frameworks have contributed to ethnic identity theory and research. One area of research on ethnic identity that has received less attention involves the relationship between ethnic identity and social context, which may suggest that ethnic identity is much more fluid and dynamic that what these two frameworks suggest (Kinket & Verkuyten, 1997). Social Context and Ethnic Identity The previous discussion examining perceptions of ethnicity through Bronfrenbrenner’s (1979) ecological model illustrates how multiple levels shape the content and form of ethnicity. Although researchers acknowledge some variability in ethnic identity (Berry, 1995; Helms, 1989; Phinney, 1996; Kim & Liem, 2003), the major assumption in psychological approaches is that ethnic identity in adulthood has substantial continuity. However, at the individual level, a useful way of examining the relationship between social context and ethnic identity is to examine the social context and the extent to which it shapes people’s identities. Studies have demonstrated that social demographic characteristics such as age, birthplace, ethnicity, and sex are more likely to be experienced as part of 13 the social self when individuals are in the minority in terms of these statuses in interpersonal situations (Kim & Liem, 2003; McGuire & Padawer-Singer, 1976; McGuire et. al., 1978; Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994). Furthermore, research has shown that context influences how individuals define their ethnic identity which may depend in large part on the intergroup context. For example, in a recent study on social categorization and group homogeneity of Australians and Americans (Haslam, Oakes, Turner, & McGarty, 1999), researchers demonstrated that when perceived differences between groups were greater than perceived differences within a group, one’s perceptions of ethnic identity became stronger than other social identities. This finding is consistent with others (Berry, 1995) who argue that identification with ethnicity may change as a function of contact between one's culture of origin and the dominant culture, where continual contact may produce changes in one's ethnic identity “unevenly” across situations. This variation in perception of one’s ethnic identity in different situations has its premise in the concept of integration or biculturality (Berry, 1995) whereby the individual engages in the activities of one culture while maintaining identity and relationships in another. In contrast to these short-term variations in ethnic identity, long-term changes may occur as the individual explores and evaluates the meaning and implication of one’s ethnicity throughout the life course. Parham (1989) proposes that adolescence and early adulthood are the earliest periods in which the individual is capable of experiencing ethnicity in meaningful ways. Prior to this 14 period, people’s views regarding ethnicity are believed to reflect parental or societal views which have been uncritically internalized by ethnic minority group members. From a developmental perspective, studies have demonstrated ethnic identity change across the life span. A study by Ethier and Deaux (1994), for example, examined ethnic identity among Latino students entering new contexts (i.e., college) and found that they adopted different paths in negotiating their ethnic identity in a new context depending on their exploration of ethnicity prior to college. Latino students who valued their ethnic identity before entering college created new links, a process called “remooring,” with their own ethnic groups in college. Over a period of a year, Latino students whose ethnic identity was strongly linked to family, neighborhood, and cultural background, forged new links with new Latino friends and organizations. These new links enabled these students to re-examine their ethnicity in light of their new context and to buffer threats to their ethnic identity from the forces of discrimination and negative evaluation. In contrast, those who did not value their Latino identity before entering college did not create new links with their ethnic group and consequently were more likely to perceive threats and to experience weakened ethnic identity. Though this study does not examine participants over an extended period of time, the findings do suggest that ethnic identities change qualitatively over time. As the discussion above suggests, the social context, especially the ethnic composition and intergroup influences within it, can have a large impact on the way people perceive and understand their ethnic identity. By and large, the ethnic 15 identity literature has assumed that ethnic identity is relatively stable and fixed across situations and over time. This stable and fixed view of ethnic identity ignores the complex process of both short-term and long-term ethnic identity change that involves variations in form and meaning of ethnic identity. This stable and fixed view further ignores the various levels of social organization and social context that may influence ethnic identity. I now turn to literature that examines links between ethnic identity and psychological correlates, especially multicultural awareness and competence. 16 Chapter 4 ETHNOCULTURAL EMPATHY The review above shows the growing importance of the social and psychological dimensions related to ethnicity and ethnic identity, which is a function of the increasing ethnic diversity at the local and national levels. In light of these shifts, scholars have commented on the changing experiences and attitudes of White Americans and ethnic minority group members. For example, researches have noted the attitudes of White Americans toward other ethnic groups and how increases in cultural diversity can lead to greater anxiety (Peck & Sears, 2005). They have also noted the shifting experiences and attitudes of ethnic minorities in a number of settings such as school and work (Abe-Kim, Okazaki, & Goto, 2001). In this context, scholars have commented on the importance of greater cultural understanding, awareness, and acceptance of individuals from different cultural and ethnic backgrounds (Sue & Sue, 1999; Wang et al., 2003). They have also commented on the need for new concepts focusing on cultural awareness and sensitivity for individuals as they interact with people who are culturally and ethnically different from them and for practitioners and educators to better meet the needs of an increasingly diverse population (Collins & Pieterse, 2007; Sue & Sue, 1999). The growth of researchers examining multicultural attitudes and experiences has been guided by concepts such as “multicultural counseling competence” (Sue, Arredondo, & McDavis, 1992) and “cultural competence” 17 (Sue & Sue, 1999). In essence these concepts refer to the awareness, knowledge, and skills that people acquire when working with individuals who may be culturally or ethnically different from them. The premise of this research is that people will need new sets of skills and attitudes at the cognitive and affective levels to enable them to interact and work effectively with an increasingly diverse population. As Munroe and Pearson (2006) argue, “presumed knowledge and beliefs, the emotional ties associated with such knowledge and beliefs, and the behavioral actions displayed owing to both” provide the basis from which multicultural attitudes are formed (p. 820). Scholars have pointed out the needs to examine and re-examine this knowledge and these attitudes and perceptions to promote greater understanding, awareness, and acceptance of and between ethnic groups. In the past two decades, researchers have investigated a number of factors that may be related to multicultural awareness. Researchers have focused on ethnicity by examining its relationship to a multicultural awareness. For example, Verkuyten and Martinovic (2006) examined a large sample (N = 355) of Dutch, Turkish, and Moroccan participants and found that group membership may shape multicultural attitudes. Specifically, they found that in-group members may cultivate more negative views of out-group members to preserve self-esteem and that they may be more disposed to accept out-group stereotypes and reluctant to take conflicting information into account. Focusing on ethnic and cultural identity, Wolsko, Park, Judd, and Wittenbrink (2000) found that maintaining one’s ethnic 18 and cultural identity was related to improved psychological health and better ability to adjust to changing social environments in the future. In addition to ethnicity and group membership, research has shown that ethnic identity may be related to more positive multicultural attitudes. Verkuyten and Martinovic (2006) found that ethnic minority group members showed more support for multiculturalism than did their majority group counterparts. They further found that ethnic minority group members with higher levels of ethnic identity showed more support for multiculturalism, whereas higher levels of ethnic identity for majority group members were found to be related to lower levels of support for multiculturalism. In addition to these findings, the researchers found that positive multicultural attitudes were associated with ingroup identification, perceived discrimination, out-group friendships, and ideological notions. It appears that while ethnic group members who identify with their group find it important to maintain their culture, forming friends with other group members facilitates feelings of sympathy and concern for members outside of the group. In addition to these factors, researchers have examined the roles of sex, age, and number of completed multicultural courses in multicultural attitudes. For example, females were found to be more likely than males to accept cultural differences (Miville, Gelso, Pannu, Liu, Touradji, Holloway, & Fuertes, 1999). Furthermore, individuals who were encouraged to reflect on the importance of improving interethnic relationships reported a reduction in prejudicial attitudes 19 (Wolsko et al., 2000). Personality has also been examined in relation to multicultural attitude development (Flynn, 2005). Researchers have shown that certain elements of the Big Five personality traits may be effective in helping measure and predict prejudice (Ekehammar & Akrami, 2003; Saucier & Goldberg, 1998; Silvestri & Richardson, 2001). Flynn’s (2005) study focused on the personality variable “openness to experience”, which refers to the willingness of an individual to modify existing feelings and attitudes in the presence of novel concepts and circumstances. The results showed that individuals who were higher on “openness to experience” scored lower in prejudicial attitudes. These studies show that a number of factors may shape people’s multicultural attitudes, from group identification to gender to personality factors. Although there has been a growth in the literature on multicultural counseling competency and cultural competency, it tends to focus on the knowledge and skills that people may acquire rather than the more subjective empathy piece that may be important in developing greater openness to those who may be culturally and ethnically different. Furthermore, this literature tends to focus on clinicians and counselors who may be working with an ethnically diverse population, and thus may not be applicable to other settings, especially undergraduate students. One interesting conceptualization of multicultural awareness involves the concept of “ethnocultural empathy” which refers to empathy toward members of ethnic groups different from one’s own (Wang et al., 2003). The researchers used a blend of definitions such as “empathy” (i.e., feeling 20 in oneself what others feel; Strayer & Eisenberg, 1987), “cultural empathy” (learned ability at the cognitive, affective, and communicative levels to allows for understanding of others; Ivey, Ivey, & Simek-Downing, 1987), and “ethnic perspective taking” (awareness of ethnic prejudice, discrimination, ethnic attitudes and perspectives; Quintana, Ybarra, Gonzalez-Doupe, & Baessa, 2000). Based on these general and culturally specific concepts, Wang at al. (2003) conceptualized ethnocultural empathy as comprised of “intellectual empathy, empathic emotions, and the communication of those two” (p. 222). They argued that ethnocultural empathy includes: a) intellectual empathy, which refers to one’s ability to understand the thinking and feeling as well as the perception of people who are ethnically different from self; b) empathic emotions, which refers to feeling the emotions and emotional condition of those who are ethnically different from self and responding to them, and; 3) communicative empathy, which refers to the expression of thoughts and feelings toward those who are ethnically different from self. They subsequently developed the “Scale of Ethnocultural Empathy (SEE)” to measure these three conceptually distinct components and later separated them into four distinct components using 31 items: a) Empathic Feeling and Expression, b) Empathic Perspective Taking, c) Acceptance of Cultural Differences, and d) Empathic Awareness. Wang et al. (2003) found that females and ethnic minorities were more likely to show empathy and reception of cultural differences than males and majority group members. These individuals were also more conscious of the 21 experiences of people from different ethnic backgrounds. Factors that were associated with higher levels of ethnocultural empathy were the degree of diversity within immediate families and friends, high school settings, and neighborhoods. Individuals who had limited contact with ethnic groups showed a delay in developing ethnic perspective taking abilities. Their findings suggested that increased interaction among ethnic groups could foster higher levels of cultural empathy. Although the concept of ethnocultural empathy and its accompanying measure are relatively new, they are promising in the research on multicultural awareness for its focus on empathy as well as its applicability to a wider audience. 22 Chapter 5 THE PRESENT STUDY Drawing on the several bodies of work reviewed above, this study examines the relationship among social context, ethnic identity, and multicultural attitudes. Although psychology tends to focus on the individual in isolation of context, work such as Bronfrenbrenner’s ecological model demonstrate the link between the individual and society and illustrate how social contexts may shape psychological processes. With respect to ethnicity, scholars have commented on how the larger social, political, and economic contexts can shape the form and content of ethnicity. In particular, they suggest how one’s understanding of ethnicity may be a function of the sociopolitical periods in which one is situated. This work suggests people’s perceptions of ethnicity can be reactive to sociopolitical contexts and thus more dynamic and fluid than traditional approaches have suggested. An important aspect of ethnic group affiliation and ethnic identity development involves the attitudes and emotions they have about their own and other ethnic groups. This body of work suggests that ethnic group membership and the ways in which ethnic minority group members negotiate their ethnic identity may shape how individuals understand ethnic and cultural differences. Given these bodies of work, this study investigated the following three sets of hypotheses: First, we examined how ethnocultural empathy would vary for White Americans and ethnic minority group members who lived in areas with 23 varying levels of ethnic diversity. We hypothesized that ethnic minority group participants in general would have greater ethnocultural empathy than would White American participants. On the other hand, we anticipated that White Americans growing up in more diverse areas would report greater ethnocultural empathy than would those who grew up in less ethnically diverse areas (see Table 1). Second, we investigated how ethnocultural empathy would vary as a function of ethnic composition of social context and ethnic identity for our ethnic minority group participants. In particular, we expected individuals with an unexamined ethnic identity orientation to have generally low ethnocultural empathy (see Table 2), whereas people with an achieved ethnic identity would have high ethnocultural empathy. We expected individuals with a search ethnic identity orientation who lived in more ethnically diverse areas to have greater ethnocultural empathy compared to those with the same ethnic identity orientation living in less ethnically diverse areas. Finally, we examined the role of generational status, age, socioeconomic status, gender, ethnic identity, and ethnic composition of social context in predicting ethnocultural empathy. We predicted that gender, age, socioeconomic status, ethnic composition of social context would influence ethnocultural empathy for both our ethnic minority and White American respondents. However, we also expected that generational status and ethnic identity would be important predictors of ethnocultural empathy for our ethnic minority respondents. 24 Table 1 Level of Ethnocultural Empathy by Ethnic Group & Ethnic Composition of Social Context Ethnic Composition Less Diverse More Diverse Total Ethnic Minorities H H H White Americans L H M Ethnic Group Note. Ethnocultural Empathy reflects: low (L), medium (M), and high (H). Table 2 Level of Ethnocultural Empathy by Ethnic Identity Orientation & Ethnic Composition of Social Context Ethnic Composition Less Diverse More Diverse Total L M L-M L-M M-H M H H H Ethnic Identity Orientation Unexamined Search Achieved Note. Ethnocultural Empathy reflects: low (L), medium (M), and high (H). 25 Chapter 6 METHOD Participants One-hundred and thirty-three participants were recruited from a public university in northern California. Of these participants, 96 were females and 37 were males. Participants were between the ages of 19 and 55, with a mean age of 25.62 (SD = 7.24). The SES levels were relatively comparable with both groups being represented most in the “100K or higher” category (see Table 3) and the “55K-64K” category. In exchange for their participation, participants were given 1 research credit if they were from the psychology research pool or offered extra credit if administered in university courses. For purposes of this study, participants were assigned to the White American ethnic group or the ethnic minority group based on their selfidentification. Seventy-eight participants were White Americans and 55 were ethnic minorities (see Table 3). The mean age for White Americans and ethnic minorities was 26.6 (SD = 8.27) and 24.2 (SD = 5.21), respectively. A majority of the White American sample was third generation or higher whereas the ethnic minority sample was distributed relatively equally among the first, second, and third generation or higher categories. Among ethnic minority participants, Asian Americans had the highest representation with 31, followed by Latinos, 11, African Americans, nine, Pacific Islanders, two, and Native Americans, two (Table 4). 26 Table 3 Demographic Characteristics of Participants Ethnic Minority (n = 55) White American (n = 78) N (%) 39 (71%) 16 (29%) N (%) 57 (73%) 21 (27%) N (%) 24.18 5.21 N (%) 26.63 8.27 GENERATION 1st 2nd 3rd or Higher N (%) 16 (29%) 19 (35%) 20 (36%) N (%) 2 (2%) 6 (8%) 70 (90%) SES < 14K 15K-24K 25K-34K 35K-44K 45K-54K 55K-64K 65K-74K 75K-84K 85K-99K > 100K N (%) 3 (5%) 5 (9%) 5 (9%) 4 (7%) 7 (13%) 8 (15%) 5 (9%) 4 (7%) 2 (4%) 12 (22%) N (%) 1 (1%) 7 (9%) 4 (5%) 4 (5%) 5 (6%) 11 (14%) 6 (8%) 9 (12%) 10 (13%) 21 (27%) Demographic Variable GENDER Females Males AGE M SD 27 Table 4 Ethnic Minority Groups Female (% of Ethnicity) Male (% of Ethnicity) African Am. 8 (89%) 1 (11%) Latino 8 (73%) 3 (27%) 11 (20%) Asian Am. 20 (65%) 11 (35%) 31 (56%) Pacific Islander 2 (100%) 0 2 (4%) Nat. Am. 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 2 (4%) Column Total 39 (69%) 16 (31%) 55 (100%) Ethnic Minority Group Total (% of Ethnic Minority Group) 9 (16%) Design In this study, we employed a 2 x 2 design to examine White Americans and ethnic minority group members in relation to ethnocultural empathy. The independent variables were ethnic group (White Americana and ethnic minority group members) and ethnic composition of social setting (ethnically more diverse, less diverse). To examine ethnic minority group members with respect to ethnocultural empathy, we used a 2 x 2 design where ethnic identity orientation (examined, search, and achievement) and ethnic composition of social setting (ethnically more diverse, less diverse) were the independent variables. 28 Procedure The researcher met the participants in a lab or classroom at their university to administer a packet of questionnaires. The participants were told that for the next thirty minutes they would complete several questionnaires related to the topic of identity. They were then instructed to read the material describing the purpose of the study and asked to sign a consent form. The researcher then gave the participants a packet of questionnaires and provided them with instructions to complete the questionnaires. The researcher addressed any questions prior to administering the questionnaires and then debriefed the participants upon completion of the questionnaires. Measures Ethnic Identity. Ethnic identity was measured using the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM) by Phinney (1992). This measure, designed for use with various ethnic groups, includes 12 items that assess three general domains: ethnic practices or behaviors (2 items), e.g., "I participate in cultural practices of my own group, such as special food, music, or customs"; positive ethnic attitudes and sense of belonging (5 items), e.g., "I am happy that I am a member of the group that I belong to," and; ethnic identity achievement, e.g., "I have a clear sense of my ethnic background and what it means for me." Items are rated using a 4-point ranging scale from strongly agree to strongly disagree. Scores are calculated by summing across items and calculating the mean. Low scores represent a low or 29 unexamined ethnic identity and high scores represent a high or achieved ethnic identity. Ethnocultural Empathy. Multicultural awareness was assessed using the Scale of Ethnocultural Empathy (SEE) by Wang et al. (2003). This measure assesses multicultural awareness or ethnocultural empathy and consists of four general areas: 1) Empathic Feeling and Expression (15 items), e.g., “I seek opportunities to speak with individuals of other racial or ethnic backgrounds about their experiences”; 2) Empathic Perspective-Taking (7 items), e.g., “I can relate to the frustration that some people feel about having fewer opportunities due to their racial or ethnic backgrounds”; 3) Acceptance of Cultural Differences (5 items), e.g., “I feel irritated when people of different racial or ethnic background speak their language around me,” and; 4) Empathic Awareness (4 items), e.g., “I can see how other racial or ethnic groups are systematically oppressed in our society.” Items are rated using a 6-point ranging scale from strongly agree to strongly disagree. The total score is calculated by reversing negatively worded items, summing across the total 31 items, and dividing by 31. Subscale scores can be obtained by adding the scores for each area and dividing by the number of items for each subscale. Percent Diversity. The Demographic Form assessed background information about participants, including gender, age, socioeconomic status, generational status, length of residence in the U.S., and places they had resided in the U.S. To capture the percent diversity of specific places participants had 30 resided, participants were asked to “list all of the places where you (and your family) have lived in the U.S.” and to indicate the zip code or city and state. They were also asked to indicate the number of years they lived in the location(s). This information was then used to calculate an index of the diversity of respondents’ home cities and towns using data from the 2000 U.S. Census Bureau. Specifically, we calculated the percentage of non-Whites in respondents’ cities and towns. For example, since ethnic minorities make up 46.1 percent of the total population of the city of San Francisco, this number could potentially represent percent diversity for any respondent indicating this location in their Demographics Form (U.S. Census, 2000). The location with the greatest number of years that participants had lived in was then used to represent percent diversity of social settings in subsequent analyses. For analyses concerning differences by ethnic group and ethnic identity, we separated levels of diversity of cities and towns by using the threshold of 50 percent or more of the population being ethnic minority. If a city or town was comprised of 50 percent or more ethnic minority, then it was assigned to the “more diverse” category; if a city or town was comprised of 49 percent or less ethnic minority, then it was assigned to the “less diverse” category. This cutoff also allowed us adequate sample sizes for statistical analyses. For analyses concerning percent diversity as a predictor variable, the raw percent diversity score was used. 31 Chapter 7 RESULTS Preliminary Analyses Descriptive analyses of the study variables are reported in Table 5. Among the four Ethnocultural Empathy variables, participants reported the highest rating with Empathic Awareness (M = 4.96, SD = .84) and the lowest rating with Empathic Perspective-Taking (M = 4.03, SD = .95). The mean scores for the Ethnocultural Empathy variables are comparable to those reported by Wang et al. (2003). The mean score for Ethnic Identity was 2.81 (SD = .95), with ethnic minority group participants reporting higher Ethnic Identity (M = 3.14, SD = .54) than did White Americans (M = 2.58, SD = .46). These results are consistent with previous studies that have shown differences by Ethnic Group, with ethnic minority group members generally reporting higher scores compared to White Americans (Kim & Liem, 2003; Phinney & Baumann, 2001). Finally, the average Percent Diversity (i.e., the ethnic minority group population relative to the White American population) for reported cities and towns was 39 percent; ethnic minority group respondents reported living in more diverse areas (M = 46%, SD = .16.4%) than did White Americans (M = 34%, SD = .18.5%). 32 Table 5 Intercorrelations Among Demographic Characteristics, Percent Diversity, Ethnic Identity, and Ethnocultural Empathy Variables Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1. EFE - 2. EPT .56** - 3. ACD .56** .35** - 4. EA .68** .45** .54** - 5. Ethnic Identity .38** .43** .28** .40** - 6. Percent Diversity -.14 -.23** .07 -.09 -.00 - 7. Age .32** .09 .08 .16 -.05 .03 - 8. SES -.13 -.17* -.11 -.12 -.28** .16 -.13 - 9. Generation -.16 -.39** -.22* -.23** -.41** .24** .03 .19* - M 4.61 4.03 4.79 4.96 2.81 .39 25.62 6.62 3.53 SD .75 .95 .90 .84 .56 .18 7.24 2.84 1.52 Note: EFE = Empathic feeling and expression; EPT = Empathic perspective-taking; ACD = Acceptance of cultural differences; EA = Empathic awareness.*p < .05. **p < .01. Preliminary analyses with key demographic variables (e.g., age, socioeconomic status, gender, generational status, and ethnicity) in relation to Ethnocultural Empathy variables were conducted. Bivariate correlational analyses with age, generational status (GS), and socioeconomic status (SES) along with the four Ethnocultural Empathy variables showed a significant relationship between GS and three of the four Ethnocultural Empathy variables (Empathetic Perspective-Taking, Acceptance of Cultural Differences, and Empathic Awareness) and between Age and one of the four Ethnocultural Empathy 33 variables (Empathic Feeling and Expression). Specifically, the results showed that participants who were lower in GS (i.e., more recent immigrants) reported greater Ethnocultural Empathy and participants who were older reported greater Ethnocultural Empathy (see Table 5). A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) in which gender served as a fixed factor and Ethnocultural Empathy variables served as dependent variables failed to show any significance. Ethnocultural Empathy by Ethnic Group To investigate how Ethnocultural Empathy might differ for White Americans and ethnic minority group members living in cities and towns that varied in Percent Diversity we analyzed the four Ethnocultural Empathy subscales (Empathic Feeling and Expression, Empathic Perspective-Taking, Acceptance of Cultural Differences, and Empathic Awareness) with a multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) where Ethnic Group (White American, Ethnic Minority Group) and Percent Diversity (More Diverse, Less Diverse) served as the independent variables. Contrary to our expectations, the results did not show an interaction effect where Ethnocultural Empathy varied as a function of Ethnic Group and Percent Diversity of social settings. No main effect for Percent Diversity was found. However, the results did show an overall main effect for Ethnic Group, F(4, 126) = 17.46, p < .001, such that ethnic minority group members reported greater Ethnocultural Empathy than did White Americans across settings (see Table 6). Specifically, Empathic Feeling and Expression, F(1, 132) = 6.32, p < .05, 34 Table 6 Multivariate Analysis of Variance of Ethnocultural Empathy Variables by Ethnic Group & Percent Diversity Source Percent Diversity (A) Ethnic Group (B) Multivariate df Fa EFEb Univariate Ethnocultural Empathy EPTb ACDb 1 1.10 .09 .04 2.34 1 17.46*** 6.32* 58.21*** 6.13* EAb .12 14.97*** AxB 1 .21 .08 .04 .57 .00 Note: Multivariate F ratios were generated from Wilks’ Lambda statistics. EFE = Empathic feeling and expression; EPT = Empathic perspective-taking, and; ACD = Acceptance of cultural differences; EA = Empathic awareness. aMultivariate df = 4, 126. bUnivariate df = 1, 132. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Empathic Perspective-Taking, F(1, 132) = 58.21, p < .001, Acceptance of Cultural Differences, F(1, 132) = 6.53, p < .05, and Empathic Awareness, F(1, 132) = 14.97, p < .001, were differentiated by Ethnic Group (see Table 7). Ethnic minority group members reported greater EFE (M = 4.81 vs. 4.47), EPT (M = 3.57 vs. 4.67), ACD (M = 5.00 vs. 4.59), and EA, (M = 5.29 vs. 4.73), across settings than did White Americans. Mean scores are reported in Table 7. These findings suggest a limited effect of Percent Diversity of social settings but a strong influence of the Ethnic Group of participants on Ethnocultural Empathy. 35 Table 7 Mean Scores on EFE, EPT, ACD, & EA by Ethnic Group & Percent Diversity EFE Percent Diversity Ethnic Group Less Diverse More Diverse Total Ethnic Minorities 4.77 4.85 4.81 White Americans 4.47 Ethnic Group 4.47 EPT Percent Diversity 4.47 Less Diverse More Diverse Total Ethnic Minorities 4.67 4.67 4.67 White Americans 3.60 Ethnic Group 3.55 ACD Percent Diversity 3.57 Less Diverse More Diverse Total Ethnic Minorities 5.07 4.94 5.00 White Americans 4.78 Ethnic Group 4.41 EA Percent Diversity 4.59 Less Diverse More Diverse Total Ethnic Minorities 5.32 5.26 5.29 White Americans 4.75 4.71 4.73 Note. EFE = Empathic Feeling & Expression; EPT = Empathic Perspective-Taking; ACD = Acceptance of Cultural Differences; EA = Empathic awareness. Ethnocultural Empathy by Ethnic Identity Orientation The assessment of Ethnocultural Empathy of ethnic minority group participants was done with a MANOVA where Ethnic Identity Orientation and 36 Percent Diversity of social setting served as independent variables and Ethnocultural Empathy variables served as dependent variables. Operationalization of Ethnic Identity Orientation The ethnic identity orientations were based on Phinney’s (1989) ethnic identity model which includes Unexamined Ethnic Identity Orientation, Search Ethnic Identity Orientation, and Achieved Ethnic Identity Orientation. We operationalized ethnic identity by using the total ethnic identity score on the MEIM and assigned participants to one of the three aforementioned ethnic identity orientations. Using the procedures employed by Kim and Liem (2003), the total ethnic identity scores were divided at the 33rd and 67th percentiles. Specifically, participants in the lowest one-third were assigned to the Unexamined Ethnic Identity Orientation group (N = 21, M = 2.62), participants in the highest one-third were placed in the Achieved Ethnic Identity Orientation group (N = 17, M = 3.21), and those in the middle group were placed in the Ethnic Identity Search Orientation group (N = 17, M = 3.72). To verify our assignment of respondents to the three Ethnic Identity Orientation groups, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyze the mean scores of respondents’ ethnic identity. In this analysis, Ethnic Identity Orientation served as the independent variable and the Ethnic Identity Mean Score served as the dependent variable. The results showed that Ethnic Identity Mean Scores varied significantly by Ethnic Identity Orientation, F(2, 54) = 69.60, p < .001. Simple effects tests showed that the Unexamined Ethnic Identity 37 Orientation group was significantly lower in its mean score relative to the Search Ethnic Identity Orientation group (p < .001) and the Achieved Ethnic Identity Orientation group (p < .001). The Ethnic Identity Search Orientation group also was significantly lower in its mean score compared to the Achieved Ethnic Identity Orientation group (p < .001). These findings suggest three distinct Ethnic Identity Orientation groups based on the non-overlapping Ethnic Identity Mean Scores and provide some validity for our ethnic identity grouping. Ethnocultural Empathy. The analyses produced a mixed picture in view of our expectations. Although findings did not support our expectations that Ethnocultural Empathy would vary as a function of Ethnic Identity Orientation and Percent Diversity of social settings, the results did show an overall main effect for Ethnic Identity Orientation, F(8, 92) = 2.98, p < .01. Similar to the earlier findings by Ethnic Group, all four Ethnocultural Empathy variables were significantly differentiated by Ethnic Identity Orientation: Empathic Feeling and Expression, F(2, 54) = 6.91, p < .01, Empathic Perspective-Taking, F(2, 54) = 4.33, p < .05, Acceptance of Cultural Differences, F(2, 54) = 3.81, p < .05, and Empathic Awareness, F(2, 54) = 6.10, p < .01 (Table 8). Simple effects tests showed that people assigned to the Unexamined Ethnic Identity Orientation group reported lower EFE (M = 4.46) than did those in the Search (M = 4.84, p < .05) and Achieved (M = 5.22, p < .001) Ethnic Identity Orientation groups. People in the Unexamined Ethnic Identity Orientation group also reported lower EPT (M = 4.28) than did those in the Search (M = 4.91, p 38 < .01) and Achieved (M = 4.88, p < .05) Ethnic Identity Orientation groups. They furthermore reported lower EA (M = 4.85) than did those in the Search (M = 5.51, p < .05) and Achieved (M = 5.55, p < .01) groups. Those in the Achieved Ethnic Identity Orientation group reported greater ACD (M = 5.39, p < .0) than did those in the Unexamined (M = 4.80, p < .01) and Search (M = 4.93, p < .05) Ethnic Identity Orientation groups. No main effect was found for Percent Diversity of social setting. Mean scores are reported in Table 9. These findings further support the effect of ethnic group affiliation and how identification with one’s ethnic group may play a role in the ways in which people understand multicultural attitudes. Table 8 Multivariate Analysis of Variance of Ethnocultural Empathy Variables by Ethnic Identity Orientation & Percent Diversity Source Percent Diversity (A) Ethnic Identity Orientation (B) Multivariate df F 1 .53a 1 2.98**b EFEc Univariate Ethnocultural Empathy EPTc ACDc 1.38 6.91** EAc .20 .03 .06 4.33* 3.81* 6.10** AxB 1 1.35b .93 .19 1.39 .32 Note: Multivariate F ratios were generated from Wilks’ Lambda statistics. EFE = Empathic feeling and expression; EPT = Empathic perspective-taking, and; ACD = Acceptance of cultural differences; EA = Empathic awareness. aMultivariate df = 4, 46. bMultivariate df = 8, 92. c Univariate df = 2, 54. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 39 Table 9 Mean Scores on EFE by Ethnic Identity Orientation & Percent Diversity EFE Percent Diversity Ethnic Identity Orientation Less Diverse More Diverse Total Unexamined 4.52 4.40 4.46 Search 4.65 5.04 4.84 Achieved 5.06 5.38 EPT Percent Diversity 5.22 Less Diverse More Diverse Total Unexamined 4.19 4.36 4.28 Search 4.96 4.87 4.91 Achieved 4.79 Ethnic Identity Orientation 4.97 ACD Percent Diversity 4.88 Less Diverse More Diverse Total Unexamined 5.00 4.60 4.80 Search 4.71 5.08 4.93 Achieved 5.42 Ethnic Identity Orientation 5.36 EA Percent Diversity 5.39 Less Diverse More Diverse Total Unexamined 4.75 4.95 4.85 Search 5.46 5.55 5.51 Ethnic Identity Orientation Achieved 5.62 5.47 5.55 Note. EFE = Empathic feeling and expression; EPT = Empathic perspective-taking; ACD = Acceptance of Cultural Differences; EA = Empathic awareness. 40 Predictors of Ethnocultural Empathy The previous two sets of analyses investigated differences between White American and ethnic minority participants and among ethnic minority group members with varying Ethnic Identity Orientations. Although differences were found for Ethnocultural Empathy between White Americans and ethnic minority group members and among Ethnic Identity Orientation groups, we found little influence of Percent Diversity of social context. To test the final hypothesis examining differences in predictors of Ethnocultural Empathy between White American and ethnic minority participants, we conducted separate simultaneous multiple regression analyses for each ethnic group. Specifically, a simultaneous multiple regression analysis for White American participants was conducted to predict the level of Ethnocultural Empathy based on Age, Gender, SES, GS, Ethnic Identity, and Percent Diversity. The regression equation conducted with Empathetic Feeling and Expression (EFE) revealed that Age (β = .42, p < .001) and Gender (β = .28, p < .01) accounted for a significant portion of the variability, R2 = .25, adjusted R2 = .19, F(6, 77) = 3.99, p < .01. This finding indicates that older participants are higher on EFE compared to younger participants. It also shows that White American females are higher on EFE than are White American males. The results are presented in Table 10. 41 A second simultaneous multiple regression analysis for ethnic minority participants was conducted to predict the level of Ethnocultural Empathy based Age, Gender, SES, GS, Ethnic Identity, and Percent Diversity. Three of the four models were significant, with the fourth reaching marginal significance (Acceptance of Cultural Differences, p = .071), for ethnic minority participants. For EFE, the regression equation showed that Ethnic Identity (β = .61, p < .001) and Percent Diversity (β = .27, p < .05) accounted for a significant portion of the variability, R2= .48, adjusted R2 = .42, F(6, 54) = 7.45, p < .001. Findings show that participants who identify more with their own ethnic group have greater EFE. Also, those participants living in areas with greater ethnic diversity have greater EFE. For Empathic Perspective-Taking (EPT), Ethnic Identity (β = .40, p < .01) and Age (β = .31, p < .05) accounted for a significant amount of the variability, R2 = .27, adjusted R2 = .18, F(6, 54) = 2.90, p < .05. Similar to EFE results, those individuals who identify more with their own ethnic group have greater EPT. Further, older respondents have greater EPT compared to their younger counterparts. For Empathic Awareness (EA), Ethnic Identity (β = .45, p < .001) accounted for a significant proportion of the variability, R2 = .28, adjusted R2 = .19, F(6, 54) = 3.08, p < .05. This finding shows that ethnic minority participants who identify more with their own ethnic group have greater EA. The results are presented in Table 10. 42 Table 10 Predictors of Ethnocultural Empathy by Ethnic Group White American Variable B SE B Ethnic Minority β Partial B SE B β Partial Dependent Variable: EFE Age Gender a SES Generational Status Percent Diversity Ethnic Identity .04 .48 -.01 -.02 .11 .17 .01 .18 .03 .08 .48 .20 .42 .28 -.03 -.02 .03 .10 .40*** .27** -.03 -.02 .03 .09 .03 .19 .02 .05 1.12 .76 .02 .17 .03 .15 .50 .14 .22 .13 .07 .10 .27 .61 .19 .12 .07 .09 .23* .59** .02 .22 .03 .07 .65 .18 .31 -.06 .01 -.01 .00 .40 .28** -.05 .01 -.01 .00 .38** .02 .22 .03 .07 .66 .18 -.04 -.19 -.07 .02 -.06 .38 -.04 -.17 -.06 .02 -.05 .37 Dependent Variable: EPT Age Gender a SES Generational Status Percent Diversity Ethnic Identity .02 .18 -.01 -.07 .15 .03 .01 .22 .04 .10 .55 .23 .20 .10 -.03 -.10 .03 .02 .19 .10 -.03 -.09 .03 .02 .04 -.09 .00 -.00 .02 .55 Dependent Variable: ACDa Age Gender a SES Generational Status Percent Diversity Ethnic Identity .02 .32 -.02 -.15 -1.04 .00 .01 .26 .04 .11 .66 .28 .12 .14 -.05 -.16 -.19 .00 .12 .14 -.04 -.14 -.18 .00 -.01 -.29 -.02 .01 -.26 .51 Dependent Variable: EA .21 .20 .04 .02 .27 Age .02 .01 a Gender .16 .22 .09 .08 -.05 .21 -.03 SES .01 .04 .02 .01 .01 .03 .05 Generational Status -.02 .10 -.03 -.03 .02 .06 .04 Percent Diversity -.08 .57 -.02 -.02 .22 .62 .05 Ethnic Identity .33 .24 .18 .16 .59 .17 .45 Note: EFE = Empathic feeling and expression; EPT = Empathic perspective-taking; ACD = Acceptance of cultural differences; EA = Empathic awareness. a 0 = male, 1 = female. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. ap = .06. .23 -.03 .05 .04 .04 .43*** 43 Chapter 8 DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to examine the interrelationship among ethnic composition of social setting, ethnic identity, and ethnocultural empathy. The theoretical framework was guided by the work of Bronfrenbrenner (1979) concerning an ecological model of human behavior and the work of Waters (1990) and others concerning the macro factors that may shape the content and form of ethnicity. The ecological model suggests that human behavior occurs in a system of levels, from the micro to the macro, and the sociological work on ethnicity suggests that people’s understanding of ethnicity is embedded in that system of levels. Thus, the degree to which a setting is ethnically diverse may shape the ways in which people understand and identify with their ethnicity. We also drew on the literature on ethnic identity development to conceptualize and measure the ethnic identity of ethnic minority group members. Phinney’s (1989) ethnic identity model assumes that identification with one's ethnicity is based on the extent to which the individual explores issues related to one’s ethnic group and is characterized by three ethnic identity orientations: unexamined, search, and achieved. Unlike the sociological view of ethnicity, this model generally assumes that one’s ethnicity is relatively stable over time and across situations. Furthermore, we drew on the literature on ethnocultural empathy to conceptualize the multicultural attitudes that White American and ethnic minority group members may adopt over time and that may play a role in their 44 understanding of self and others. A large body of literature examines the importance of multicultural attitudes and the psychological and social factors that may be related to them in various clinical, business, and educational settings. Based on these areas of work, we examined for differences in ethnocultural empathy between White American and ethnic minority group members and among ethnic minority group members with varying ethnic identity orientations. We furthermore examined variables that predicted ethnocultural empathy for ethnic minorities and White Americans. In our first set of analyses, we expected differences in ethnocultural empathy between White American and ethnic minority group participants who lived in areas with varying ethnic compositions. Although we expected ethnic minority group respondents to have greater overall ethnocultural empathy than White American respondents, we believed the ethnic composition of social context would influence this level such that respondents living in more ethnically diverse areas would have greater ethnocultural empathy than those living in less diverse areas. In our second set of analyses examining differences among ethnic minority group members, we expected individuals with an achieved ethnic identity orientation to have greater ethnocultural empathy than would those with an unexamined and search ethnic identity orientation. However, we expected the ethnic composition to influence the level of ethnocultural empathy for people in the unexamined and search ethnic identity orientations. We believed that ethnocultural empathy would be greater for those in the unexamined ethnic identity orientation living in more diverse areas 45 compared to those living in less diverse areas. Similarly, we believed that those in the search ethnic identity orientation living in more diverse areas would have greater ethnocultural empathy than would those living in less diverse areas. Finally, in our last set of analyses, we anticipated differences in factors such as gender, age, SES, and generational status that would play a role in ethnocultural empathy for ethnic minorities and White Americans. We expected generational status and ethnic identity to predict ethnocultural empathy for ethnic minorities but not for White Americans. MANOVAs were employed to first examine differences in ethnocultural empathy between White American and ethnic minority participants who grew up in varying levels of ethnic diversity and among ethnic minority group members assigned to different ethnic identity orientation groups. Simultaneous multiple regression analyses were used to investigate differences in variables that may predict ethnocultural empathy for White American and ethnic minority participants. The results of these analyses are organized in the following manner. The first section presents findings concerning differences in ethnocultural empathy between White American and ethnic minority participants. The second section shares the results concerning differences in ethnocultural empathy among ethnic minority participants with varying ethnic identity orientations. This is followed by a discussion of predictors of ethnocultural empathy for White American and 46 ethnic minority participants. The final section addresses limitations and future directions for this line of research. Ethnocultural Empathy among White Americans and Ethnic Minority Group Members Our first goal was to examine any differences in ethnocultural empathy between White Americans and ethnic minority group members. The literature on ethnicity suggests that White Americans and ethnic minority group members have different experiences in relation to views and attitudes about their own and other ethnic groups. As we expected, White Americans and ethnic minority group members had differences in their ethnocultural empathy, with the former group reporting lower ethnocultural empathy than did the latter group. While one may assume that where these participants lived during their formative years, especially the degree of exposure to and interaction with other ethnic group members, may be related to their ethnocultural empathy, our results did not show any relationship to the degree of ethnic diversity of cities and towns they resided in and ethnocultural empathy. Thus, our expectations that ethnocultural empathy would depend on the percent diversity or ethnic composition failed to show any support. To interpret these findings with White American and ethnic minority group participants, we need to bear in mind the relative positioning of these groups which may afford varying experiences in relation to multiculturalism. Given the majority status of White Americans in the United States, it may be the case that the local ethnic composition may be less influential or important to them 47 compared to their ethnic minority counterparts. The literature shows that White Americans are generally less aware of their ethnicity and are rarely in situations where their ethnicity is made salient to them (Hardiman & Jackson, 1992; Helms, 1996; Phinney & Alipura, 1996). This is not to suggest that White Americans are not aware of their own and other’s ethnicity but rather that events at the macro level may be more influential than those at the micro levels. For example, even though a White American may live in an ethnically diverse area that may comprise close to 60 percent ethnic minority group members, the exposure of macro settings such as businesses and government being comprised mainly of White Americans may have a stronger effect on perceptions of ethnicity and multiculturalism. What is clear is that one’s affiliation with an ethnic group plays an important role in the level of ethnocultural empathy of participants. Ethnocultural Empathy and Ethnic Minority Group Members’ Ethnic Identity Orientation Although we expected differences in ethnocultural empathy between White Americans and ethnic minority group members, we also anticipated differences in ethnocultural empathy among ethnic minority group members with different ethnic identity orientations. Our expectations were partially supported with a strong ethnic identity orientation effect but little influence of percent diversity of social context. Respondents assigned to the unexamined ethnic identity orientation group distinguished themselves from those assigned to the search and achieved ethnic identity orientation groups by reporting lower levels 48 for three of the four ethnocultural empathy variables (EFE, EPT, and EA). With the fourth ethnocultural empathy variable, ACD, respondents assigned to the achieved ethnic identity orientation reported greater levels compared to the other two groups. Thus, with respect to ethnic identity orientation, we observed some differences in how people understood multicultural attitudes. The moderate differences that we observed with ethnic identity orientation was surprising given that conceptually individuals in the three orientations are at different places in their understanding of their own ethnic group. Consequently, we expected greater differences in ethnocultural empathy, especially between those individuals in the search and achieved ethnic identity orientation groups. However, other than ACD, these two groups did not differ in their reported ethnocultural empathy views. It may be the case that the measure used to operationalize these three ethnic identity orientation groups was not sensitive enough to reflect differences in participants’ experiences and views concerning multiculturalism. This is a point to which we will return later. Individuals in the unexamined ethnic identity orientation group displayed patterns similar to those of White Americans. According to the literature, these individuals have not had much exposure or given much thought to their own ethnic and cultural heritage. The literature suggests that the ethnic identity search stage is a volatile period for ethnic minorities as they try to negotiate different attitudes and emotions related to their own and other ethnic groups and to better understand their ethnic and cultural heritage. In general, the social context and especially the ethnic 49 composition is considered to be an important factor that can shape positive or negative views about their own ethnic group for these individuals. However, our findings did not support the role of ethnic composition for these individuals. Interestingly, people assigned to the achieved ethnic identity orientation group reported more acceptance of cultural differences compared to the other two ethnic identity orientation groups. Of the four ethnocultural empathy components, this is perhaps the most challenging area that people must negotiate with respect to multiculturalism. Whereas the other three components deal with awareness and empathy, this component deals with the actual acceptance of cultural differences. Predictors of Ethnocultural Empathy Separate simultaneous multiple regression analyses were conducted for White American and ethnic minority participants. Our findings showed that while three of the four models of ethnocultural empathy variables were significant (EFE, EPT, EA) for ethnic minority respondents, only one of the four models (EFE) was significant for the White American counterparts. Age appears to play a role in the ethnocultural empathy variables that reached significance or marginal significance such that respondents reported greater ethnocultural empathy with age. However, our results showed differences in gender and ethnic identity in relation to ethnocultural empathy between White American and ethnic minority participants. It appears that that gender plays a role for White Americans but not for ethnic minorities. This finding partially supports the cultural competency literature which shows that gender is an important variable shaping multicultural attitudes. With 50 ethnic identity appears to be an important predictor of ethnocultural empathy for ethnic minority group members but not for White Americans. This finding is consistent with the literature which suggests that ethnic identity is more important for ethnic minority group members than it is for White Americans (Kim & Liem; Phinney, 1996b), though ethnic identity is growing in importance for White Americans in specific settings (e.g., clinical). Surprisingly, percent diversity of social context was significant for only one of the ethnocultural empathy variables with ethnic minority participants. Given that greater exposure to other ethnic groups could result in more positive multicultural attitudes, the lack of stronger findings was surprising. However, findings suggest that ethnic identity may be an important variable for further examination in relation to ethnocultural empathy. To capture the degree of ethnic diversity in any particular location and linking it to variables such as ethnocultural empathy appears to be a more challenging task. Limitations and Future Directions Given the lack of clarity in our results, the findings presented in this study should be viewed with caution. In particular, the operationalization of the percent diversity of social context may have been less than ideal. If you recall, participants were asked to list up to four places they had lived in the United States with a zip code or the names of the cities and state(s) and indicate the number of years they had lived in each location. Drawing on data from the U.S. Census (2000), a percentage of the ethnic minority population was calculated for each location. The 51 percent diversity associated with the location with the most number of years was then used for subsequent analyses of ethnic composition of social context. For MANOVAs examining differences between ethnic groups and then among ethnic identity orientation groups, our assignment of participants into two groups based on their percent diversity may have been problematic. Although we used this method to separate participants into “more diverse” and “less diverse” locations and to preserve adequate sample sizes, there may not have been much separation between these two groups in terms of their experiences with cultural and ethnic diversity. Moreover, given that the sample was recruited from an area that is one of the most diverse in the U.S. may have made it more challenging to capture substantial differences along this dimension. Furthermore, given the relatively weak influence of the ethnic composition of social settings, it was difficult to assess whether or not these locations accurately captured the extent to which participants interacted with the degree of ethnic diversity reflected in the numbers. It may have been the case that participants residing in more diverse locations interacted more with same ethnic group members. Another limitation involves the period in which participants resided in the areas they indicated. These periods may not have reflected critical events or windows of opportunity where participants were socialized about their own and other ethnic groups. Future studies may want to include an item asking about the ethnic makeup of participants’ immediate social groups. 52 Another limitation concerns the operationalization of ethnic identity which was based on the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (Phinney, 1992) that assesses the strength of one’s ethnic identity. Although in principle applicable to all ethnic groups and used extensively (Lee, 2003; Simons et al., 2002), it is possible that it may not have captured participants’ ethnic identity accurately. For example, the MEIM assesses two main components of ethnic identity (ethnic identity achievement and sense of belonging), but does not pay much attention to racist experiences or minority group status. Given that some of the cities and towns were quite diverse with potential for ethnic tension, experiences with prejudice or discrimination may be important factors to consider with one’s ethnic identity. However, these kinds of experiences are not included in the MEIM. Conclusion Given the changing demographics, especially ethnic diversification, people’s understanding of their own and other ethnic groups is becoming increasingly important. This study attempted to examine how people’s attachment to their ethnicity and their attitudes toward other ethnic groups were shaped by their social context, especially the ethnic composition. Although our findings only partially supported our expectations, we did find important differences in the experiences of White Americans and ethnic minority group members. Future studies should continue to examine social context and individual ethnic variables and their relationship to fully understand the links between the individual and society in relation to ethnicity. 53 APPENDIX A INSTRUMENTS CONSENT TO PARTICPATE AS A RESEARCH SUBJECT You will be asked to fill out a series of questionnaires that examine attitudes, perceptions, and experiences related to social contexts. The research will take place online and will require 15 - 30 minutes of my time. I understand that I will receive extra credit points by participating in this study. I understand that this research may have the following benefits: By participating in this research, you may gain additional insight into the attitudes, perceptions, and experiences related to social contexts. Findings from this study will add to the body of literature on the relationship between social context and various social attitudes, perceptions, and experiences. I understand that the research may involve the following risks: Some of the items in the questionnaires may seem personal, but you do not have to answer any question you do not feel comfortable answering. Your honesty and openness are appreciated, but you have the final say in how much you reveal to us, and what questions you feel comfortable answering. If you have concerns related to any items in this research, you can contact Psychological Services at the Student Health Center. This information was explained to me by Patty Nguyen, graduate student in the Department of Psychology at California State University, Sacramento. I understand that she will answer any questions I may have now or later about this research. Patty Nguyen can be reached at pknnguyen@hotmail.com. I understand that I may discontinue my participation at any time without any penalty other than loss of research credit, and that the investigator may discontinue my participation at any time. If you agree to participate in this study, please fill in your initials and birthdate, and then proceed to the next page. Please respond as honestly as possible, relying on your current feelings of the particular issues raised. Your responses will be kept confidential. Your name will not be connected to specific results of the survey. All parts of the survey should be completed. 1. Please write the initials of your first and last name in the two spaces provided: ___ ___ 2. Please write your birth date in the spaces provided (month / day / year): ___ / ___ / ___ 54 INSTRUCTIONS: In this country, people come from many different countries and cultures, and there are many different words to describe the different backgrounds or ethnic groups that people come from. Some examples of the names of ethnic groups are Hispanic or Latino, Black or African American, Asian American, Chinese, Filipino, American Indian, Mexican American, Caucasian or White, Italian American, and many others. These questions are about your ethnicity or your ethnic group and how you feel about it or react to it. Please fill in: In terms of ethnic group, I consider myself to be Use the numbers given below to indicate how much you agree or disagree with each statement. strongly disagree 1 disagree 2 agree 3 strongly agree 4 strongly disagree disagree 1. I have spent time trying to find out more about my ethnic group, such as its history, traditions, and customs. 2. I am active in organizations or social groups that include mostly members of my own ethnic group. 3. I have a clear sense of my ethnic background and what it means for me. 4. I think a lot about how my life will be affected by my ethnic group membership. 5. I am happy that I am a member of the group I belong to. 6. I have a strong sense of belonging to my own ethnic group. 7. I understand pretty well what my ethnic group membership means to me, in terms of how to relate to my own group and other groups. 8. In order to learn more about my ethnic background, I have often talked to other people about my ethnic group. 9. I have a lot of pride in my ethnic group and its accomplishments. 10. I participate in cultural practices of my own group, such as special food, music, or customs. 11. I feel a strong attachment towards my own ethnic group. 12. I feel good about my cultural or ethnic background. agree strongly agree 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 55 INSTRUCTIONS: Below is a list of statements. Circle the number that best describes your response: strongly disagree that it describes me 1 2 3 4 strongly agree that it describes me 6 5 1. I feel annoyed when people do not speak standard English. 1 2 3 4 5 6 2. I don’t know a lot of information about important social and political events of racial and ethnic groups other than my own. 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. I am touched by movies or books about discrimination issues faced by racial or ethnic groups other than my own. 1 2 3 4 5 6 4. I know what it feels like to be the only person of a certain race or ethnicity in a group of people. 1 2 3 4 5 6 5. I get impatient when communicating with people from other racial or ethnic backgrounds, regardless of how well they speak English. 1 2 3 4 5 6 6. I can relate to the frustration that some people feel about having fewer opportunities due to their racial or ethnic backgrounds. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7. I am aware of institutional barriers (e.g., restricted opportunities for job promotion) that discriminate against racial or ethnic groups other than my own. 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. I don’t understand why people of different racial or ethnic backgrounds enjoy wearing traditional clothing. 1 2 3 4 5 6 9. I seek opportunities to speak with individuals of other racial or ethnic backgrounds about their experiences. 1 2 3 4 5 6 10. I feel irritated when people of different racial or ethnic backgrounds speak their language around me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 11. When I know my friends are treated unfairly because of their racial or ethnic backgrounds, I speak up for them. 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. I share the anger of those who face injustice because of their racial and ethnic backgrounds. 1 2 3 4 5 6 13. When I interact with people from other racial or ethnic backgrounds, I show my appreciation of their cultural norms. 1 2 3 4 5 6 14. I feel supportive of people of other racial and ethnic groups, if I think they are being taken advantage of. 1 2 3 4 5 6 15. I get disturbed when other people experience misfortunes due to their racial or ethnic backgrounds. 1 2 3 4 5 6 56 16. I rarely think about the impact of a racist or ethnic joke on the feelings of people who are targeted. 1 2 3 4 5 6 17. I am not likely to participate in events that promote equal rights for people of all racial and ethnic backgrounds. 1 2 3 4 5 6 18. I express my concern about discrimination to people from other racial or ethnic groups. 1 2 3 4 5 6 19. It is easy for me to understand what it would feel like to be a person of another racial or ethnic background other than my own. 1 2 3 4 5 6 20. I can see how other racial or ethnic groups are systematically oppressed in our society. 1 2 3 4 5 6 21. I don’t care if people make racist statements against other racial or ethnic groups. 1 2 3 4 5 6 22. When I see people who come from a different racial or ethnic background succeed in the public arena, I share their pride. 1 2 3 4 5 6 23. When other people struggle with racial or ethnic oppression, I share their frustration. 1 2 3 4 5 6 24. I recognize that the media often portrays people based on racial or ethnic stereotypes. 1 2 3 4 5 6 25. I am aware of how society differentially treats racial or ethnic groups other than my own. 1 2 3 4 5 6 26. I share the anger of people who are victims of hate crimes (e.g., intentional violence because of race or ethnicity). 1 2 3 4 5 6 27. I do not understand why people want to keep their indigenous racial or ethnic cultural traditions instead of trying to fit into the mainstream. 1 2 3 4 5 6 28. It is difficult for me to put myself in the shoes of someone who is racially and/or ethnically different from me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 29. I feel uncomfortable when I am around a significant number of people who are racially/ethnically different than me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 30. When I hear people make racist jokes, I tell them I am offended even though they are not referring to my racial or ethnic group. 1 2 3 4 5 6 31. It is difficult for me to relate to stories in which people talk about racial or ethnic discrimination they experience in their day to day lives. 1 2 3 4 5 6 57 INSTRUCTIONS: Please answer the following questions. 1. Your gender (check one): Male . Female . 2. Your ethnicity (pl ease check all those that apply): African American Asian American / Pacific Islander White (non-Latino/a) Latino/Latina Native Indian / Alaskan Native Other (please specify): 3. Year in School (1st, 2nd, 3rd, 4th, etc.): . . 4. Major(s): 5. What is your parents' yearly household income? a. less than $14,999 per year b. $15,000 - $24,999 per year c. $25,000 - $34,999 per year d. $35,000 - $44,999 per year e. $45,000 - $54,999 per year f. $55,000 - $64,999 per year g. $65,000 - $74,999 per year h. $75,000 - $84,999 per year i. $85,000 - $99,999 per year j. $100,000 or more per year 6. Please check ONE of the following that best describes your family history in the U.S. a. You, your parents, and your grandparents were born in another country other than the U.S. b. You were born in the U.S.; either or both parents, as well as grandparents, were born in another country. . 58 c. You were born in the U.S., both parents were born in the U.S. and all grandparents were born in another country. d. You were born in the U.S., both parents were born in the U.S. and at least one grandparent was born in another country. e. You, your parents, and all grandparents were born in the U.S. 7. If you chose (a) on question 6, where were you born? 8. If you chose (a) on question 6, how long have you lived in the U.S.? 9. Please list all of the places where you (and your family) have lived in the U.S. Please indicate by zip code (e.g., 95819) or by city and state (e.g., Sacramento, CA): * Enter at least 1 response and no more than 4 responses. Location 1: Location 2: Location 3: Location 4: 10. Years that you have lived in the location(s) above:* Enter at least 1 response and no more than 4 responses. Location 1: Location 2: Location 3: Location 4: 59 DEBRIEFING FORM PURPOSE: The purpose of this research is to examine the relationship among social context, ethnic identity, multicultural attitudes, and civic engagement. Specifically, this research examines how the ethnic and racial composition of social contexts may be related to ethnic identity, multicultural attitudes, and civic engagement through the use of questionnaires. HYPOTHESES AND SUPPORTING RESEARCH: Previous studies have found an influence of ethnic and racial composition on awareness of ethnicity for various ethnic and racial groups (Kim & Liem, 2003; Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994). As an extension of these studies, I examined how the ethnic and racial composition of social contexts may be related to ethnic identity and multicultural attitudes. I hypothesized that the ethnic and racial composition of where you grew up would be related to your ethnic identity and multicultural attitudes. I also hypothesized that multicultural attitudes would be related to greater civic engagement. To test our hypotheses, you received a series of questionnaires that assessed these areas. I will compare the relationship among these variables to identify any significant correlations. CONTACT INFORMATION: The results of this study will be available by December 2007. If you would like further information about the study or have questions regarding the experiment, please contact Patty Nguyen at pknnguyen@hotmail.com at your convenience. PSYCHOLOGICAL SERVICES: If this study evoked any painful memories or negative emotional responses that are troubling you, please contact the Student Health Center’s Psychological Services at 2786416. The counselors there will be happy to provide assistance to you. CLOSING: Thank you for participating! 60 REFERENCES Abe-Kim, J., Okazaki, S., & Goto, S. G. (2001). Unidimensional versus multidimensional approaches to the assessment of acculturation for Asian American populations. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 7(3), 232-246. Alvarez, A.N., Juang, L., & Liang, C.T.H. (2006). Asian Americans and racism: When bad things happen to “model minorities.” Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12(3), 477– 492. Berry, J. W. (1995). Psychology of acculturation. In N.R. Goldberger & J. B. Veroff (Eds.), The Culture and psychology reader (pp. 457-488). New York University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Christian, J., Gadfield, N., Giles, H., & Taylor, D. (1976). The multidimensional and dynamic nature of ethnic identity. International Journal of Psychology, 11, 281-291. Cameron, J.E., & Lalonde, R. N. (1994). Self, ethnicity, and social group memberships in two generations of Italian Canadians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 514-520. Collins, N.M., & Pieterse, A.L. (2007). Critical incident analysis based training: An approach for developing active racial/cultural awareness. Journal of Counseling & Development, 85, 14-23 61 Gurin, P., Hurtado, A., & Peng, T. (1994). Group contacts and ethnicity in the social identities of Mexicanos and Chicanos. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 521-532. Ekehammar, B., & Akrami, N. (2003). The relation between personality and prejudice: A variable- and a person-centred approach. European Journal of Personality, 17, 449-464. Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton. Ethier, K. A., & Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social identity in a changing context: Maintaining identification and responding to threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 243-251. Giles, H. (1977). Language, ethnicity and intergroup relations. London: Academic Press. Gurin, P., Hurtado, A., & Peng, T. (1994). Group contacts and ethnicity in the social identities of Mexicanos and Chicanos. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 521-532. Hardiman, R., & Jackson, B. (1992). Racial identity development: Understanding racial dynamics in college classrooms and on campus. In M. Adams (Ed.), Promoting diversity in college classrooms (pp. 21-37). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, Inc, Publishers. Haslam, S.A., Oakes, P.J., Reynolds, K.J., & Turner, J.C. (1999). Social identity salience and the emergence of stereotype consensus. Society for 62 Personality and Social Psychology, 25(7), 809-818. Flynn, F.J. (2005). Having an open mind: The impact of openness to experience on interracial attitudes and impression formation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88, 816–826. Helms, J. E. (1989). Considering some methodological issues in racial identity counseling research. Counseling Psychologist, 17, 227-252. Helms, J. E. (1990). Black and White racial identity. New York: Greenwood Press. Ivey, A. E., Ivey, M. B., & Simek-Downing, L. (1987). Individual and cultural empathy. In E. Ivey, M. B. Ivey, & L. Simek-Morgan (Eds.), Counseling and psychotherapy: Integrating skills, theory, and practice (2nd ed., pp. 91–118). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Kim, G.M., & Liem, R. (2003). Ethnic self-awareness as a function of ethnic group status, group composition, and ethnic identity orientation. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 9, 289-302. Kinket, B., & Verkuyten, M. (1997). Levels of ethnic self-identification and social context. Social Psychology Quarterly, 60, 338-354. Lee, R. M. (2003). Do ethnic identity and other-group orientation protect against discrimination for Asian Americans? Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 133–141. Marcia, J. (1966). Development and validation of ego-identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551-558. 63 Marcia, J. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 159-187). New York: Wiley. McGuire, W. J., McGuire, C. V., Child, P., & Fujioka, T. (1978). Salience of ethnicity in the spontaneous self-concept as a function of one's ethnic distinctiveness in the social environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 511-520. McGuire, W. J., & Padawer-Singer, A. (1976). Trait salience in the spontaneous self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 743-754. Miville, M.L., Holloway, P., Gelso, C.J., Pannu, R., Liu, W., Touradji, P., & Fuertes, J. (1999). Appreciating similarities and valuing differences: The Miville-Guzman universality-diversity scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology 46, 291-307. Munroe, A., & Pearson, C. (2006). The Munroe multicultural attitude scale questionnaire: A new instrument for multicultural studies. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, 819-834. Mossakowski, K. (2003). Coping with perceived discrimination: Does ethnic identity protect mental health?” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 44, 318-331. Nagel, J. (1994). Constructing ethnicity: Creating and recreating ethnic identity and culture. Social Problems, 41, 152-176. Negy, C., Shreve, Jensen, B.J., & Uddin, N. (2003). Ethnic identity, self-esteem, and ethnocentrism: A study of social identity versus multicultural theory of 64 development. Cultural Diversity and Mental Health, 9(4), 333-334. Peck, C., & Sears, A. (2005) Uncharted Territory: Mapping Children's Conceptions of Ethnic Diversity. Canadian Ethnic Studies/Études ethniques au Canada. Phinney, J. (1989). Stages of ethnic identity in minority group adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 9, 34-49. Phinney, J. (1990). Ethnic identity in adolescents and adults: Review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 499-514. Phinney, J. (1993). A three-stage model of ethnic identity development in adolescence. In M. Bernal & G. Knight (Eds.), Ethnic identity: Formation and transmission among Hispanics and other minorities (pp. 61-79). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Phinney, J. (1996a). When we talk about American ethnic groups, what do we mean? American Psychologist, 51, 918-927. Phinney, J. (1996b). Understanding ethnic diversity: The role of ethnic identity. American Behavioral Scientist, 40, 143-152. Phinney, J., & Alipuria, L. (1996). At the interface of cultures: Multiethnic/multiracial high school and college students. Journal of Social Psychology, 136, 130-158. Phinney, J., Cantu, C., & Kurtz, D. (1997). Ethnic and American identity as predictors of self-esteem among African American, Latino, and White adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 165-185. 65 Quintana, S. M., Ybarra, V. C., Gonzalez-Doupe, P., & Baessa, Y. D. (2000). Cross-cultural evaluation of ethnic perspective-taking ability: An exploratory investigation with U.S. Latino and Guatemalan Latino Children. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 6, 334–351. Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1998). What is beyond the Big Five? Journal of Personality, 66, 495-524. Sellers, R.M., Smith, M.A., Shelton, J.N., Rowley, R.A., & Chavous, T. M. (1997). Multidimensionality model of racial identity: A reconceptualization of African American racial identity. Unpublished Manuscript. University of Michigan. Silvestri, T. J., & Richardson, T. Q. (2001). White racial identity statuses and NEO personality constructs. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 79(1), 68-76. Simons, R. L., Murry, V., McLoyd, V., Lin, K.-H., Cutrona, C., & Conger, R. D. (2002). Discrimination, crime, ethnic identity, and parenting as correlates of depressive symptoms among African American children: A multilevel analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 371–393. Spencer. M. B., & Markstrom-Adams, C. (1990). Identity processes among racial and ethnic minority children in America. Child Development, 61, 290-310. Sue, D.W., Arredondo, P., & McDavis, R.J. (1992). Multicultural counseling competencies and standards: A call to the profession. Journal of Counseling and Development, 70(4), 477-486. 66 Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (1999). Counseling the culturally different: Theory and practice (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley. Strayer, J., & Eisenberg, N. (1987). Empathy viewed in context. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 389–398). New York: Cambridge University Press. Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology. Cambridge University Press. Tajfel, H. (1982). Instrumentality, identity and social comparisons. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social identity and intergroup relations (pp. 483-507). Cambridge University Press. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In S. Worchel & W.G. Austin (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-48). Monterey, Ca: Brooks-Cole. Umana-Taylor, D., & Updegraff, K.A. (1997). Latino adolescents’ mental health: Exploring the interrelations among discrimination, ethnic identity, cultural orientation, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Adolescence, 30, 549–567. Verkuyten, M. & Martinovic, B. (2006) Understanding multicultural attitudes: The role of group status, identification, friendships, and justifying ideologies. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30(1), 1-18. Wang, Y.W., Davidson, M.M., Yakushko, O.F., Savoy, H.B., Tan, J.A., & Bleier, J.K. (2003). The Scale of Ethnocultural Empathy: Development, 67 validation, and reliability. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50(2), 221234. Wolsko, C., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2000). Framing interethnic ideology: Effects of multicultural and colorblind perspectives on judgments of groups and individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 635-654. Waters, M. C. (1990). Ethnic options: Choosing identities in America. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Yates, M., & Youniss, J. (1999). Roots of civic identity: International perspectives on community service and activism in youth (pp. 16-31). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.