Supplementary Methods Permits Experiments were conducted under the following permits: Virginia Tech IACUC (10059-BIOL), Alabama Department of Conservation and Natural Resources (5436), United States Fish and Wildlife Service (MB158404-1), United States Geological Survey Bird Banding Lab (23513). Study Subjects, Housing, and Inoculation Twenty-eight female, hatch-year house finches were captured on the campus of Auburn University in Auburn, Alabama, USA (32° 25’ 29.78” N, 85° 29’13.59” W) on 9 and 10 September, 2011. Birds were transported via temperature-controlled state vehicle to Blacksburg, VA, a trip of roughly 8 hours, on 10 September, 2011. While only females were used in this study, previous work in this species has shown minimal to no sex effects on pathology or pathogen load in response to Mycoplasma gallisepticum (MG) infection, and no effects of sex on responses to novel antigens or on the total amount of food consumed [1-6], suggesting that results of the current study apply to both sexes. No birds showed clinical signs of MG or had detectable pathogen load (see methods below) prior to the start of the experiment. One MGtreated bird was excluded from the experiment when it was found to be seropositive for anti-MG antibodies prior to treatment [7]. One control bird was also found to be seropositive, but was retained in the analyses. Birds were housed in pairs in indoor cages (76cm x 46cm x 46cm) for two months at temperatures ranging between 20-23°C, then moved to individual cages of the same size two weeks prior to the start of the experiment. Ten days before experimental inoculation, birds were 1 placed into one of two temperature-controlled rooms, each with a lighting regime of 11hrs light and 13 hrs dark. Initial temperatures were a constant 22°C and were gradually adjusted on days 8, -6, and -4 prior to inoculation to yield final thermoneutral temperatures of 28°C during the day and 22°C at night, and final sub-thermoneutral temperatures of 19°C during the day and 13°C at night [8-10]. For thermoneutral birds, day-time temperatures were increased by 2°C on each adjustment day; for sub-thermoneutral birds, day-time temperatures were decreased by 1°C and night-time temperatures were decreased by 3°C on each of these adjustment days [10]. General Notes on Captive Studies of House Finches While field studies provide invaluable insights into disease dynamics, and MG in particular, a full understanding of such systems requires experimental work, which oftentimes must be conducted in captivity for both ethical and logistical reasons. However, captive studies of organismal responses to stressors, which can include disease, are justifiably criticised, and require some evidence that captive animals provide insight into their wild conspecifics [11]. Endocrine data suggest that experimental studies of captive house finches are viable tools for understanding organismal responses to MG in the wild. First, with acclimation periods to captivity comparable to or shorter than those in the current study, baseline and stress-induced corticosterone levels are highly similar between captive and free-living house finches. Both baseline and stress induced (30min restraint in cloth bags) corticosterone levels were comparable among free-living house finches in upstate NY [12] (means ± 1SE estimated from figure 3 in reference [12], baseline: 6.4 ± 1.3 ng/mL; stress-induced:18.9 ± 1.5 ng/mL) and those caught from the same population at the same time and held in captivity for 2-3 weeks [4] (means ± 1SE estimated from figure 3b in reference [4], baseline: 4.9 ± 1 ng/mL; stress-induced: 16.9 ± 2.5 2 ng/mL). Additionally, Adelman and Hawley (unpublished data) found that captive house finches have baseline corticosterone levels comparable to their free-living counter parts [12], both when healthy and infected with MG (males held in captivity for 3.5 months (n=4), means ± 1SE: PreInfection: 6.5 ± 1.1 ng/mL, Post-Infection: 12.4 ± 2.9 ng/mL; free-living: (values estimated from figure 3 in reference [12]: Healthy: 7.5 ± 1 ng/mL, Infected: 11 ± 5 ng/mL). Moreover, freeliving birds from AL (location of origin for birds in the current study) show similar baseline corticosterone levels as do birds from upstate NY, suggesting that comparisons across studies mentioned here are germane to the present experiment [12, 13]. Inoculation On day 0, birds were inoculated bilaterally in the palpebral conjunctiva with 40µL of Mycoplasma gallisepticum suspended in Frey’s media or media alone. We used the 7th in vitro passage of a MG isolate collected in Virginia, USA, in 1994 (7994-1 7P 2/12/09, David H. Ley, NC State University, College of Veterinary Medicine, Raleigh, NC, USA 27606), which had a viable count of 2.24 x 107 colour changing units (CCU), as determined by the most probable number method [14]. As this strain has been used for experimental infections in numerous publications, its use facilitates comparison among studies [15]. Moreover, this strain has been found circulating in wild house finch populations recently, indicating its continued ecological relevance [16]. Eye Lesion Scoring Eye lesions were scored on a scale of 0-3 per eye by two observers on each day of sampling. Briefly, scores corresponded to pathology as follows: 0) no pathology, 1) mild 3 swelling around the eye, 2) moderate swelling and mild eversion of the conjunctiva, 3) severe swelling and substantial discharge. Left and right eye scores were each averaged between observers and then summed to yield a total eye score, with a highest possible value of 6 [7] . Food Consumption Details Food consumption was calculated as: 𝑓𝑜𝑜𝑑 𝑐𝑜𝑛𝑠𝑢𝑚𝑒𝑑 = 𝑓𝑒𝑒𝑑𝑒𝑟 𝑚𝑎𝑠𝑠𝑖𝑛𝑖𝑡𝑖𝑎𝑙 – (𝑓𝑒𝑒𝑑𝑒𝑟 𝑚𝑎𝑠𝑠𝑓𝑖𝑛𝑎𝑙 + 𝑠𝑝𝑖𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑑 𝑓𝑜𝑜𝑑 𝑚𝑎𝑠𝑠) Foraging Behaviours During focal behavioural sampling of video, the number of times each bird pecked at the feeder port and the amount of time each bird spent within one body-width of the feeder were quantified using JWatcher [17]. Periods of time in which birds were immobile (no detectable movement), but within one body-width of the feeder were not counted as time at the feeder, as this inactivity precluded any feeding behaviour. Including these periods of inactivity did not qualitatively alter our results. Additional Statistical Details Linear mixed effects models were performed on conjunctival pathogen load, conjunctival lesions, food consumption, and foraging behaviours in R [18, 19]. Residuals from all models followed normal distributions, suggesting that assumptions of the techniques were met. Because our two measures of foraging behaviour, number of pecks at food and time spent within one body-width of the feeder, were significantly correlated (Spearman’s rho = 0.59, p < 0.001), we performed a principal components analysis to collapses these metrics into a single 4 variable to be used in analysis of pathogen deposition. Both behavioural metrics loaded with the same sign onto PC1, which explained 76% of the variation in these two variables (table S2). As such, we used PC1 as our metric of foraging behaviour in the additive mixed model explained below. Because time series data are often autocorrelated, mixed effects models of all variables included a function adjusting for temporal autocorrelation if this function improved overall fit of the maximal model, as assessed by AICc [20]. Autocorrelation functions tested were those included in the “nlme” package for R [19]. Because we were interested in the amount of pathogen from the conjunctiva that a bird deposited on a feeder, we assessed the effect of ambient temperature on pathogen deposition while controlling for individual conjunctival pathogen load, mass, and foraging behaviours using a generalized additive mixed effects model (GAMM). The initial model contained fixed effects of temperature treatment, mass (taken 2-4d prior to other metrics), PC1 (see above), and separate functions of pathogen deposition by conjunctival pathogen load (cubic spline smoothers) within each temperature treatment, incorporating bird ID as a random effect [21]. We simplified this model by first using only a single function of deposition by conjunctival load for all birds, then by removing the main effects of other variables, beginning with the highest p-values. The model fits were assessed using ΔAICc values; models with ΔAICc values < 2 were presumed to be significantly better fits to the data [20]. To determine how conjunctival pathology and food consumption affected pathogen deposition at the individual level, while controlling for all variables in the initial, maximal GAMM, we performed a linear mixed effects model on the residuals from that model, using total eye score and food consumption as main effects and bird ID as a random effect. 5 Effects of Temperature on MG Detection To determine if ambient temperature influenced our ability to detect MG DNA on inert substrates (e.g. bird feeders), we performed an in vitro incubation experiment. MG stock (isolate 2006.080-5-4P, (~2.5 X105 CCU/ml, David Ley, North Carolina State University) was serially diluted to yield four working solutions: full strength, 1:10 dilution, 1:100 dilution, and 1:1,000 dilution. We placed 5μL of each solution onto each of six replicate glass microscope slides. Three of the replicates from each dilution were incubated at 19oC, the other three at 28oC (the daytime temperatures for the sub-thermoneutral and thermoneutral groups of the main experiment, respectively). After nine hours (equivalent to the time between lights-on and feeder swabbing in the main experiment), slides were swabbed for 10s with a sterile cotton swab dipped in TPB, in the same manner that feeders were swabbed the main experiment. Swabs were then swirled and wrung out into a tube containing 100 l of TPB solution and stored at -20oC until DNA extraction the following day. DNA extraction and qPCR were performed as in the main methods section [7, 22]. Data were analyzed using ANOVA, with dilution, incubation temperature, and their interaction as main effects. Neither incubation temperature (F1,16 = 0.15, p = 0.71) nor the interaction between incubation temperature and dilution (F3,16 = 0.07, p = 0.75) influenced the amount of MG DNA detected (Figure S1). This result suggests that the observations of MG deposition in our main experiment were not biased by any effect of temperature on MG detection probability. 6 Supplementary Tables and Figures Table S1. Timeline of sampling during the experiment. MG load MG deposition food week day in eyes on feeders consumed 0 0 inoculation 1 4 X X X 2 12 X X X 3 17 X X X 4 24/25 X X X (MG = Mycoplasma gallisepticum, X = sampling performed) Table S2. Summary of the principal components analysis on food consumption and feeding behaviours. variable number of pecks at food proportion of time within one body-width of feeder cumulative proportion of variance explained rotation PC1 -0.71 rotation PC2 0.71 -0.71 -0.71 0.76 1.00 7 Figure S1. Ambient temperature did not influence our ability to detect Mycoplasma gallisepticum (MG) on glass slides using quantitative PCR. 5μL aliquots of four different dilutions of MG were incubated on glass slides for nine hours at either 19°C or 28°C. Bars show means ± 1SE for three replicate slides at each combination of temperature and dilution. 8 References 1. Altizer S, Davis AK, Cook KC, Cherry JJ. 2004 Age, sex, and season affect the risk of mycoplasmal conjunctivitis in a southeastern house finch population. Can. J. Zool. 82, 755-763. 2. Hawley DM, Jennelle CS, Sydenstricker KV, Dhondt AA. 2007 Pathogen resistance and immunocompetence covary with social status in house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus). Funct. Ecol. 21, 520-527. 3. Kollias GV, Sydenstricker KV, Kollias HW, Ley DH, Hosseini PR, Connolly V, Dhondt AA. 2004 Experimental infection of house finches with Mycoplasma gallisepticum. J. Wildl. Dis. 40, 79-86. 4. Hawley DM, Lindström K, Wikelski M. 2006 Experimentally increased social competition compromises humoral immune responses in house finches. Horm. Behav. 49, 417– 424. 5. Bascuñán AL, Tourville EA, Toomey MB, McGraw KJ. 2009 Food color preferences of molting house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) in relation to sex and plumage coloration. Ethology 115, 1066-1073. 6. Behbahaninia H, Butler MW, Toomey MB, McGraw KJ. 2012 Food color preferences against a dark, textured background vary in relation to sex and age in house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus). Behaviour 149, 51-65. 7. Hawley DM, Grodio J, Frasca S, Jr., Kirkpatrick L, Ley DH. 2011 Experimental infection of domestic canaries (Serinus canaria domestica) with Mycoplasma gallisepticum: a new model system for a wildlife disease. Avian Pathol. 40, 321-327. 8. Dawson WR, Buttemer WA, Carey C. 1985 A reexamination of the metabolic response of house finches to temperature. Condor 87, 424-427. 9 9. Root TL, Oconnor TP, Dawson WR. 1991 Standard metabolic level and insulative characteristics of eastern house finches, Carpodacus mexicanus (Müller). Physiol. Zool. 64, 1279-1295. 10. Hawley DM, DuRant SE, Wilson AF, Adelman JS, Hopkins WA. 2012 Additive metabolic costs of thermoregulation and pathogen infection. Funct. Ecol. 26, 701-710. 11. Calisi RM, Bentley GE. 2009 Lab and field experiments: Are they the same animal? Horm. Behav. 56, 1-10. 12. Lindström KM, Hawley DM, Davis AK, Wikelski M. 2005 Stress responses and disease in three wintering house finch (Carpodacus mexicanus) populations along a latitudinal gradient. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 143, 231-239. 13. Duckworth RA, Mendonca MT, Hill GE. 2001 A condition dependent link between testosterone and disease resistance in the house finch. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 268, 2467-2472. 14. Meynell GG, Meynell E. 1970 Theory and practice in experimental bacteriology. 2nd ed. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press; 347 p. 15. Hawley DM, Dhondt KV, Dobson AP, Grodio JL, Hochachka WM, Ley DH, Osnas EE, Schat KA, Dhondt AA. 2010 Common garden experiment reveals pathogen isolate but no host genetic diversity effect on the dynamics of an emerging wildlife disease. J. Evol. Biol. 23, 16801688. 16. Hochachka WM, Dhondt AA, Dobson A, Hawley DM, Ley DH, Lovette IJ. Multiple host transfers, but only one successful lineage in a continent-spanning emergent pathogen. In Review. 17. Blumstein DT, Evans CS, Daniel JC. 2006. JWatcher, v.1.0. www.jwatcher.ucla.edu. 18. R Development Core Team. 2011. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, v.2.13.1. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 10 19. Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. 2000 Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. Berlin, Springer; xvi, 528 p. 20. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. 2002 Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York, Springer; 488 p. 21. Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. 2009 Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York ; London, Springer; xxii, 574 p. p. 22. Grodio JL, Dhondt KV, O'Connell PH, Schat KA. 2008 Detection and quantification of Mycoplasma gallisepticum genome load in conjunctival samples of experimentally infected house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) using real-time polymerase chain reaction. Avian Pathol. 37, 385-391. 11