Imagination Library: Improving Literacy in Grant County Ann Harvey The Grant County Imagination Library http://www.imaginationlibrarygc.org/ is based in Silver City, the county seat of a large rural county in southwest New Mexico. The goal of the Grant County Imagination Library (IL) has been to deliver books monthly to homes of the local children from birth to age five and thereby promote literature experiences with language and reading. All of the children of the area are eligible to receive books. The parents are encouraged to enroll their children for the program online or at many community events. A local office is also maintained where the parents can enroll their child or ask questions about the program. Early exposure to books would help establish a basis for future success in school. The IL vision is that all children in the area would start Kindergarten ready and able to succeed with reading. The Imagination Library of Grant County oversees the delivery of approximately 1,400 free, age-appropriate, high-quality books each month to preschool children who are under the age of five. Almost eighty percent of the county’s eligible children have received a total of over 50,000 books since the first order in 2011. The organization’s tasks are to register preschool children, and to raise funds to pay for the books and their mailing. The Dollywood Foundation is responsible for the publishing and mailing of the books to the recipients. In 2011, Loren and Barbara Nelson completed a needs analysis of the underserved mining district of Grant County. From this study, five zip codes in the mining district surrounding Silver City, New Mexico, were identified as the areas with the greatest need for early childhood literacy intervention. Under the umbrella of a local 501(c)(3) literacy program, the Grant County program was created by completing the necessary arrangements with the Dollywood Foundation. The Nelsons provided seed money for eight children to be registered, and the first book order was placed. During the first annual appeal, one hundred letters were sent to community members and to local business. These letters produced forty-seven donations. After two years and a small grant from the Freeport McMoRan Copper and Gold Foundation, all sixteen zip codes in the county were being served. The cost of delivering a book to a child is approximately $2.10. All parents are encouraged to enroll children using this format: 1. Parents register their child with the Imagination Library and promise to read to them. 2. The child’s name along with the child’s birth date and address are entered in the Dollywood Foundation’s database in Dolly Parton’s home town of Pigeon Forge, Tennessee. 3. On the first of each month, the Dollywood Foundation sends the list of eligible children and the appropriate book titles to the Penguin Book Group in New York. 4. After printing the books, the Penguin Book Group sends them to a mail house in Knoxville, Tennessee, where the books are shrink-wrapped, collated, addressed to the child and sent by bulk mail to post offices across the country. 5. The local mail carrier delivers the book to the child. __________________________ Picture 1 here __________________________ The Imagination Library Picture Books The IL books follow a theme of inspiration and imagination with love of reading and learning; regard for diversity of people, their roles, culture, and environment; promotion of selfesteem and confidence, and appreciation of art and aesthetics. Each series of books contains specific characteristics, which are appropriate for a developmental level. Year One books emphasize the senses of touch with board pages and vision with bright big and colorful pages. The text is minimal to encourage point and say activities. Other text has rhyme, rhythm, lullabies, and playful sounds to emphasize listening. Year 1 Sample Books Giggles With Daddy Gingerbread Man Look Look! The books chosen for Year Two continue and build on the concepts already introduced. The text is predictable with repetition. The books encourage body awareness with real photo illustrations and motor skills with mention of self-help skills. They introduce colors, letters, and numbers. All of Baby Nose to Toes Year 2 Sample Books A Mud Pie for Mother ABC Look At Me In Year Three, wordless books are offered for language development. Themes of the books include character development and values. Bibliotherapy books addressing issues of fear, conflict, love, and safety are included. Year 3 Sample Books Roar of a Snore Tomie dePaola’s Mother Where’s My T-R-U-C-K? During Year Four, more complex stories are offered with problems and heroes who resolve them. Different faces and environments introduce the concept of diverse peoples. The theme that being different is OK resonates with play, humor, and fun in books chosen for this stage. Year 4 Sample Books Bringing the Rain to the Kapiti Plain Llama Llama Misses Mama The Snowy Day Year 5 introduces school preparation and readiness. Books which use picture for words (rebus) help with print awareness. Genres include science, non-fiction, folk tales, and poetry. Year Five Sample Books Mary and Her Little Lamb Owl Moon The Tale of Peter Rabbit When a child registers for the Imagination library, they receive Dolly Parton’s favorite, The Little Engine That Could as their first book and at age five graduate with Look Out Kindergarten, Here I Come! A child who is registered at birth and graduates at age five can collect up to 60 free books for their very own library. Literature Review The connection between reading aloud to young children and their success in school has been confirmed for some time. A direct relationship exists between children whose parents read to them frequently and those who learn to read easily. “Early childhood experiences shape language development and are associated with school readiness, early reading success, and effective grade progression” (Lawhon, 2000, p. 5). Even for very young children, reading seemed to positively influence the development of both vocabulary and cognition skills. In the Raikes, et al. (2006) study, reading to very young children showed gains in vocabulary and comprehension at 14 months and gains in vocabulary cognitive development at 24 months. “A pattern of daily reading over the three data points for English-speaking children and daily reading at any one data point for Spanish-speaking children predicted children's language and cognition at 36 months. Path analyses suggest reciprocal and snowballing relations between maternal book reading and children's vocabulary development” (Raikes, et. al, 2006, p. 950). Children who develop good oral language skills “before entering first grade become highability readers and those who develop low oral language skills eventually become low-ability readers” (Lonigan , Burgess, & Anthony, 2000, p. 613). Children with poorly developed oral language skills tend to come from low socio-economic families. Conversely, the children with well-developed oral language skills usually come from high socio-economic families. Higher socio-economic families have greater resources to enhance their children’s learning by purchasing books or taking them to a museum. Lower income families often do not have the time or resources to offer these benefits, especially when they live in a remote rural community. Hart and Risley wrote, “Simply in words heard, the average low SES child was having half as much experience per hour (616 words per hour) as the average SES child (1,251 words per hour) and less than one-third that of the average child in a high SES family with 2,153 words per hour” (Stight, 2011, p.36). Therefore, the more words children hear before starting school, the more successful they will be in reading and writing. The boost to low socio-economic children’s language development, as suggested by the Raikes, et al. (2006) study, comes when they hear their parents read to them using vocabulary that is often more complex than that used in daily conversation. According to a longevity study by Evans, Kelly, Sikora, and Treiman (2010) at the University of Nevada, the number of books in the home predicted the level of education of a child more accurately than did the educational level of the parents. “Children of lesser educated parents benefited the most from having books in the home” (Evans, Kelly, Sikora, & Treiman, 2010, p. 180). If books are available in the home, parents are more likely to participate in reading time with their child (Ridzi, Sylvia, & Singh, 2011). Reading books aloud during interactive reading practices further improves the child’s chances for school success. “Previous research indicates that greater parent child reading practices predict greater receptive vocabulary, understanding of story and print concepts, and pre-literacy skills among low-income children” (Bracken & Fischel, 2008, p.50). Several studies have noted the influence of the IL on school readiness and early literacy behavior upon entry into Kindergarten. The Tennessee Board of Regents conducted a web-based survey of 150 teachers who evaluated 320 entry-level kindergarteners divided into groups of those who had participated in the IL program and those who had not. A five-point rating scale was used to measure overall learning preparedness including reading, thinking, listening, and social skills. “Teachers were asked to consider all students in each group as a whole, and compare the students to those in previous classes”(Tennessee Board of Regents, 2008b, p. 1). While the study was not controlled for other preschool experiences, the results produced higher scores for the IL group over the four measures. Reading Skills proved to produce the biggest gains, where IL students scored .86 points higher on a scale of five. Similar research was conducted recently in Shelby County, TN, and tested the sustained effects of students who were exposed to early storybook reading by comparing second grade reading scores. One hundred seventy (170) of these students had participated in Books from Birth prior to kindergarten entry and 164 students had not participated in the program. Those students who had participated in the Books from Birth program prior to kindergarten entry had higher scores in reading development on the Istation Early Reading measure (http://www.istation.co) in second grade. The Books from Birth group scored higher on the subtests of vocabulary and reading comprehension scores compared to students who had not participated at a p= .02 level of significance. These differences remained even after controlling for socioeconomic status, gender, mobility and attendance rates (Sell, Imig, & Samiei, 2014, p. 2). There was no difference between the spelling scores of the two groups. Higher vocabulary and comprehension scores with lower spelling scores would be expected because reading a story exposed children to the context of the story and provided an automatic scaffold for learning new words. If the parents discuss the illustrations and identify the actions of the characters in the story, even more opportunity for learning new vocabulary is offered. The body of literature concerning results from IL participation demonstrates a strong link between participation in the program and early literacy success. Parent Surveys The success of the Grant County Imagination Library program had been measured by the parents’ response to surveys, which have been consistently positive. A ten-question survey was sent to 469 parents (35% of 1325 book recipients). A 20 percent response rate with an n= 93 was produced. The survey was sent to email addresses that were provided using MailChimp and SurveyMonkey and the Dollywood Foundation’s secure database. The Likert Scale analysis of question 1 revealed that before entering the program, the average family read to their child only three or four times a week. After entering the program, the results of question two indicate that the average family read to their child once a day (Harvey, 2014). According to the results of the Likert Scale analysis of question three, the average family reported that their child asked to be read to more often after entering the program. This question demonstrates the connection between the exposure to books and the motivation for reading them. The bonding that occurs during read-aloud time makes reading a comforting experience. An increased interest in reading books was also recorded by question four. The results of the Likert Scale analysis demonstrated that most families reported that their child was more interested in reading after entering the program. Almost half (47%) of children were reported to have more interest in reading. Both questions four and five focused on the motivation to read and the bonding process. These responses demonstrate that family time spent sharing books had a positive impact on the family (Harvey, 2014). __________________________ Picture 2 here __________________________ Question seven asked about the number of family members who read the Imagination Library books. As many as 67% of the families reported that more than one person in the household benefited from the program, according to the results of the Likert Scale analysis. Often more than one member of the family received books, and this doubled the exposure to the number of books read to the child. Open-ended responses show that older children enjoyed receiving books in the mail so that they could read to the younger children. For example, several responses suggested an increase of family bonding after reading the books together: “We read the books when they come in the mail instead of watching TV.” “It gives me a reason to spend more quality time with my child.” Fathers also spent more time reading to the children (Harvey, 2014, p.8). The parent survey provided needed validation for the operation of the program. Conclusion The literature reflects the fact that reading to children helps to prepare them for learning to read. Children bond with parents during this time and learn to quiet themselves to pay attention to the story (Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University working paper 11, 2011, p. 3). The children also gain new vocabulary and see that reading is important because it is an activity in which their parents participate. The Imagination Library has influenced parents and encouraged them to spend time reading to their children. Having a book arrive in the mail is an added incentive for them to spend time reading as a family. Community funders and policy makers should continue to support this worthwhile literacy organization. References Bracken, S.S., & Fischel, J.E. (2008). Family reading behavior and early literacy skills and preschool children from low-income backgrounds. Early Education and Development, 19(1), 45-67. Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University (2011). Building the Brain’s “Air Traffic Control” System: How Early Experiences Shape the Development of Executive Function: Working Paper No. 11. Retrieved from www.developingchild.harvard.edu Evans, M.D., Kelly, J., Sikora, J., & Treiman, D. J. (2010). Family scholarly culture and educational success: Evidence from 27 nations. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 28(2), 171-197. DOI: 10.1016/j.rssm.2010.01.002 Evaluating the relationship between the Imagination Library early childhood literacy program and kindergarten readiness. (2011). Retrieved from: http://usa.imaginationlibrary.com/ medias/SCBFB%20IL%20Final%20for%20Resubmission%20SS.pdf Harvey, A. (2014). Engaging Parents with Early Literacy Practices. Unpublished paper. Lawhon, T. (2000). Creating Language and Print Awareness environments for Young Children. Contemporary Education, 71(3), 5. Lonigan C.J. , Burgess S. R., Anthony J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 36(5), 596-613. NM Public Education Department. (2014). NAEP Reading 2013 State Snapshot Report New Mexico Grade 4 Public Schools. Retrieved from: http://ped.state.nm.us/ped/NAEP_index.html Raikes, H., Pan, B. A., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., Tarullo, L. B., Raikes, H. A., & Rodriguez, E. (2006). Mother–child bookreading in low-income families: Correlated and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77(4), 924-953. Ridzi, F. Sylvia, M. R. & Singh, S. (2011). Imagination Library: Do More Books in Hand Mean More Shared Book Reading? Retrieved from: http://usa.imaginationlibrary.com/medias/SCBFB%20IL%20Final%20for%20Resubmissio n%20SS.pdf Sell, M., Imig, D., & Samiei, S. (2014). Links between Books from Birth participation and second-grade reading performance. Retrieved from http://www.urbanchildinstitute.org/articles/updates/links-between-books-from-birthparticipation-and-second-gradereading Sticht, T.G. (2011). Getting It Right from the Start: The Case for Early Parenthood Education. American Educator, 35(1), 35-39. Tennessee Board of Regents Tennessee’s Imagination Library Improves School Preparedness.(2006) Retrieved from: https://usa.imaginationlibrary.com/medias/file/ books_from_birth.pdf Children’s Books Cited Carlson, N. (2001). Look Out Kindergarten, Here I Come! Puffin Books. Piper, W. (1930; 1991, 2011). The Little Engine That Could. Grosset & Dunlap Ann Harvey is Professor of Reading at Western New Mexico University, Silver City, New Mexico. Girl with the book The Little Engine That Could. Mother Rosie with two children and their new books Dolphins Sands and The Little Engine That Could