Rejection Sensitivity and Negative Relationships in Adolescent Offspring of Mothers with Borderline Personality Disorder Jennifer M. Strimpfel, Chris D. Watkins, Christina G. Mena, Amineh Abbas, & Jenny Macfie Presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting April 2013, Seattle, WA Abstract This study assessed rejection sensitivity and borderline features (affective instability, identity problems, negative relationships, and self-harm) in adolescents at risk for developing Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD). A low SES sample of adolescent offspring of women with BPD age 14-18, n = 27, was compared with a normative comparison group, n = 28. We found no significant difference in levels of rejection sensitivity between the offspring of women with BPD and the normative comparisons. However, in the sample as a whole, there was a significant positive correlation between the adolescent participants’ level of rejection sensitivity and their own self-reported borderline feature of negative relationships. Moreover, female adolescent participants reported higher levels of rejection sensitivity than did the male participants. These results lend support to the “self-fulfilling prophecy” model explaining negative relationship seen in individuals high in rejection sensitivity (Downey & Feldman, 1996). Introduction Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is a chronic and severe disorder that is associated with high suicide risk and high utilization of mental health resources. Symptoms of BPD reflect difficulties with self-regulation (impulsivity, affective instability, difficulty controlling anger, suicidal gestures), with a stable sense of self (unstable self-image, feelings of emptiness, dissociative symptoms) and with relationships (fear of abandonment, intense and unstable relationships) that first manifest in late adolescence to early adulthood (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). From a developmental psychopathology perspective, the study of offspring of mothers with BPD, who are at risk from both genetic and environmental factors, may inform pathways to this disorder (Lenzenweger & Cicchetti, 2005). Rejection Sensitivity is defined as “anxious expectations of rejection in situations that afford the possibility of rejection by significant others”(Downey & Feldman, 1996). A high level of rejection sensitivity is conceptually similar to “frantic efforts to avoid real or imagined abandonment”, a symptom of BPD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Recent research has shown that individuals with BPD have higher levels of rejection sensitivity than do normative comparisons (Berenson, Downey, Rafaeli, Coifman, & Paquin, 2011; Selby, Ward, & Joiner, 2010), and that higher levels of rejection sensitivity are related to borderline features in non-clinical samples of young adults (Ayduk et al., 2008; Tragesser, Lippman, Trull, & Barrett, 2008). Although there has been previous research linking high levels of rejection sensitivity in adolescence to externalizing behaviors, aggression, and poor school outcomes, (Downey, Lebolt, Rincón, & Freitas, 1998), as well as depression and anxiety (Harper, Dickson, & Welsh, 2006; Marston, Hare, & Allen, 2010; McCarty, Vander Stoep, & McCauley, 2007; McDonald, Bowker, Rubin, Laursen, & Duchene, 2010), there has been no study of rejection sensitivity in the adolescent offspring of mothers with BPD. This is an important omission, because high levels of rejection sensitivity may place these adolescents at a higher risk for developing symptoms of BPD themselves. In individuals with BPD, difficulties in relationships, reflected in a fear of abandonment, may be related to rejection sensitivity. Increased levels of rejection sensitivity in individuals with BPD may explain their difficulties with relationships through the “self-fulfilling prophecy” model of rejection sensitivity first proposed by Downey, et al., (1996). An individual with high rejection sensitivity may perceive rejection in ambiguous situations and then over-react with hostility and aggression towards the person that they perceived to be rejecting them (Downey, Irwin, Ramsay, & Ayduk, 2004). The individual’s hostile reaction to perceived rejection then may cause interpersonal difficulties and actual rejection from a significant other, sustaining or increasing their level of rejection sensitivity: in effect, a “self-fulfilling prophecy” (Downey, Freitas, Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998). Several studies have supported the “self-fulfilling prophecy” model of rejection sensitivity in adults (Ayduk, Downey, Testa, Yen, & Shoda, 1999; Downey & Feldman, 1996; Downey, Freitas, et al., 1998). Recent research has also supported the link between high levels of rejection sensitivity and difficulties in romantic relationships in adolescents (Galliher & Bentley, 2010; Purdie & Downey, 2000; Smith, Welsh, & Fite, 2010; Volz & Kerig, 2010). Among early adolescent girls, high levels of rejection sensitivity predicted higher insecurity about their partner’s commitment to the relationship (Purdie & Downey, 2000). In adolescents who were in romantic relationships, higher levels of rejection sensitivity was related to higher levels of aggression in the relationship and lower levels of relationship satisfaction (Galliher & Bentley, 2010). Another study of adolescents found that the relationship between high levels of rejection sensitivity and perpetrating dating violence was fully mediated by insecurity about the relationship (Volz & Kerig, 2010). In addition to a positive relationship between overall borderline features and rejection sensitivity, in the present study, we expected rejection sensitivity to be positively correlated with the borderline feature of negative relationships in the adolescent participants, because of the self-fulfilling prophecy model. In addition to BPD group differences and the relationship between rejection sensitivity and the borderline feature of negative relationships, we were also interested in gender differences. Although in community samples, BPD is found equally in both men and women, women diagnosed with BPD are lower functioning than are men diagnosed with BPD, and are more likely to seek treatment (Grant et al., 2008). Previous research on gender differences in rejection sensitivity has been mixed. A previous study has shown no gender differences in levels of rejection sensitivity in preadolescents (Downey, Lebolt, et al., 1998). In college students, several studies have shown no gender differences in levels of rejection sensitivity (Ayduk, Gyurak, & Luerssen, 2009; Downey & Feldman, 1996; Mellin, 2008), while one large study of 500 college students showed that females had higher levels of rejection sensitivity (Erozkan, 2009). Of studies of rejection sensitivity in mid- to late-adolescence, one previous study has shown higher levels of rejection sensitivity in 16-17 year old boys, with gender differences disappearing by age 18 (Marston et al., 2010). Another study of rejection sensitivity in mid-adolescence showed that females had higher levels of rejection sensitivity (Volz & Kerig, 2010). In the present study population encompassing the period of mid-adolescence, we anticipated that there would be a gender difference in rejection sensitivity, with females reporting higher levels of rejection sensitivity than males. In testing hypotheses, we controlled for Major Depressive Disorder (MDD), because it is the most frequently found disorder to be co-morbid with BPD (Zanarini et al., 1998) and because of the negative effects that maternal MDD can have on their children’s development (Downey & Coyne, 1990). We hypothesized that 1) adolescents whose mothers have BPD have higher levels of rejection sensitivity than normative comparisons, when controlling for maternal Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) diagnosis; 2) adolescents’ rejection sensitivity is positively correlated with their own overall borderline features score and subscale scores of affective instability, identity problems, negative relationships, and self-harm; 3) female adolescents report higher levels of rejection sensitivity than male adolescents. Method Participants: The sample consisted of adolescents age 14-18, n = 27 whose mothers have BPD and n = 28 normative comparisons. The only significant difference in the demographics data was that the mothers in the BPD group were less likely to have a high school diploma or GED than were the mothers in the comparison group. See Table 1. Attainment of a high school diploma or GED was not significantly correlated with the dependent variables, however, so it was not controlled for in analyses. Measures: Demographics. Demographic information for this study was collected through an interview with the mothers (Mt. Hope Family Center, 1995). BPD Diagnosis. Mother participants were interviewed using the SCID-II Structured Clinical Interview (First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997). MDD Diagnosis. Mother participants were interviewed using the SCID-I Structured Clinical Interview (First, Gibbon, Spitzer, & Williams, 2002). Borderline Features. Adolescent participants completed the Borderline Features Scale of the PAI (PAI-BOR). The PAI-BOR has four subscales that measure four factors common in BPD: affective instability, identity problems, negative relationships, and self-harm (Morey, 1991). Rejection Sensitivity. Adolescent participants completed a modified version of the Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire (RSQ-R), a self-report measure of rejection sensitivity (Downey & Feldman, 1996). Results Hypothesis 1. The results did not support hypothesis 1, as there was no significant difference between the mean RSQ-R scores in the BPD group (M = 10.02, SD = 3.72) and the comparison group (M = 9.74, SD = 2.72), F(1,52) = 0.30, p > .10, when controlling for maternal MDD diagnosis. Hypothesis 2. As hypothesized, the adolescents’ level of rejection sensitivity was significantly correlated (p < .01) with their own negative relationships, and marginally correlated (p < .10) with their own identity problems and overall borderline features. However, contrary to prediction, adolescents’ level of rejection sensitivity was not significantly correlated with their own affective instability and self-harm. See Table 3. Hypothesis 3. Rejection sensitivity was significantly higher in girls (M = 10.80, SD = 3.73) than in boys (M = 8.99, SD = 2.38), t(53) = 2.15, p = < .05 Discussion Group differences in RS reported by the adolescents in this study were not significant. Although not investigated in the present study, relationships with caregivers other than the mother could have shaped these adolescents’ internal working models of viewing others as rejecting or not. Also, the participants in both the BPD group and the comparison group of the present study all had low family SES. Low SES is related to increased risk of family violence, possibly due to risk variables associated with low SES such as parental stress (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). Exposure to family violence has been found to be related to higher levels of RS (Feldman & Downey, 1994). Exposure to family violence may in part explain the similar levels of RS found in both groups in the current study. Future research could involve families across the socioeconomic spectrum to determine if early exposure to violence in low SES families might confound group differences in rejection sensitivity between adolescents who have a parent with BPD versus normative comparisons. In the present study, rejection sensitivity was higher in the adolescent girls than in the adolescent boys. There is a need for more research on gender differences in rejection sensitivity in middle to late adolescence. Further research should explore the possible implications of higher levels of rejection sensitivity in women on the observed gender differences in the severity of BPD in adults, such that women diagnosed with BPD report higher levels of disability due to the disorder than men diagnosed with BPD (Grant et al., 2008). The present finding that the adolescents’ self-reported rejection sensitivity was positively correlated with their negative relationships supports the “self-fulfilling prophecy” model first proposed by Downey, et al., (1996). The present finding that levels of rejection sensitivity were correlated with negative relationships in adolescents in the sample as a whole may have important clinical implications. Interventions could focus on cognitive approaches to challenge the tendency to over-perceive rejection in adolescents high in rejection sensitivity in order to break the “self-fulfilling prophecy” prior to the development of more severe relationship difficulties. To learn more about the Child and Adolescent Development Lab, please visit : http://web.utk.edu/~macfie/ Table 1 Demographic Information _________________________________________________________________ Variable BPD Group n = 27 M (SD) Comparison Group n = 28 M (SD) t(53)_ Family Income ($) 22,988 (12,769) 28,668 (16,081) 1.447 Adolescent Age (years) 15.26 (1.13) 15.69 (1.26) 1.345 Number of Adults In the Home 1.70 (0.67) 1.93 (0.77) 1.158 Number of Children In the Home 2.22 (1.37) 2.54 (1.67) 0.775 Χ² Minority Ethnic Status of Adolescent 4% 11% 1.002 Female Adolescents 48% 50% 0.019 Mother has GED/ H.S. Diploma 70% 100% 9.708** Mother has Partner 71% 67% 0.146__ ** = p < .01 Table 2 Bivariate Correlations Between Adolescents’ level of Rejection Sensitivity and Adolescents’ Borderline Features ______________________________________________________________________________ Variable n M SD Total BPD Affective Features Instability Identity Problems Negative Relationships Self Harm Rejection 55 9.88 3.22 .25† .20 .22† .47** -.05 Sensitivity ______________________________________________________________________________ † = p < .10 ** = p < .01 References American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Ayduk, O., Downey, G., Testa, A., Yen, Y., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Does rejection elicit hostility in rejection sensitive women? Social Cognition. Special Issue: Social cognition and relationships, 17(2), 245-271. Ayduk, O., Gyurak, A., & Luerssen, A. (2009). Rejection sensitivity moderates the impact of rejection on self-concept clarity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(11), 1467-1478. Ayduk, O., Zayas, V., Downey, G., Cole, A. B., Shoda, Y., & Mischel, W. (2008). Rejection sensitivity and executive control: Joint predictors of borderline personality features. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(1), 151-168. Berenson, K. R., Downey, G., Rafaeli, E., Coifman, K. G., & Paquin, N. L. (2011). The rejection– rage contingency in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(3), 681-690. Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371-399. Downey, G., & Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of depressed parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 108(1), 50-76. Downey, G., & Feldman, S. I. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1327-1343. Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 545-560. Downey, G., Irwin, L., Ramsay, M., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Rejection Sensitivity and Girls' Aggression. In M. Moretti, C. Odgers & M. Jackson (Eds.), Girls and Aggression: Contributing Factors and Intervention Principles. Washington, DC: American Psychological Press. Downey, G., Lebolt, A., Rincón, C., & Freitas, A. L. (1998). Rejection sensitivity and children's interpersonal difficulties. Child Development, 69(4), 1074-1091. Erozkan, A. (2009). Rejection sensitivity levels with respect to attachment styles, gender, and parenting styles: A study with Turkish students. Social Behavior and Personality, 37(1), 114. Feldman, S. I., & Downey, G. (1994). Rejection sensitivity as a mediator of the impact of childhood exposure to family violence on adult attachment behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 231-247. First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2002). User's Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders-Research Version (SCID-I for DSM-IV-TR, Nov. 2002 Revision). First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997). User's Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Galliher, R. V., & Bentley, C. G. (2010). Links between rejection sensitivity and adolescent romantic relationship functioning: The mediating role of problem-solving behaviors. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 19(6), 603-623. Grant, B. F., Chou, S. P., Goldstein, R. B., Huang, B., Stinson, F. S., Saha, T. D., . . . Ruan, W. J. (2008). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(4), 533-545. doi: 10.1016/800063223(02)01324-0 Harper, M. S., Dickson, J. W., & Welsh, D. P. (2006). Self-Silencing and Rejection Sensitivity in Adolescent Romantic Relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35(3), 459-467. Lenzenweger, M. F., & Cicchetti, D. (2005). Toward a developmental psychopathology approach to borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 17(4), 893-898. Marston, E. G., Hare, A., & Allen, J. P. (2010). Rejection sensitivity in late adolescence: Social and emotional sequelae. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(4), 959-982. McCarty, C. A., Vander Stoep, A., & McCauley, E. (2007). Cognitive features associated with depressive symptoms in adolescence: Directionality and specificity. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(2), 147-158. McDonald, K. L., Bowker, J. C., Rubin, K. H., Laursen, B., & Duchene, M. S. (2010). Interactions between rejection sensitivity and supportive relationships in the prediction of adolescents’ internalizing difficulties. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(5), 563-574. Mellin, E. A. (2008). Rejection sensitivity and college student depression: Findings and implications for counseling. Journal of College Counseling, 11(1), 32-41. Morey, L. C. (1991). Personality Assessment Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Mt. Hope Family Center. (1995). Mt. Hope Demographic Interview. University of Rochester. Purdie, V., & Downey, G. (2000). Rejection sensitivity and adolescent girls' vulnerability to relationship-centered difficulties. Child Maltreatment, 5(4), 338-349. Selby, E. A., Ward, A. C., & Joiner, T. E. (2010). Dysregulated eating behaviors in borderline personality disorder: Are rejection sensitivity and emotion dysregulation linking mechanisms? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 43(7), 667-670. Smith, J. D., Welsh, D. P., & Fite, P. J. (2010). Adolescents' relational schemas and their subjective understanding of romantic relationship interactions. Journal of Adolescence, 33(1), 147-157. Tragesser, S. L., Lippman, L. G., Trull, T. J., & Barrett, K. C. (2008). Borderline personality disorder features and cognitive, emotional, and predicted behavioral reactions to teasing. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(6), 1512-1523. Volz, A. R., & Kerig, P. K. (2010). Relational dynamics associated with adolescent dating violence: The roles of rejection sensitivity and relational insecurity. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 19(6), 587-602. Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Dubo, E. D., Sickel, A. E., Trikha, A., Levin, A., & Reynolds, V. (1998). Axis I comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(12), 1733-1739.