Water maze topics of interest to the Laboratory of Comparative Neuropsychology (LCN) 1) Multiple memory systems – This is the original research area the LCN. Brain structures such as the hippocampus and striatum are important for different kinds of learning and memory – cognitive vs habit formation, respectively (Petri & Mishkin, 1994). To our surprise, we found that the hippocampus and striatum both make important contributions to the place navigation in the water maze (Devan, Goad, & Petri, 1996) which is the gold standard for testing cognitive memory in rodents. Our working hypothesis is that the hippocampus is involved in the construction of cognitive maps and the medial subregion of the striatum (corresponding to the caudate nucleus in humans) somehow allows this cognitive information to control behavioral performance in different situations, most notably when simple stimulus-response or other noncognitive systems are in control. What aspects of spatial navigation performance are related to multiple memory systems. For example, is the spatial bias rats show on the probe test comparable to declarative memory in humans or is the persistence to search at one place related to the strength of the previously reinforced cognitive behavior? Another is the prolonged thigmotaxis observed in animals with damage to the medial striatum (Devan, et al., 1996; Devan, McDonald, & White, 1999; Devan & White, 1999). Would humans with Huntington’s or Parkinson’s diseases show similar behavior in the virtual version of the water maze? What about findings that fail to show a dissociation between medial and lateral striatal subregions (Pistell et al., 2009); can the water maze be used to tease such effects apart? Given the comparative nature of the lab, we are particularly interested in studies that directly compare rodent and human performances in the water maze. For example, the sex difference we have observed in rodents (unpublished data), where females perform better than males after pretraining and latent learning, while males show more maladaptive persistent searching on the probe test, appears consistent with hunting gathering theories of mammalian evolution and consequently may also apply to human performance in the virtual water maze. The effects of partial reinforcement (Devan & McDonald, 2001; Devan, Stouffer, Petri, McDonald, & Olds, 2003), and other manipulations such as reduced lighting and passive placement on the hidden platform (Devan, Blank, & Petri, 1992; Devan et al., 2002) are important for defining cognitive and habit components of the water maze and may allow us to understand the complex interactions among systems that take place during a complex behavioral performance that involves cognitive-spatial processing. 2) cognitive enhancement - effects of drugs, nutraceuticals, food and drink (and other endopsychopharmacological manipulations-see below). Phosphodiesterase (PDE) inhibitors and other drugs that promote neurogenesis, neuro- and glio-transmission, enhance information processing and increase blood flow in addition to reducing brain inflammation to areas of the brain important for learning and memory (Devan et al., 2005; Devan et al., 2007; Devan et al., 2014; Devan et al., 2004). Common drink/food related chemicals such as caffeine have recently been shown to act as PDE inhibitors. PDE10 inhibitors such as papaverine and a novel Pfizer chemical called TP-10 are localized in striatum and hippocampus, brain areas involved in cognition. Melatonin and other naturally occurring chemicals [e.g., the amino acid Dserine: (Stouffer, Petri, & Devan, 2004)] that act as sleep aids may enhance memory because long-term memory consolidation is dependent on intact sleep/wake cycles (e.g., phase-shifting the cycle can lead to long-term impairment in spatial memory). Procyanidin (vitamin P) – found in high concentrations in red wine, apples and chocolate may improve spatial memory if given for an extended period of time (410 weeks). Effects of hormones: testosterone and estrogen have been studied a lot but others like oxytocin, progestin and cortisol (among others) have not. Antioxidants – fruits and vegetables that are high in antioxidants such as blueberry, strawberry and spinach (4 a day the color way) have been shown to improve cognition but the so-called superfruits that have recently become popular should also be investigated. We have done preliminary work with goji and acai juices that are 30k and 50k on the oxygen radical absorbance capacity (ORAC) scale (by comparison blueberry, which has been shown to have enhancing effects is 2.4k). Omega 3 fatty acids – fish oil but also other foods. For example, one source of fish omega 3 is sea parsley which is a culinary product but may soon be developed as a nutraceutical supplement. Endogenous Psychopharmacology (a.k.a. endopsychopharmacology)– is a term we use to refer to any noninvasive manipulation which is not ingested, injected or otherwise directly administered systemically but nevertheless has been shown to have effects on endogenous release and levels of neurotransmitters and hormones. Some common examples would be the release of oxytocin (important for pairbonding) by simply seeing a baby’s face, testosterone and epinephrine level increases by watching sporting events, and stress hormone release (cortisol) when scared or startled by something frightening. 3) Aging, neurodegenerative disease and psychological illness – Obviously a broad area of interest that to some extent draws on the previous two basic research areas as a basis for having translational value in a more applied research setting. Some of the potential applied value of multiple memory systems and the use of the water maze have been described (McDonald, Devan, & Hong, 2004; McDonald, Hong, & Devan, 2004) although this is an open area with many research possibilities. One theory states that there are multiple co-factors involved in age-related dementia (McDonald, 2002), therefore instead of looking for a silver bullet of aging or a single therapeutic treatment, complex combinations of compounds may target interactive signal transduction pathways in the brain (Pistell et al., 2007). Therapeutics and nutraceuticals to treat Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s and Huntington’s diseases, among others are of particular interest and immense importance as technological advances increase the average lifespan of individuals in developed nations increases. Studies with aged animals are expensive but there are drugs that mimic certain aspects of aging, for example cholinergic blockade with scopolamine (Devan, et al., 2004; Janas et al., 2005). In instances where we have data with aged animals the preliminary results show that PDE5 inhibitors may have some beneficial effects in the water maze, but by no means is it a cure for age-related dementia. We believe that adaptations of the water maze may be ideal for developing animal models of anxiety, schizophrenia and obsessive compulsive disorders. Unpublished data from our lab has revealed a curious behavior when rats are passively placed on the platform for a latent learning exposure. Often the rats will leave the safety of the platform to go and check other hidden locations where they have previously escaped. The anxiety of being left on the platform for an extended period leads to a checking behavior that is in some ways similar to checking behavior in OCD patients. Drug induced schizophrenic symptoms may also be investigated using a similar behavioral test that we have termed spatial response initiation. References Devan, B. D., Blank, G. S., & Petri, H. L. (1992). Place navigation in the Morris water task: Effects of reduced platform interval lighting and pseudorandom platform positioning. Psychobiology, 20(2), 120-126. Devan, B. D., Duffy, K. B., Bowker, J. L., Bharati, I. S., Nelson, C. M., Daffin, L. W., . . . Ingram, D. K. (2005). Phosphodiesterase type 5 (PDE5) inhibition and cognitive enhancement. Drugs of the Future, 30(7), 725-736. Devan, B. D., Goad, E. H., & Petri, H. L. (1996). Dissociation of hippocampal and striatal contributions to spatial navigation in the water maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 66(3), 305323. Devan, B. D., & McDonald, R. J. (2001). A cautionary note on interpreting the effects of partial reinforcement on place learning performance in the water maze. Behav Brain Res, 119(2), 213-216. Devan, B. D., McDonald, R. J., & White, N. M. (1999). Effects of medial and lateral caudateputamen lesions on place- and cue-guided behaviors in the water maze: relation to thigmotaxis. Behav Brain Res, 100(1-2), 5-14. Devan, B. D., Petri, H. L., Mishkin, M., Stouffer, E. M., Bowker, J. L., Yin, P. B., . . . Olds, J. L. (2002). A room with a view and a polarizing cue: individual differences in the stimulus control of place navigation and passive latent learning in the water maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 78(1), 79-99. Devan, B. D., Pistell, P. J., Daffin Jr., L. W., Nelson, C. M., Duffy, K. B., Bowker, J. L., . . . Ingram, D. K. (2007). Sildenafil citrate attenuates a complex maze impairment induced by intracerebroventricular infusion of the NOS inhibitor N-nitro-Larginine methyl ester. European Journal of Pharmacology, 563(1-3), 134-140. Devan, B. D., Pistell, P. J., Duffy, K. B., Kelley-Bell, B., Spangler, E. L., & Ingram, D. K. (2014). Phosphodiesterase inhibition facilitates cognitive restoration in rodent models of age-related memory decline. NeuroRehabilitation, 34(1), 101-111. doi: 10.3233/NRE131022971754LVW1620400 [pii] Devan, B. D., Sierra-Mercado, D., Jr., Jimenez, M., Bowker, J. L., Duffy, K. B., Spangler, E. L., & Ingram, D. K. (2004). Phosphodiesterase inhibition by sildenafil citrate attenuates the learning impairment induced by blockade of cholinergic muscarinic receptors in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 79(4), 691-699. Devan, B. D., Stouffer, E. M., Petri, H. L., McDonald, R. J., & Olds, J. L. (2003). Partial reinforcement across trials impairs escape performance but spares place learning in the water maze. Behav Brain Res, 141(2), 91-104. Devan, B. D., & White, N. M. (1999). Parallel information processing in the dorsal striatum: relation to hippocampal function. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(7), 2789-2798. Janas, A. M., Cunningham, S. C., Duffy, K. B., Devan, B. D., Greig, N. H., Holloway, H. W., . . . Spangler, E. L. (2005). The cholinesterase inhibitor, phenserine, improves Morris water maze performance of scopolamine-treated rats. Life Sci, 76(10), 1073-1081. McDonald, R. J. (2002). Multiple combinations of co-factors produce variants of age-related cognitive decline: A theory. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale, 56(3), 221-239. McDonald, R. J., Devan, B. D., & Hong, N. S. (2004). Multiple memory systems: the power of interactions. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 82(3), 333-346. McDonald, R. J., Hong, N. S., & Devan, B. D. (2004). The challenges of understanding mammalian cognition and memory-based behaviours: an interactive learning and memory systems approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 28(7), 719-745. Petri, H. L., & Mishkin, M. (1994). Behaviorism, cognitivism and the neuropsychology of memory. American Scientist, 82, 30-37. Pistell, P. J., Daffin, L. W., Jr., Nelson, C. M., Duffy, K. B., Bowker, J. L., Spangler, E. L., . . . Devan, B. D. (2007). Combined administration of subthreshold doses of the nitric oxide inhibitor, nitro-L-arginine, and muscarinic receptor antagonist, scopolamine, impairs complex maze learning in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 18(8), 801-805. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282f18d2f00008877-200712000-00011 [pii] Pistell, P. J., Nelson, C. M., Miller, M. G., Spangler, E. L., Ingram, D. K., & Devan, B. D. (2009). Striatal lesions interfere with acquisition of a complex maze task in rats. Behav Brain Res, 197(1), 138-143. Stouffer, E. M., Petri, H. L., & Devan, B. D. (2004). Effect of D-serine on a delayed match-toplace task for the water maze. Behav Brain Res, 152(2), 447-452.