1. SLIDE #1) Before getting started, I’d just like to thank my collaborators on this project, Ann Spilker and Joe Allen, for all of their help in preparation of the talk this morning, and to direct you to our lab’s website if you are interested in additional information about our research. 2. SLIDE #2) So as mentioned, today I’m focusing broadly on the relationships between emotionally stressful family environments and internalizing symptoms in a sample of late adolescents, followed longitudinally for 3 years 3. SLIDE #3) If you are sitting in this room, then you are probably well aware of both the various types and definitions of abuse. Among them are physical abuse, physical neglect, emotional abuse, and emotional neglect. a. Childhood abuse has far reaching implications on development and adjustment in childhood, adolescence and beyond, ranging from depression (Toth et al. 1992), physical and relational aggression (Teisl and Cicchetti 2008), delinquency (Salzinger et al. 2007), and general internalizing and externalizing problems in general (Manly et al. 2001). b. One theory suggests that children who are maltreated are unable to develop the ability to negotiate important developmental challenges, and that maltreatment interferes with their capacities to establish and resolve developmental milestones throughout childhood and adolescence (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1995) 4. SLIDE #4) We are going to be focusing specifically on the emotional and psychological forms of abuse, as research has encouraged the investigation of the unique effects of these different forms of abuse, a. and partly because some research has found that the emotional types can actually be more potent predictors of internalizing symptoms than physical types. b. Also Kaplan and colleagues, in their recent review of abuse and neglect research, commented that emotional maltreatment is oftentimes underreported because of the subtlety in which it can occur and because of the emphasis of social agencies on the physical safety of children. Therefore, the impact of emotional abuse on later internalizing symptoms may not be as well understood. 5. SLIDE #5) Previous literature suggests that children with a history of emotional maltreatment are at an increased risk for range of internalizing symptoms including depression, low self-esteem, and anxiety. a. One potential problem with some previous research, however, is that they oftentimes ask participants to retrospectively report on childhood abuse or maltreatment, which can be problematic presumably with more time passing and memories becoming biased. b. Additionally, many maltreatment studies investigate abuse and neglect experienced during childhood, but fail to concentrate on other important time periods in the lifespan such as late adolescence. 6. SLIDE #6) Therefore, using a two year longitudinal design, is to flesh out a bit more about relationships between emotional stress and internalizing symptoms in a period in mid to late adolescence from age 16 to age 18 that has not been adequately studied but can be particularly important for understanding adolescents’ adjustment trajectories. For instance, at least one study has shown that the average age of onset of a first depressive episode is somewhere around age 15, so better understanding what might contribute to that is vital. We will focus on emotional abuse, and neglect, as well as psychological control in predicting changes in depressive symptoms, anxious symptoms, and self-worth. 7. SLIDE #7) We’ll be examining these questions in a sample of 173 adolescents, drawn from the public school system of Charlottesville, Virginia, who were interviewed and observed in interactions with their parents and best friends. This larger longitudinal study began assessing teens when they were 13 years old a. But we’ll be reporting results of annual assessments beginning at age 16 up through age 18 b. The sample was evenly divided between males and females c. And is a normative community sample, so it was representative of the population of the surrounding area in terms of socioeconomics and race. d. The study overall has maintained very low attrition over the course of the now 12 years that it has been running, and generally has over 90% participation at any given time point. 8. SLIDE #8) We begin by examining the predictive contribution of emotional neglect on internalizing symptoms at 18. a. To do this, we measured emotional neglect using teens’ reports at age 16 on the emotional neglect scale from the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire, which asks adolescents to answer questions about what it was like growing up in their family in the past year. b. Sample items are given on the slide here and include statements like “People in my family felt close to one another, I felt loved.” Note that positively worded items were reverse coded so that higher scores equal higher levels of reported emotional neglect. 9. SLIDE #9) The first internalizing symptom we focused on was general anxiety, measured using self reports on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Sample items on this scale include “I worry too much over something that doesn’t really matter” and “I feel difficulties are piling up so that I cannot overcome them.” We assessed general anxiety at both 16 and 18. 10. SLIDE #10) So what did we find? Note that in all analyses, gender and income were controlled for, and were not significant unless otherwise noted. Also, Full Information Maximum Likelihood analyses were used for each model presented today. So you can see here from regression analyses that emotional neglect moderately predicts relative increases in general anxiety from age 16 to age 18. 11. SLIDE #11) Next we examined the relationship between emotional neglect and social anxiety. Social anxiety was assessed using teens’ reports on the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents. a. We measured social anxiety at age 18 but did not have a measure for social anxiety at age 16, so we used general anxiety levels as a baseline for how these teens were functioning at age 16. Sample items include “I get nervous when I meet new people” and “I only talk to people I know really well.” 12. SLIDE #12) Regression analyses revealed that emotional neglect also moderately predicts higher levels of social anxiety. Note that because we did not have a social anxiety assessment at age 16, this is not a perfect change model, but we still see increases in social anxiety from emotional neglect at 16, even after controlling for general levels of baseline anxiety. 13. SLIDE #13) We then turned to self worth, which was measured using self reports on the Harter Self Perception Profile for Adolescents. This was assessed at both 16 and 18. The format for this measure asks the teen’s closest friend to choose between two contrasting descriptors and then rate the extent to which their choice is sort of true or really true about the target teen. Item responses are scored on a 4-point scale and then summed, with higher scores indicating higher levels of self worth. 14. SLIDE #14) We see the same pattern yet again, such that emotional neglect predicts relative changes in self worth at age 18, such that higher levels of reported neglect at 16 predicts relative decreases in self worth at age 18 and explains about a third of the variance in self worth. 15. SLIDE #15) Also, just to point out, the three outcomes are only moderately correlated, so what we’re seeing is three relatively different internalizing outcomes of emotional neglect. 16. SLIDE #16) Last but not least we tested the association between neglect and depression. Depressive symptoms at age 16 was measured using the well-known Childhood Depression Inventory and then we switched to using the Beck Depression Inventory at 18, which is a more appropriate measure this age group. a. Both of these measures use a continuum vs. a threshold approach, recognizing that higher levels of depressive symptoms that do not necessarily meet diagnostic threshold may still be important in predicting future dysfunction (Lewinsohn, Solomon, Seeley, & Zeiss, 2000). 17. SLIDE #17) Surprisingly, we did not see the same pattern here and found that emotional neglect did not predict depressive symptoms at age 18. 18. SLIDE #18) In summary then we find that: a. Emotional neglect longitudinally predicts increases in general anxiety and social anxiety, and decreases in self worth, but does not predict changes in depressive symptoms 19. SLIDE #19) Turning now to what does predict depressive symptoms in late adolescence, we drew upon diathesis stress models of depression to inform our next set of analyses and considered whether perhaps there were individual differences in what predicts depressive symptoms. We focused on investigating more direct forms of maltreatment as the stressors and also looked in rejection sensitivity as an individual diathesis that could help explain why some individuals who experience direct emotional stress within the family go on to develop depressive symptoms and some do not. a. Rejection sensitivity has been defined as the disposition to anxiously expect, readily perceive, and intensely react to rejection by significant others, and has been linked to depression in the literature, though not always consistently b. We, therefore, anticipated that direct forms of mistreatment combined with an individual diathesis to be particularly sensitive to interpersonal rejection would predict increases in depressive symptoms. 20. SLIDE #20) One emotionally harsh family environment that is quite direct is emotional abuse, which was assessed again with teens’ reports on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire at age 16. You’ll notice from the sample item the difference in the sort of bluntness of this type of mistreatment in the family, and likely would not be interpreted by these teens as anything but unloving, harsh, and cruel. 21. SLIDE #21) We also assessed rejection sensitivity through teens self reports at age 16 using a revised version of the Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire, which was originally designed for college students so some of the wording was changed to be used with the adolescents in our study. This measure consists of 18 hypothetical scenarios, and asks teens to determine how anxious they would be about how the other person in the scenario would respond and how likely they think the other person is going to respond in a specific way. (higher scores= higher sensitivity) 22. SLIDE #22) What did we find? Well first, let me orient you to the graph that you see here. So we have depression level at age 18 on the Y-axis, with points below 0 indicating lower levels of depressive symptoms and points above zero indicating higher levels. And then we have high and low levels of reported emotional abuse on the X axis. e. So you can see visually that for teens who are not very sensitive to rejection, their levels of experienced emotional abuse in the family doesn’t much matter in terms of their level of depression 2 years later. f. Note that the significant interaction was found after controlling for gender, income, and baseline levels of depressive symptoms at age 16. SLIDE #23) However, highly rejection sensitive teens seem particularly vulnerable to emotional abuse, such that abuse predicts increases in depressive symptoms at age 18 ONLY for teens who are highly rejection sensitive. a. We further tested the slope of the red line using Aikin & West’s simple slope analysis guidelines and found that this increase in depressive symptoms is significant. The slope of the blue line, however, was not significant. 23. SLIDE #24) We then looked at one last emotionally stressful family environment combined with rejection sensitivity, and that was paternal psychological control. A subset of fathers’ reported on their own level of psychological control vs. autonomy granting towards their teens on the Childhood Report of Parenting Behavior when teens were 16. Again, note the directness of psychological control in items such as I am always telling my son/daughter how he/she should behave. 24. SLIDE #25) We find another significant interaction between paternal psychological control and rejection sensitivity in predicting increases in depressive symptoms after accounting for gender, income, and depressive symptoms at 16. a. Again, we see that paternal psychological control does not much matter in predicting depressive symptoms for teens who are low in rejection sensitivity. 25. SLIDE #26) However, teens who are highly rejection sensitive are much more vulnerable to the fathers’ controlling nature, such that psychological control predicts increases in depressive symptoms at age 18 only for teens who particularly sensitive to rejection. a. Simple slope analysis also revealed that the slope of the highly rejection sensitive group was significant, while the slope of low rejection sensitive group was not. 26. SLIDE #27) Before getting to our overall conclusions, I’d like to point out a few limitations of this study a. First, the majority of our measures presented today were self report aside from the father reports of control, so it could very well be that adolescents who report high levels of mistreatment from their families also tend to report high levels of internalizing symptoms and vice versa b. Additionally, these data are non-experimental and we are not assuming any causal relationships c. We are only examining three types of stress and abuse, and as we acknowledged in the beginning of this talk today, there are several other forms of maltreatment that could be investigated, and would likely have important implications for internalizing symptoms d. Finally, we are only looking at a very short time frame in adolescence, and although this study sheds light on how these things play out into late adolescence, it will be important for future work to consider what happens in terms of the development of internalizing symptoms into young adulthood and beyond 27. SLIDE #28) One overarching theme of these findings is that psychological forms of abuse seem to interfere with a key developmental task in adolescence, which is the establishment of individuals’ autonomy. These teens likely internalize the messages they are receiving from caretakers that they are unlovable, worthless and inadequate, and potentially fail to develop a sense of agency, leading them to shut down in social situations, feel badly about themselves, and become sad and down. a. Additionally, consistent with cognitive diathesis stress models of depression, the last set of findings suggest that in mid to late adolescence, perhaps emotional abuse and psychological control are not enough on their own but that combined with a heightened sensitivity to interpersonal rejection contribute to increased depressive symptoms. b. It may be that rejection sensitive adolescents interpret their families’ criticism and over-bearing behaviors even more personally, subsequently assume they are flawed or unworthy, and/or anticipate a catastrophic outcome of their this negativity (e.g., Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989; Beck, 1987). In this case, depressive symptoms may develop because these adolescents are left feeling particularly overwhelmed, demoralized and/or hopeless in this type of controlling and stressful family environment. c. Our findings also suggest that perhaps neglect and abuse are related to internalizing symptoms in slightly different ways. i. An extension of the hopelessness theory by Rose and Abramson theorizes that emotional abuse is more likely to lead to a depressogenic, negative cognitive styles than other forms of abuse because the extremely negative cognitions such as “you are unlovable and inadequate” are directly supplied to the person being abused or controlled, whereas in a less direct form of maltreatment like neglect, the adolescent has more opportunity to make benevolent or maybe even neutral attributions about themselves. ii. Perhaps then neglect serves to reinforce a lack of confidence or ability in oneself or with others in social situations, whereas abuse combined with an already existing vulnerability to rejection contributes to more severe depressogenic states. iii. Of course, future research will be needed to parse apart the differential predictions of abuse and neglect in late adolescence. Future research should also continue to look into individual differences and mechanisms in the experience of extreme stress in the family, as some may not go on to experience psychopathology, and understanding why that is will be important to further uncover. 28. SLIDE #29) Thank you, and thanks to all of my collaborators as well as the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development for funding awarded to Joe Allen.