In search of the Teachable Moment: A study of teacher autonomy, judgement and intuition. Abstract Aspects of teaching, such as those relating to curriculum development and the protocols of the teaching cycle (planning, delivery, assessment) receive much attention in literature and are regularly the focus of evaluation. Somewhat neglected by comparison is the subject of teacher action in the moment; the teachable moment (TM). Analysis of ‘delivery’ has tended to focus on the organisation of learning: didactic or dialogic, whole-class or groups, transmission or discovery for example, and is often closely linked with theoretical perspectives on learning such as constructivism and social constructivism and general principles of classroom management. However, discussion of these approaches can easily overlook the detail of how teachers operate from moment to moment and exercise their professional judgement and intuition during the flow of classroom activity. Whatever the approach or strategy, teaching and learning can be unpredictable. This study looks at the dynamic nature of teaching and learning and how teachers negotiate solutions, guide learners and respond to uncertainty, idiosyncrasies and contextual factors in everyday practice; Teachable Moment Behaviours (TMBs). It examines factors, at classroom, institutional and societal levels, which influence TMs and TMBs. The study examines teacher action, firstly through the theoretical lenses of Structurism, Agency and Power and later using Csikszentmihalyi’s concept of Flow and ideas about professionalism, intuition and judgement. It draws on the experience of three current primary school teachers as they reflect on their own professional practice, and analyses how teacher autonomy, judgement and intuition, which guide much of classroom discourse, can be easily undermined by the instruments of surveillance and scrutiny to which teachers are routinely subjected. The conclusions acknowledge that much of the expertise of the teacher lies in these skills, and makes recommendations about how teachers can preserve and develop them. 1 Introduction This study is concerned with how teachers behave whilst teaching. It examines Teachable Moments (TMs), which for the purposes of the research are defined as unanticipated instances which arise during a teaching and learning exchange which present opportunities for improvised and intuitive response from the teacher. TMs lead to Teachable Moment Behaviours (TMBs) which are defined as the behaviours (actions, words, decisions, U-turns, organisation/reorganisation) which teachers call on in response to TMs. Whilst recognising that learning is the result of an exchange involving teachers and pupils, the sole focus here is on teachers and any reference to pupil behaviour is made in so much as it helps to describe and analyse how teachers respond to it. The primary objectives here are to define and categorise these behaviours (TMBs), to analyse them against the theoretical frameworks of structure and agency, to gather data on teachers’ use of them in their everyday practice and to analyse those teachers’ reflections on their own pedagogic approaches to TMs. The analysis will examine the factors which influence how, when and why teachers behave in these ways, and the conditions in which TMBs arise. The project places teacher agency in the centre of attention and will explore how teachers respond to the non-linear (Larson Freeman, 2003, cited in Van Lier, 2007) and unpredictable nature of learning, using de Bot’s (2007) vision of the classroom as a dynamic system, in which teachers and pupils are continually ‘adjusting, reckoning, evaluating, bargaining, acting and changing.’ (Woods, 1979:2) The rationale for a focus on this particular aspect of teachers’ professional practice is threefold. Firstly, improvisation, judgements in the moment and intuitive practice are considerably neglected in literature on the skills of the teacher and on Initial Teacher Education (ITE) programmes. This is not surprising as these particular skills are not protocols to which a rubric can be easily attached and therefore describing, and perhaps more importantly in the current educational climate, measuring them is problematic. Without writing about or teaching about the dynamic nature of teaching and learning and the skills which exist in the gaps between the protocols there is little hope of developing practice in this area. 2 Secondly, there has been a significant political drive in recent years towards personalised learning in primary schools in England and Wales. Whilst this includes broader issues than what teachers do in classrooms (personalised curriculum, personalised support, personalised homework tasks etc.) personalising learning requires a knowledge of individual children and their interests, motivations, aptitudes, attention spans, language capabilities and social development, to name only a few. Whilst knowledge of these elements is to an extent gained through assessment protocols and can be addressed via teachers’ planning instruments, teachers will inevitably respond to such matters as they arise in the flow of classroom activity and will therefore also require a repertoire of instinctive behaviours in order to do this efficiently and effectively. Finally, the current political conception of the teacher is as technician, having mediocre understanding of theoretical perspectives and wider factors influencing their everyday activity, but an expert proficiency with relevant tools and techniques. This is largely in response to the standards agenda in which National Curriculum levels are seen as the output of a factory style educational process. Successive governments appear to want professionals well versed in protocols and technique. This is exemplified in the use of the term Initial Teacher Training (ITT) rather than Initial Teacher Education (ITE) in recent legislative material sent to Higher Education (HE) providers; that teachers are trained, not educated. This tendency towards a technicist view of teaching is not new however. Fears over falling standards in core subjects and under performance on international league tables (Boyson, 1975; (see Brown, 1998; Slee & Weiner, 1998 and Gorard, 2001 for a response)) and lack of social mobility have, amongst other factors, prompted a gradual increase in centralised control over primary education in the last thirty years. The introduction of the National Literacy and Numeracy Strategies in 1998 and 1999 respectively brought with it previously unseen levels of government prescription (see Alexander, 2004; Barkham & Miller, 2008) with stipulated curricula, and stipulated pedagogy. Statutory guidance bore closer resemblance to ‘standard operating procedures’ than pedagogic principles. In the last ten years the move away from prescription (Fisher, 2004) via curricula and National Strategies has coincided with periodic changes to the Ofsted Inspection Framework and the pressure 3 to pedagogic conformity now comes in the form of Ofsted grading criteria and the necessity to meet pupil progress targets. The coalition government have expressed a commitment to ‘sweep away the culture of compliance’ (www.Independent.co.uk, 2010) enabling and promoting teachers’ professional judgements, increasing teacher choice and flexibility and decreasing prescription. In January 2011 the government began a review of the primary National Curriculum which will be in place by September 2014. In the most recent iteration of the Ofsted Inspection Framework (Ofsted, 2012) four out of the ten criteria for judging the quality of teaching make reference to teacher action during lessons and, whilst no specific reference is made to improvisation or teachers using their instincts and intuition, all of the listed criteria could be interpreted as implying professional judgement. This period of transition may prove to be an interesting and fruitful time to gather data on and make recommendations concerning the pedagogic practice and personas of primary school teachers. In the discussion which follows I will be examining TMBs firstly through the sociological frameworks of structure and agency, with specific reference to perspectives from Giddens, Bourdieu and Foucault, and secondly through the lens of pedagogy. The former is a useful theoretical tool for scrutinising how the profession at large, the political context and school communities can influence teachers’ behaviour. The latter is a useful heading under which to explore how interactions at classroom level may affect teachers’ capacity to improvise and think on their feet. 4 Structure , agency and power: Sociological influences. Structure, agency and power are important foundational principles upon which much study of human social behaviour has been built. What follows is a brief exploration of these principles in relation to pedagogy. An examination of TMs and TMBs is a study of teachers’ capacity (be it their ability or the extent to which they are permitted, by others or themselves) to act according to their professional pedagogical instincts in a given moment. The extent to which teachers are able to improvise in a given teaching and learning episode, to choose a line of questioning, a physical demonstration or a timescale other than that which was planned , or contrary to the modus operandi of the community, is partly influenced by individual agency, the efficacy to make choices, decisions and judgements about what they do. At an institutional and societal level Structuralism can be a useful theoretical tool for examining teachers’ capacity to improvise and exploit TMs in their practice; in particular, the interaction between structure and agency. Early twentieth century sociologists such as Durkheim (1933) and Simmel (1971:324) emphasised the dualism of structure and agency and saw the struggle of the individual against the ‘sovereign powers of society’ as the defining problem facing humanity. In the later twentieth century Giddens (1979; 1984; 1998) and Bourdieu (1977) amongst others, attempted to reconcile this apparent dichotomy with theoretical frameworks which explore the relationship between the individual agent and the collective more thoroughly. Giddens interprets structures as expressions of what people do, and offers a succinct definition of agency as ‘the capability to have done otherwise’ (1998:78). In the classroom context, the opportunity, and therefore capacity, to do ‘otherwise’ may be governed by the expectations of the head teacher, the practice of other colleagues or a shared sense within the institution about what constitutes appropriate practice, all of which are likely to affect professional instincts and decision making. Unlike Durkheim, who characterised society as separate from its individual agents (1893; 1938) and portrayed structure as an opposing force to agency, Giddens asserts that whilst structures govern our individual action (sometimes inhibiting, sometimes encouraging), their relationship to agency is in fact more complex than simple opposites. He attributes a dynamic nature to the relationship, 5 suggesting that structure is in fact created as a consequence of the collective agency of individuals (Giddens, 1979). This implies that factors governing individual behaviour in society or in institutions are to a large extent the result of the body of individuals exercising their capability to do ‘otherwise.’ (similar to Durkheim’s ‘Collective Consciousness’, 1938:xiv). Paradoxically, this collective otherwise-ness can also act as a constraint on individual agency. In terms of school culture and classroom practice, one interpretation of this is that expectations and institutional norms (structures) influencing teachers’ behaviours are not fixed and external, but constitute a consensus about how things should be done, based on what all individuals in the community ‘do’. Structures exist and in some contexts they can be quite rigid, but they have no physical form. They only have form in so much as they are reproduced in what people do. This does not mean that structure is ‘plastic to the will of the indvidual’ (Giddens, 1998:80), but it implies that individual agents contribute to its construction. On the question of power and teacher agency, Pignatelli (1993:412) advocates teachers challenging prevailing structures, but also stresses the importance of recognising ‘their own complicity’ in them. In fact, one could argue that the former would not be possible without the latter. In Bourdieu’s notion of ‘Habitus’ (1977) the agent internalises the relationships, beliefs and values of the community and becomes inhabited by its expectations; an internalising of the external. The gradual assimilation of the agent into the cultural norms of the community is exemplified by his or her actions which in turn co-create and perpetuate those cultural values and practices; an externalising of the internal. This, according to Bourdieu (1977:72) is not an intentional or conscious assimilation, ‘..collectively orchestrated, without (…) a conductor’ but one which occurs organically. In this hypothesis doing ‘otherwise’ appears an even greater challenge to the agent. The struggle for individual agency could be viewed as a struggle against enculturation; against the tendency to allow beliefs, values and actions within an environment or community to take up residence within us. For Bourdieu though, ‘Habitus’ is not an uncomfortable or undesirable disposition. In fact, we could not function within society without its effect. Understanding and awareness of our tendency towards ‘habitus’ however, is important for the individual agent. Calhoun(1995:304) interprets ‘Habitus’ as ‘the embodied sensibility that makes possible structured improvisation.’ In this elaboration, the phenomena is characterised not as a straight-jacket which inhibits action, 6 but as a framework through which it is facilitated. This is salient in an examination of teacher action. Viewed in this way, the structures which are often seen by teachers as constraints on creativity: centralised pedagogic and curricular strategies, surveillance, excessive planning to name a few, could be reinterpreted as tools for autonomous action. Giddens (1981:27) sees this interplay between freedom and constraint as constant and all pervasive, and refers to it as the ‘dialectic of control’. The key similarity between Giddens and Bourdieu here is the emphasis on agents as collaborators in defining and developing structure, not simply as victims of it. Both conceptualise structure as more than merely the background against which actions occur, and both view agency as the freedom to recognise and respond to causal events in the world. In this sense, according to Bourdieu, structures are not only objective, they can also be subjective. As Grenfell and James (1998) point out, structures can take on phenomenological characteristics as they are built by and understood as products of human experience and activity. Translated into a classroom context, for the teacher this consists in the capacity to notice events in the dynamic environment; questions or comments, degrees of understanding among learners, and the ability to choose a causal response, viewing learners as interactive partners and the classroom itself as a dynamic space in which agents interact. This capacity depends in part on what the teacher contributes to the collective values of the community. If structure is created by the individual agents in a community, one could ask what it is that inhibits agents (teachers) from exercising greater autonomy over their action within that community, assuming that their intentions are always to serve the educational interests of their learners? To answer this question it is necessary to define more clearly the origins and form of structures which influence a teacher’s behaviour. Ozga and Lawn (1981) point to the political and economic structures which have resulted in the progressive deskilling of the profession. They argue that while teachers are prepared to accept evaluation, scrutiny and appraisal as essentially worthwhile and for the greater good, they are contributing to the very system which robs them of control over their labour. There is an implied call to organisation and militancy here. 7 Government solutions to the challenges of societal progress tend to favour centrally designed and universally applied programmes and strategies. Often based on regional research findings and generalisations about learning, these large scale prescriptions, referred to by Eisner (1985) as scientific methods have been applied with increasing frequency by successive governments since the arrival of the National Curriculum in 1989. The difficulty for the teacher as agent is that repeated dispatching, revising and repackaging of protocol from central government sends the message that innovation begins not in the classroom, but in the corridors of Whitehall. As far back as 1981 Ozga and Lawn warned about the ‘proletarianisation’ of the profession. The analogy to workers lacking ownership of the means of production, with only their labour to sell is an interesting one. This power relationship is contrary to Connelly’s (1972) conception of the teacher as ‘user-developer’, who leads curriculum development from the classroom through reflective practice. Within this climate of surveillance and rubric, performance management and grading criteria argued Brighouse (1987:11, cited in Woods, 1990), the teachers who survive will be the ‘systems people’; those who can adhere to the syllabus and adhere to the script, exercise their agency within the imposed structure. Whilst it is easy to understand how teachers contribute to the structures in their immediate environment, local structures you might say; often the most imposing and inhibiting structures to everyday classroom practice are those of national policy. Centralised control of education, whether exercised via government initiatives, publishing of performance data or routine inspection, exemplifies Foucault’s hypothesis about coercive power; that it is at its most potent when it is hidden. He illustrates this insight into the nature of power by describing penal reforms from Renaissance to Classical Europe (1977). In the former, punishments were brutal and public and designed to send unequivocal messages to the population about the supreme power of the monarch. With the introduction of prisons in 19th century Britain the emphasis shifted towards the new, more humane notion of rehabilitation. What in fact happened, according to Foucault (1977:135), is that public mutilation and death was replaced by the production of ‘docile bodies’, automatons reduced by the power of the state, for the purposes of the state. A current version of this history can be seen in the way that governments promote control of curricula, setting of teaching standards and surveillance of performance, as performing its public duty, and 8 schools as ostensibly benevolent for complying with this agenda (or failing when incompliant.) The modern meta-narrative (Peters, 2001) being that these practices will bring people out of poverty, promote social mobility, spread prosperity and widen opportunity. This can be seen as a small-scale outworking of Foucault’s ‘Governmentality’ (Foucault, 1991), the rationalities and technologies with which society is rendered governable. Less emphasis however, is given to the by-products of this system, in which, according to Hartley (1994:230), young people become resources and commodities to grease the wheels of the capitalist project. He describes this as an ‘isomorphic structure (...) whose purpose is the management of consent, and whose justification appeals to the culture of consumption.’ This sort of power is strong, according to Foucault, precisely because it solicits our: ‘…willingness to accept and internalise questionable limits on what we can know about ourselves and how we might act, as a natural condition.’ (Pignatelli, 1993:412) Schools, continues Pignatelli, can be hazardous places for those wishing to extend personal and professional agency, as they: ‘..extend the self-limiting rule of normality and the marginalizationrehabilitation of the deviant.’ (1993:420) The implication here is that compliance can easily become firmly established as the Cultural Capital (Bourdieu, 1986) amongst the staff in a community, and disagreement or nonconformity frowned upon. Ozga (2000:14) suggests that such deviancy in the teaching profession is ‘managed’ by either promulgation of professional ideology, in which behaviour is regulated through ‘reasonable co-option’, or via direct regulation. Whilst Giddens and Bourdieu devote their time to the question of what agents do in exercising freedom and power, Foucault, in offering vivid portrayals of historical characters and events as points of reference, is more preoccupied with prompting readers to consider who they are. Self-knowledge seems particularly important for teachers wishing to identify 9 and manage what they invest in their surrounding professional structures. This could be seen as a call to teacher rebelliousness, but agency, according to Murphy (1988:182), is much more concerned with ‘persons assuming responsibility for truth’ than with ‘normlessness.’ (According to Foucault (1980), power always attaches itself to truth.) This could in fact be seen as a call to research. By engaging in field studies, teachers confront, and perhaps dilute the forces of disempowerment by shifting the locus of power towards themselves as agents. To move from consumers, or indeed victims, of research, to engineers of it. From ‘docile bodies’ to active agents. The extent to which teachers can assume active control of their professional activity is questionable however. In some school contexts the structures and technologies of control are so unyielding that opportunities for decision making, creativity and flexibility (with curriculum, timetable, approaches etc.) are minimal. One prevalent feature linking the technologies of power (Foucault, 1991) (surveillance, coercion, public accountability) in recent societal discourse is discomfort with uncertainty. It is paradoxical in this postmodern age, in which Newtonian certainties have been replaced by quantum uncertainties and unpredictability in the scientific field, in which existential questions previously answered with certainty by the church are now open to interpretation, in which objectivity has been all but replaced by subjectivity, that the need for certainty exerts such a significant grip on societal structures in Western democracies. In education, as in other domains, this is most clearly articulated through the increasing requirement to state outcomes and products of learning before the process of teaching and learning has begun. This could be seen as a considerable obstacle to teacher agency. When discussing research methods later, I will elaborate on the challenges of using a postmodern approach and qualitative methods and instruments to evaluate practice in a stubbornly modernist system (education) and stubbornly modernist institutions (schools). The discussion so far, whilst acknowledging the interdependence of structure and agency, has nevertheless cast structure as the villain and agency as the hero. Or, put another way, structure as the oppressor and agency as the oppressed. In the classroom context this is something of an Orwellian over-simplification (planning and preparation bad, improvisation good) and this is certainly not my intention. The interplay between protocols teachers must 10 follow and the intuitions and the hunches they may act upon is an interesting one. The two are certainly not mutually exclusive, and represent a microcosm of the relationship between structure and agency. Planning does to an extent facilitate improvisation because it can provide vital space for professional reflection and is therefore linked inextricably to action and evaluation. However, as Hoyle and John (1995) point out, practitioner autonomy is at the heart of professionalism. In their view, the very term professional assumes dealing with uncertainty and exercising judgement, and suggest that judgement is in fact ‘more important than routine’ in professional practice. They go on: ‘..it is essential to effective practice that they (professionals) should be sufficiently free from bureaucratic and political constraints to act on judgements made in the best interests (as they see them) of the clients.’ (1995:77). Professional autonomy is by its very nature limited autonomy however. As well as asserting agency, professionalism asserts structure and limitations on freedom to act, professionalism is structure. Here we find an interesting counterpoint. Professional status is defined simultaneously by both autonomy and boundaries, by agency and structure; professionals have a licence, not just licence Hoyle and John (1995). A critical question here therefore is how these two forces can be balanced in the best interests of pupils. Figures 1 and 2 below present possible relationships between structure and agency which may help to answer this question. Structure Agency Fig.1 (my model) In this model structure and agency are seen on the same continuum. A move towards one is a move away from the other; as one increases the other decreases proportionally. In this 11 conception of their relationship, agency becomes a function of the degree of structure present in a system (a school for example). An increase in structure increasingly limits freedom, autonomy and possibly creativity and spontaneity. Increased agency in this view is an indication of decreased structure; however structure is the causal mechanism here, not agency. D B C Agency A Fig.2 (my model) Structure The relationship presented in Fig.2 is significantly different from that of Fig.1. In this conception there is no causal correlation between the two forces and thus no inverse proportionality. An increase in one does not necessitate a decrease in the other, and vice versa. This model does not assume that all professionals want high levels of autonomy and presents agency as residing with individuals, largely independent of structure, dependent on the degree to which the individual chooses to exercise their agency; on a measure of their intention to act autonomously. This Kantian view of autonomy posits that as creatures of will, human beings are able to act ‘for the sake of principles we have set for ourselves.’ (Lindley, 1986:16) Bandura (1977), Elder (1995), Flammer (1997) and others interested in efficacy have emphasised this internal process of ‘acting individual’ (Evans, 2008) in relation to the external environment. 12 Therefore, a professional at position A with high agent intentionality operating in a system low on structure may experience high levels of professional satisfaction. A professional at position B with low agent intentionality operating in a similar system may experience some professional anxiety. A professional at position C with low agent intentionality operating in a system high in structure may enjoy similar levels of professional satisfaction as their colleague at position A, but for different reasons and finally, a professional at position D, with high levels of intentionality operating in a highly structured system may also experience professional anxiety. Factors which may influence degrees of teacher agent intentionality in the classroom include personality, experience, characteristics of the pupils, value placed on this characteristic of professionalism by the school and perhaps teacher education. Some of these factors will be analysed in relation to the participants of this research, in the Insights and Analysis section. Hoyle and John (1995:81) reflect on the means by which autonomy is either afforded to or removed from teachers and imply that it is rarely explicit. ‘Given the inescapable fact of a strong link between educational achievements and occupation in industrialised society, the content goals of education (...) have an appeal to consumers. ...this is currently taken as the legitimating of a more prescriptive approach to educational goals...the teacher’s autonomy is thereby reduced.’ This interpretation of how teacher autonomy decreases places emphasis on how educational goals are defined. The more clearly defined and linked to ‘credentials’, the less autonomy teachers enjoy. The less defined the goals and the more emphasis is placed on the processes of education, the greater the teacher autonomy. This continuum can be mapped onto Fig.1 as shown in Fig.3 13 Emphasis on educational processes Emphasis on defined goals Fig.3 (my model) Agency Structure The more defined the goals, the more focussed the accountability. The less defined the goals, the more room there is for teacher interpretation. However, as iterated more than once already, this does not mean that structure is the enemy of agency, or accountability the enemy of interpretation. On the contrary, these elements are engaged in a necessary dance. An important question here is not which is right, or which should exist, but how can they relate to one another most productively? There are parallels here with Camus’ conclusion about the complex relationship between freedom and justice in his novel The Rebel: ‘Absolute freedom mocks at justice. Absolute justice denies freedom. To be fruitful, the two ideas must find their limits in each other.’ (Camus, 1951:291) The interdependence expressed here can also be applied to structure and agency, that absolute structure neutralises individual agency and absolute agency renders structures impotent and that neither case is fruitful. In the case of teachers and teaching, we see this interplay in the relationship between curriculum prescription and pedagogical flexibility; the fruit in this case might be job satisfaction for the teacher and learning for the pupil. Evans (2008:93) characterises this relationship through the concept of ‘bounded agency’, that is, autonomy conceived as socially situated, ‘influenced by but not determined by environments’. The similarities here with Giddens and Bourdieu are clear, both emphasise the relationship of the individual agent to their surrounding structures and see agents as contributing to social reproduction. This forms part of a wider sociological discussion (see Beck, 1992; Giddens, 1998; Furlong & Cartmel, 1997 and Engel & Strausser, 1998) about the nature of gradual individualisation in industrialised societies. Bringing the discussion into 14 the primary school, Pollard (2008:91) explores how this ‘dialectical relationship’ between society and individuals brings about a ‘constant interplay between social forces and individual actions.’ The next chapter will examine exactly how this interplay can influence the day to day professional practices of teachers. 15 Improvisation, responsiveness and professional judgement: Teacher action at classroom level. According to Clark and Yinger (1987:98), teachers are constantly engaged in creative processes because of the unpredictable, uncertain and complex nature of the classroom. They note that every teaching and learning situation is the result of ‘uniquely configured events’ and requires unique solutions. Tripp (1993:18) conceptualises teaching in terms of problem solving and describes the teacher’s task as to ‘problematise’ the critical incidents of everyday practice. Noticing and capitalising on TMs requires a particular set of tools, a certain understanding of and disposition towards learners and the learning process. Woods (1990:33) highlights the importance of teachers ‘cultural attunement’ to their pupils and suggests that alertness to and understanding of each situation in the classroom is a prerequisite to fruitful exchanges. This is similar to Eisner’s (1985) idea that connoisseurship in teaching lies in the art of perception and appreciation. Woods however, emphasises the unpredictable nature of classroom exchanges and notes that: ‘A brilliant and original programme of work cultivated over a weekend might fall on stony ground on Monday morning, forcing a fall-back on to routines.’ (Woods 1990:33) This unpredictability stems from the many variables and dynamics which exist in everyday classroom activity; factors ranging from the children, their experiences, personalities and emotional states and the teacher’s preferences, confidence and approach, to the physical environment and structure of the timetable. Such factors contribute to what Eisner (1985:104) describes as the ‘inordinately complicated affair’ that is teaching. He cites examples such as pupils’ expressions, posture and pace as factors to be interpreted and made use of by the teacher, and wonders what the teacher is communicating to the pupils via their the tone, use of praise and the pace of the lesson. How teachers manage and capitalise on uncertainty is of crucial importance to this study. Woods (1990) posits that breakthroughs in learning are sometimes the result of deliberate experimentation in which the teacher plans for uncertainty. In such situations the interactions and activities may involve the teacher and/or the pupils trying various methods, demonstrations, approaches, settings or groupings. On other occasions, breakthroughs may be the result of sudden or 16 gradual realisations during classroom discourse; unplanned for and unanticipated. Whatever the means by which the breakthrough occurs, the task of the teacher remains the same, to seize it and capitalise on it. Herbert (2010:70) highlights the similarities between the teacher and the artist, referring to Schleiermacher’s notion that pedagogy is ‘art in practice’. Two particular features of this comparison should be emphasised. Firstly, that learning emerges out of interaction and secondly, that the course the learning takes is the result of negotiation and development of an initial framework, which the teacher ‘bears in mind’. Taking these in turn, the very nature of interaction presupposes that both parties (in this case the teacher and the pupils) contribute to whatever is being created. According to the Oxford English Dictionary (1988:283) to interact is ‘to act on each other’. The traditional model of the teacher-pupil relationship in which the pupil is the passive recipient of whatever the teacher transmits can barely be considered an interaction in this sense because of the unidirectional flow of influence. In this interpretation of teaching and learning the teacher influences the action, utterances and outputs of the learner, but not vice versa. Schleiermacher’s assertion, inspired in part by Schiller, suggests that both parties influence and respond to one another (Winkler and Brachmann, Eds., 2000), and that the explanations, demonstrations, and facilitation of activity are, to a degree, subject to those actions and utterances. In this view, a proportion of the planning for learning must take place in the moment, during the lesson. That is not to underestimate the importance of pre-planning, which in itself can be a response to learners’ previous utterances and knowledge of their dispositions, however, in view of the unpredictability of children’s questions and comments (Pollard, 2008) and the numerous factors determining theirs and teachers’ behaviour, it could be argued that some of the most important decisions affecting learning will take place during lessons, not necessarily before them. This leads to the second point; planning provides a framework, a rough sketch or map from which learning episodes develop. A teacher will know the desired destination and possible directions for the lesson, but the route can be negotiated based partly on what happens in the lesson itself. The ubiquitous and hermeneutic view of education espoused by Schleiermacher (Winkler and Brachmann, Eds., 2000), Goethe (Oberski & McNally, 2007), 17 Pestalozzi (Gutek, 1987) and other European polymaths of the post-enlightenment era, in which learning is viewed as non-linear, negotiated and sometimes unpredictable is of interest here because they share a common conception of the teaching and learning process. Since the goal of education, according to these pedagogues, is to provide a radical response to the individual, teaching is seen as a process of interpretation, in which the teacher acts, and then finds in the experienced results of that action (the learners’ responses) clues as to what should follow, a sort of trial and error approach. The key point here is that until the initial action has been carried out, be it questioning, an activity, challenge or classroom discourse, the next set of teacher actions remain unknown. The implication here, elaborated upon by Herbert (2010) and Atkinson & Claxton (2000) is that certain subtleties of individual pupils’ (and collective) needs emerge during lessons, and are not apparent before them. Classroom interaction itself uncovers new perspectives on learners’ dispositions, attitudes and understandings as they grapple with new concepts, which are of use to the teacher in designing next steps and approaches. A key challenge for teachers in responding in these moments is dealing with uncertainty. Dreyfus’ (1981) ‘novice to expert’ rubric, Csikszentmihalyi’s (2008, 1990, 1997) notion of ‘Flow’ and Atkinson & Claxton’s (2000) examination of intuitive practice, provide an interesting framework within which to further explore TMs and TMBs. According to Dreyfus, the key characteristics of a novice are reliance on taught rules and plans (Structure), limited situational awareness and a lack of discretionary judgement. In the classroom this might translate as a high dependency on guidance from initial education, inservice training and pre-prepared plans; emphasis on one’s own performance and a limited overview of children’s performance or classroom dynamics, and few, if any, judgements made which deviate far from the lesson plan. The characteristics of the expert are the direct opposites, little or no reliance on structures, comprehensive situational perception and instinctive decision making. Dreyfus’ implication here is that development from novice to expert takes place over time and tracks alongside experience and, whilst he attaches no timeline to this transition, following it to its logical conclusion one might assume that everyday classroom practice will only show characteristics of expertise after several years of teaching. My own experience of teaching and of observing student teachers’ practice leads me to question the validity of this assumption and raises a question about what other 18 factors might influence the development of expertise. The relationship between experience and expertise will be explore further in later sections. Csikszentmihalyi describes ‘Flow’ as a state of optimal performance in which ‘consciousness is harmoniously ordered’ (2008:6) and intrinsic motivation high. Flow is a psychological state of full absorption in which individuals perform tasks and strive to meet goals effortlessly, and when creativity thinking peaks. There are similarities here with Dreyfus’ suggestions about expertise and one could posit that Flow might be a characteristic of the expert, most notably the implication that optimal performance is characterised by spontaneous, unanalysed thought and action. Csikszentmihalyi’s own analysis of what he calls the Flow Channel indicates that Flow is not a constant state of mind and action, individuals move in and out of the Channel as their own levels of skill and challenge, boredom and anxiety shift. High levels of challenge and low levels of skill produce anxiety, high levels of skill and low levels of challenge bring about boredom, both of which position the protagonist outside the Flow Channel. It seems that experiencing Flow in action is dependent on an individual’s relationship to the task at hand, their level of proficiency and how well they are matched to the particular challenge. Here we find another correlation with Dreyfus, how good one is at a particular activity bears strongly on the likelihood that one will perform fluently and instinctively. There is also however, an important difference in the way these two notions are conceptualised. According to Csikszentmihalyi Flow can be experienced by novices as much as by experts. It is not a goal in itself, but rather a psychological state one experiences when levels of skill and challenge are balanced, novices are as likely to experience this in the early stages of development and experts are after years of practice. In contrast, the characteristics of expertise are presented by Dreyfus as unobtainable to novices. The language used to describe the two concepts also reveals a key difference in the two conceptions. Expertise is explained in terms of what protagonists ‘do’, whereas Flow is explained as something protagonists experience. It is the accessibility of Flow to novices that makes it interesting as a tool for analysing improvisation and responsiveness in teaching because it suggests that TMBs are perhaps as likely to be seen in the everyday practice of inexperienced teachers as they are in that of experienced ones. The difficulty though is that whereas in Csikszentmihalyi’s model the novice takes on challenges proportionate to their experience, in teaching, the demands 19 made on newly qualified teachers are not dissimilar to those made on their more experienced colleagues. The question remains therefore (and will be addressed later in my description of the teacher participants and analysis of findings) whether the features of professional practice associated with TMs and expertise can be expected or encouraged in novice teachers. A third lens through which TMBs can be analysed is that of intuition. Atkinson and Claxton (2000) identify six varieties of (‘non-mystical’) intuition: Expertise, Learning, Judgement, Sensitivity, Creativity and Problem-solving and Rumination. They cite Bastick (1982) who identifies key features common to all varieties of intuition: a resistance to abstract, analytical or logical thought, a tendency to see situations holistically, achieving synthesis without the need to break the experience into its constituent parts first and an ability to draw on experience in flexible and centripetal ways. In his analysis of intuition as ‘expertise’ Claxton (2000:34) offers the following description of professional practice: ‘The expert teacher may go through a whole lesson, adjusting or even abandoning their actions and intentions as they go, without being conscious of much reasoning, and without being able to say why or how they made the ‘decisions’ they did, or to what clues they were responding.’ This has clear parallels with how Dreyfus characterises expertise and Csikszentmihalyi’s model of Flow. There is a necessary absence of premeditation and self-consciousness about the action. On this, Claxton goes on to suggest that: ‘..becoming too aware of and reflective about one’s action, in the heat of the moment, may result in a loss of fluency and even, in extremes of self-consciousness, in paralysis.’ The suggestion is that feeling one’s way, holding on loosely to lesson plans and teaching without knowing outcomes in advance can facilitate fluency and responsiveness. This appears to be an endorsement of the value of ‘not always knowing what one is doing.’ Atkinson and Claxton (2000). 20 The teachers participating in this research used the term creative when describing the attributes teachers need in order to notice and capitalise on TMs. Creativity is a somewhat panoptic concept (Sternberg, 1999) which has been particularly difficult to characterise in research (Boden, 1990) because of its broad scope. When humans create, they call upon a wide constellation of skills, dispositions and abilities and these are influenced by an equally wide range of environmental and contextual factors. Creativity is much bigger than TMBs and not all TMBs might necessarily be described as creative, however there are certain skills and dispositions within that constellation upon which teachers might call in a TM. Improvisation, for example, is not creativity itself but some creative expression results directly from it. Taking risks is not in itself creative, but creative people are often risk takers. Intuition and Csikszentmihalyi’s Flow concept are not creativity, yet both are routinely associated with creative endeavour (Atkinson and Claxton, 2000; Csikszentmihalyi’s, 2008, 1990, 1997; Henessey and Amabile, 1988). In the insights and analysis section I will take a closer look at these skills and dispositions, the conditions in which they may arise and discuss how creativity relates to TMs and TMBs. 21 Methodology The Teachers: The three participants in this research project are qualified, practising primary school teachers of varying lengths of service. The following table shows profile information for each teacher which is relevant to the analysis which follows. Gender Years Management Current Number experience responsibilities year Age range Other teaching in class experience experience 30 Year 2 Civil Servant. group/s Amy female 3 none Year 3 Animal demonstrator at a zoo. Robert Rufus male Male 15 23 years Deputy Head Mixed None 22 Years 3-6 1 year teaching class grade 6 (11-12 yrs 4,5 yr olds) in &6 Canada Year 5 30 Years 3-6 1 year teaching currently, but grade 3 (7-8 previously Key year olds) in Stage 2 leader. Australia In the interest of promoting individual agency through this process each teacher was asked to choose a pseudonym (as above) and write their own brief introduction, including whatever information they chose about themselves. Rufus: has been teaching for twenty three years. He began his career in inner London, working as firstly a class teacher and then a support teacher for children with English as an additional language before returning to class teaching. He then taught in Australia for a year as part of an exchange programme. In 2000 he became a Key Stage Two leader in an inner city school in Bristol. For the past five years he has returned once again to class teaching 22 while occasionally supplementing his income by training volunteers from local businesses who come into school to support children in their maths. Robert: has been a primary school teacher for fifteen years and is currently deputy head of a small rural school on the outskirts of Bristol. He has taught previously in the suburbs of Bristol and rural east Essex. In his role as deputy head he mentors and offers pastoral support to other teachers as well as being a critical friend to the head teacher. He is passionate about children, their learning and all round development. He enjoys getting children to think critically and creatively. He is also the ICT co-ordinator at his current school. Amy is in her fourth year of primary teaching during which she has taught at the same school in a large suburban area in north Bristol. She has taught year two throughout her career so far. Amy's additional responsibilities are music subject leader which she has held for three years. Amy has previously worked in the civil service and as a volunteer animal demonstrator in a zoo at weekends. Professional context: All three teachers, whilst working in different schools are subject to certain common contextual factors. Each has their own class for which they plan and teach the curriculum. Each has a line manager, in the case of Rufus and Amy this is their key stage co-ordinator and for Robert this is the head teacher. All three have professional pupil progress targets relating to performance management objectives which are reviewed at least annually. At the time of the focus group discussion Amy had undergone an Ofsted inspection the previous week, Rufus’ school was inspected in the autumn term earlier in the year and given a second consecutive ‘Satisfactory’ judgement and Robert’s school was anticipating an inspection. 23 Gaining insights into practice The research paradigm of this project is largely interpretive. Whilst acknowledging some of the rigours of the positivist paradigm (e.g. checklist data and video evidence) it is characterised primarily by the subjective dimensions of participant observation and personal and professional reflection. (I use the term ‘Professional feelings’ to describe how the teachers feel about their improvisatory behaviours in the classroom.) The process of gathering insights and interpreting practice is intended to be a developmental opportunity for the participants, thus demonstrating absolute respect (Levinas’ ‘Radical Alterity’, 1989) towards the teachers involved. The underlying philosophy here is hermeneutic and reflects my own assumptions about the nature of teaching and learning as an exchange, and about my beliefs on the ‘social realities’ (Blaikie, 2000) of the practice of teaching. The design of the strategies outlined below is rooted in the epistemological belief that the most effective way to understand what, why and how teachers do what they do is to give them the opportunity and tools to tell you themselves; to understand the practices and behaviours from within (Burrell and Morgan, 1979). Insights into the teachers’ professional experience were collected in the following forms and via the following means: 1. TMB Checklist– the teachers used a checklist of TMBs each day for one week to log those they have made use of. The behaviours on the list were initially identified by me but extended and modified through consultation with the teachers. At an initial meeting they made suggestions of additional TMBs and through discussion the wording, definitions and interpretations of each behaviour was negotiated and agreed. It was decided by consensus that in addition to the defined behaviours, there would also be a category titled ‘Other’ in which they would record new TMBs which occurred to them during the process. It seems appropriate that a study examining improvisation and the classroom as a ‘dynamic system’ (de Bott, 2007) should be open to novel behaviours arising in the Teachable Moment. The participants completed five versions of the checklist over the week of their choosing during the Autumn term of 2011, highlighting the behaviours and indicating the context (e.g. lesson, stage of the lesson etc). (These can be found in Appendix A.) 24 2. TMB Reflective Log– each teacher also completed a daily entry in a reflective log in which they enlarged upon their TMBs and explored their own professional feelings about this aspect of professional practice in this area over the week. There were prompt questions to aid their thinking, however the remit here was flexible. The log could be used to elaborate further on one particular TM and the TMBs which accompanied it; it could be used to give an overview of improvisations in one particular lesson, half day or day. They could explore the wider contexts (environment, hierarchies, relationships etc.) which may have influenced their behaviour. The only stipulation placed on this activity was that participants should explore how it felt to be in that moment (Professional feelings). My hope was that the teachers would each take ownership over this reflective process. (These can also be found in Appendix A.) 3. Videoed Lesson – each participant videoed themselves teach one lesson during the week and watched it back, evaluating their own improvisations and intuitions in TMs. There were no specific questions or prompts for this analysis, but all participants were asked to make some notes whilst watching the video about the TMBs they observed in their own practice. Only the teachers themselves watched their own videoed lessons. Their own observations, evaluations and analysis of themselves in action formed the basis of the last activity, a semi-structured focus group discussion. 4. Focus group discussion– all three participants took part in the discussion and were asked to reflect on their TMBs as captured in the checklist, reflective log and videoed lesson, and on their thoughts and professional feelings about this aspect of their professional development. The questions also prompted them to consider factors at different levels influencing their behaviour. (Questions used in the discussion can be found in Appendix B.) In addition, within this group interview the teachers were given two tasks which they carried out independently as a group. Task 1: (appendix C) to discuss and decide upon five likely characteristics of teachers who are expert at improvising in TMs. Task 2: (appendix D) to discuss and decide upon five necessary 25 characteristics of schools in which such teachers thrive. For both of these tasks the teachers were left alone. Finally, the participants were prompted to evaluate the research process. Ethics in brief: The teachers and I discussed the aims and objectives of the research in advance. They were given the opportunity to ask questions and clarify expectations. Their participation was on the basis of voluntary informed consent and the right to withdraw at any point during the process (BERA, 2011). It was explained that neither their names or the names of their schools would not be published, this enabled them to contribute candidly at each stage of the research without anxiety about the consequences (Newby, 2010). This was of particular importance because, as discussed in the previous section, a key factor influencing TMBs is a teacher’s sense that they are permitted to improvise and exercise their own judgement. Comments alluding to lack of permission prohibiting such behaviours could be seen as criticism of line managers, head teachers or even school governance of their respective schools, therefore anonymity was essential to the process. Situated ethics and postmodern evaluation: Co-researching with the ‘Other’. When viewed as situated knowledge (Simons, 2000), ethics and research methods become intertwined and arguably are most usefully be discussed together. The following section explores these elements in relation to postmodern interpretations of qualitative research and their relevance to this study. Levinas (1989) criticises the way that research ethics are dominated by epistemology in his rejection of binary oppositions that have traditionally held sway in qualitative research, with promises of clarity, certainty and universal generalisation. That is, ethics understood primarily as evaluators obtaining accurate data from subjects. This, he writes has led to a lack of respect for the Other. The crux of the criticism here is that in the dominant methodologies the Other (participants) is seen as distinct; an ‘object of knowledge.’ (Usher, 2000:176). By drawing on Cartesian dualisms of mind-body, cognition-emotion and 26 individual-social to seek absolutes and certainty from participants and data derived from them, the researcher risks failing to acknowledge what is uncertain, or unknowable. Here lies an ethical compromise, that the Other becomes reduced to something knowable and universal, losing his or her singularity. In a useful overview of post-colonial commentators (Trinh, 1989; Spivak, 1993; Bhabha, 1994) Stronach and Maclure (1997:4) suggest that ‘disappointment’ with the myth of representational clarity (post-structuralism) is something researchers in social sciences should both seek out and find comfort with. Conceptualising the Other in part as inaccessible and unrepresentable is, according to Levinas, more respectful. In response to this I have avoided any attempt to objectify the teachers, either through psycho-analysis, judgement or categorising behaviour; preferring instead to let them tell their own stories. Levinas borrows analysis from Lacan’s (1973) work on identity and suggests that the radical alterity of the Other, the unassailable uniqueness of each individual, is what is at stake. In Lacan’s conception of the Other , the individual is considered a ‘locus’, an originator of self and can only become a subject (i.e. subordinate or ‘participant’) if they choose to adopt this position. In this study, the teachers volunteered because they recognised the value in opportunities for professional dialogue and reflection which the process facilitated. Centrally controlled curricula radically subordinates individuals, relieving them of their alterity. It was my intention that the evaluative processes in this research would not confound this tendency but, if anything, counteract it in some way. Teaching is a socially complex phenomena and this research aims to represent the multiplicity of its participants’ voices and their lived experience. The practical implications of this concept of ethics (that absolute respect for participants underpins all activity) are that what could be described as a data gathering exercise becomes reframed as an attempt to come to a deeper understanding of practice, and that the ‘methods’ employed are instruments which uphold, preserve and encourage the agency and Otherness (whatever that might be and however it may manifest itself) of the participants and do not undermine it. In other words, the means of finding out in this study should not inhibit or discredit the object of its gaze – personal agency. My ‘position’ (Thomas, 2009) in the study is therefore not just an ontological question, but also an ethical 27 one, and has to do with where power (Scott & Morrison, 2007) resides within the evaluative process. In attempting to define postmodernism, Hebdige (1988:226) speaks favourably of the ‘erosion of triangular formations of power and knowledge’ which place the expert at the apex and the ‘masses’ at the base. There are parallels with Marxism here, however this also provides a useful analogy for thinking about the ethics of qualitative evaluation in the postpositivist paradigm. To conduct observations of the participants in the classroom, to watch and analyse the videos of them teaching and make my own judgements about their improvisations would be to place myself at the apex and keep them firmly at the base. They provide the footage, I provide the analysis; thus I become an accomplice in stripping them of their agency and self-efficacy. In doing so it would also perpetuate one of the key hypotheses of this study; that surveillance, assessment, judgements, performance targets and other technologies of power, limit improvisatory and intuitive professional practice. By asking participants to evaluate themselves through reflection, observation and discussion I tried to avoid replicating the architecture (surveillance, accountability, prescription) of the system within which they practice. If a radical respect for the Other in research demands less focus on measurable and useable recommendations is it of any use? It seems legitimate to ask how one argues for the authoritativeness or usefulness of epistemologies and research methodologies which are unlikely to influence politicians or inform policy. Stronach and Maclure (1997:6) advocate not trying. Their argument rejects any ‘attempt to claim authority for educational research by ‘getting our message across’ more clearly to policy-makers, press and public; or... imposing a unanimity of voice; or...offering simple solutions to intractable problems such as ‘school effectiveness’. The argument here is that the issues which occupy time, energy and column inches of those engaged in mainstream educational debate are in themselves trivialities and misnomers and can therefore only be discussed using ‘populist rhetoric’ and positivist vocabulary. Postmodern or poststructuralist evaluation of practice on the other hand views the 28 limitations of its own interpretations as its strength, presenting complexity, contradiction and uncertainty as indicators of authenticity and perturbing the relationships between causes and effects, and reality and impression. This suggests that far from ‘usefulness’ and ‘application’ being the unfortunate victims of respectful research practices, they may in fact not be desired outcomes in the first place. The teachers in this study evaluated their own practice by identifying and analysing instances in which they seized and capitalised upon Teachable Moments, reflecting on the circumstances from which their improvisations arose (TMs), strategies and intuitions they employed in the moment (TMBs) and their ‘professional feelings’ about this. This selfevaluative approach places them firmly in the driving seat (Treleaven, 1994; Reason, 1994) of the process, but does also raise questions of validity. Reflections and initial conclusions are from the teachers own perspectives and, as such, subject to their interpretation of situations, of TMs and TMBs, and to their professional identity and self-efficacy. This emphasis on self-evaluation is an important aspect of the research as it acknowledges the inevitability of subjectivity in qualitative evaluation and places the teachers themselves in the role of co-researchers and co-interpreters (engineers of research, not consumers of it); radically respected. This feature of the research however, marginalises objectivity and any notion that there is an ‘independent reality’ (Ratner, 2002:10) that can be grasped and analysed. This poses a problem for the post-structuralist researcher. Documenting and disseminating data about an independent reality is considerably less demanding than negotiating the maelstrom of uncertainty that post-structuralist and postmodernist paradigms can be. Hargeaves (1994) argues that adopting postmodernism as a research position is inadvisable due to its relativistic nature. Stronach and Maclure (1997:9) speak of the desire, apparent in educational texts, to ‘contain’ postmodernism. The implication here is that by embracing the relativist position with open arms, research activity may glean little or nothing of any disseminatable value. They cite Lather (1991) who prefers a divide and rule strategy over containment. Here the desirable characteristics of postmodernist research (democratic, progressive, critical, emancipatory) are siphoned away from the less desirable (reactionary, fascistic, acquiescent, nihilistic). In this research relativism is actively encouraged, however it is tempered by my own evaluations of the teachers’ self-evaluation. The intention here is 29 to be ‘open’ Stronach and Maclure (1997) without opening up to the ‘abyss’. To resist closure (looking for absolutes) but not completely. If, as Giddens (1986) believes, objectivism and subjectivism are not mutually exclusive, it should be possible to hold both lenses in balance with one another; finding something of disseminatable value within an interpretive process. In this case, the object of value might be a question rather than a conclusion, or a tool for refining discussion and debate, rather than new knowledge. It is hoped that this research will contribute towards a wider discussion about teacher autonomy and professional judgement. One feature of this research which epitomises the democratic approach is that the list of behaviours against which the teachers evaluated their practice was drawn up collectively in the initial meeting. In order to clarify what is and is not considered a TM or TMB for the purposes of the research, the teachers gave examples of typical classroom practices and asked if these would fall under one or other of the TMB categories on the checklist. During discussion about these typical practices and about behaviours which might fall under the ‘other’ category on the checklist, the teachers themselves suggested alternatives. In this way the process of identifying the behaviours to be highlighted and evaluated was collegiate and the categories of evaluation developed collaboratively. The principle benefit here according to Schensul and Schensul (1992) is that participants bring expert knowledge of the field. In this case, by identifying and articulating some of the categories of behaviours under evaluation, the teachers actually began to think in an evaluative way about their practice before the reflective process had formally begun. The issues of reliability and validity, whilst always an important consideration, are perhaps less dominant in a post-modern interpretive research context. Since this study sets out to interpret insights and experiences rather than to prove anything, questions about the validity of the ‘data’ are less pertinent perhaps than questions about the interpretations. As Punch (2009:244) notes, ‘reliability is a central concept in measurement’, however, this study aims to gauge, or get a sense of teachers’ actions and reflections; not measure them. Nevertheless, there were certain factors to be mindful of which may have influenced the teachers’ practice and reflections. 30 Firstly, something similar to the Hawthorn effect was possible in the TMB checklist, reflective log and videoed lesson. Rufus commented on this in the focus group discussion, suggesting that when monitoring a particular aspect of one’s own practice, it is difficult to know whether one’s actions are entirely natural. “I don’t know whether I was doing this or that because that’s what I would naturally have done, or because I was looking out for those things.” Two things mitigated against this being problematic. The teachers were all aware of this phenomenon and cognisant of it during the various stages of the research. In addition, their specific actions in practice represented a relatively small part of the overall data, and was of comparatively little importance compared to their reflections on the concepts of TMs and TMBs, their feelings whilst teaching and analysis of their own professional autonomy. Documenting their everyday practice was a means to initiate reflection, evaluation and analysis, not an end in itself. Secondly, the issue of researcher influence and power relationships. Every effort was made to redistribute power (Ebbs, 1996) in this qualitative process. The teachers’ voices were encouraged in all stages of the research, however relational dynamics exist in all interpersonal exchanges and the focus group discussion was no exception to this. Although it was made clear to all three teachers that there were no set or expected answers to any of the questions and that they could reflect freely on their experiences, potential always exists for participants to feel inhibited about saying certain things and perhaps to want to present themselves to each other in a particular way. My epistemological position was characterised by what Karnieli-Miller et al.(2009) refer to as the co-production of knowledge, in which ‘the division between researcher and subject is blurred, and control over representation is increasingly shared’ (Gergen & Gergen, 2000, p. 1035). There were one or two small indicators of inhibition at the beginning of the focus group as might be expected, however these did not appear to impact on the quality of the discussion. During the focus group interview all three teachers commented that the process of evaluating their own professional behaviour was useful to them. In the case of Rufus, it brought a significant living contradiction (Whitehead, 1988) into relief, but also reassured 31 him about aspects of his own practice. For Robert, the process refocused his attention on his educational values and encouraged him to think in depth about why he does certain things when teaching. Amy reported that the self-evaluation had convinced her of the importance of improvisation in teaching and helped her realise that this was an area of strength for her. Insights and analysis: Significant themes which arise from the focus group discussion, the group tasks, the selfevaluation and the video reflection exercise and which were common to all three teachers’ experiences are presented and reviewed in this section. These are risk-taking, creativity, confidence and permission. The first part relates to the nature of these Teachable Moments, factors which trigger them, the conditions in which they arise and how the teachers responded to them. The second part relates to factors which influence teachers’ TM Behaviour and explores recurring concepts such as autonomy, permission and professional trust, to which all evaluation and discussion of TMs and TMBs in this study regularly returned. I have drawn on quotations from the teachers’ checklists, reflective journals, group tasks and the focus group discussion throughout to illustrate and comment on their perspectives. Understanding Teachable Moments: TMs identified by the teachers fell broadly into two categories, expected and unexpected; the latter being by far the most commonly occurring. Expected TMs are those which arise in lessons in which planning is minimal and improvisation is expected or encouraged. These were described by Robert as moments “designated” for responsiveness to children’s ideas, questions and concerns. The most common context for this was pastoral, in sessions such as Circle Time, in which the whole class discuss social, emotional and moral questions relating to the children’s experience. It is interesting to note that all three teachers felt significantly more permission from Head teachers and line managers to improvise in the context of children’s social and emotional development than in the context of their 32 academic development. Robert described how he began each Circle Time with one or two set questions, after which he improvised the rest of the session, following the children’s lead: “I have these set questions like ‘how are you feeling?’ and ‘what are you looking forward to?’ but these are really just a plinth from which I can gauge the emotional temperature of the children. After that, it’s all Teachable Moments because I haven’t got a clue in advance what they’re going to bring up.” The issue of trust came up several times during the focus group discussion and group tasks, and it is interesting to note the contexts in which the teachers felt trusted by their line managers to improvise and those in which they did not. Though circle time is structured differently in the three schools, there was unanimous agreement that pastoral care whether in structured or unstructured moments, was considered a suitable site for improvisation and following intuitions. Rufus described these as occasions when “the box (referring to boxes on planning sheets) is left deliberately blank.” Other contexts in which this was deemed to be permitted were early morning work (during registration) and small group reading. One interpretation of the apparent permissiveness here is that when measured against national priorities and the standards agenda, these are relatively low stakes activities. This raises the possibility that one could tell what a head teacher valued by examining when and where they permit or encourage teachers to improvise. The majority of TMs described by the teachers were unexpected. These moments are unplanned for and arise incidentally or tangentially out of classroom discourse and the trigger could come either from pupils or teachers, or from neither. Pupil prompted TMs tended to arise from children’s questions, anecdotes, changes in emotional state and misunderstandings, to which teachers respond; as in the following example from Rufus’ journal: “DT lesson. It was meant to be a straight forward introduction about forces, but became a discussion/demonstration of gravity as it was clear that this was something the children had not encountered or discussed before.” 33 Here, according to Rufus the TM arose from a realisation that there was a mismatch between assumed and actual understanding. It could be argued that better knowledge of the children’s understandings would have pre-empted this, nevertheless, the moment was seized and capitalised upon. Amy gave a similar example in her journal: “Literacy: I wanted the children to make their own instructions to make a Chinese lantern. I pointed to the lanterns the children had made the previous week, hanging from the ceiling and asked them to talk to the person next to them about how they were made. I soon realised they had forgotten and some were talking about Florence Nightingale lanterns. I quickly got a piece of paper, some glue, scissors and tissue paper, laid them on a table and asked the children the same question. This time it was much better.” In this example the TM arose out of children’s misunderstandings. Again it could be argued that better assessment of children’s learning or knowledge of their capacities would have made the adjustments unnecessary. What this particular TM highlights though, is the intersubjectivity between teacher and pupils, that pupil learning demands a match between teacher assumed and pupil actual understanding. In another example from Amy’s log she describes how she adapted a mathematics task to match the understandings of certain groups of children in the class. In fact, an intervention with one group led to Amy redesigning the plenary of the lesson for all the children. “I did my plenary like this, instead of what was on the plan.” These examples raise an interesting point linking the nature of TMs and the nature of the teaching and learning process. In all cases, an unpredictable event was followed by adjustment which provides a useful illustration of how unpredictability in the dynamics of an environment demands improvisation from those managing that environment. If teaching and learning is a set of dynamic processes and children’s understandings and capabilities are also dynamic then one might expect lessons to be an ongoing series of judgements, misjudgements and adjustments on the part of the teacher (and indeed the learner). Interestingly however, the prevailing feeling among the teachers in this study was that 34 misjudging children’s understandings and capabilities whilst being observed was a sign of poor teacher assessment and poor knowledge of pupils. Far from being considered a skill, these inevitable moments of dynamic judgement and adjustment are frowned upon. Rufus, whose videoed lesson was also observed by his line manager as part of an ongoing internal scrutiny process, summed it up this way in his journal, “I felt a real pressure of expectations working against any spontaneity, particularly the idea of pace inhibiting my confidence in going ‘off script’.” For a teacher of so many years experience to express this decline in confidence struck me as interesting. However, this is a stark illustration of the possibility for intuitional paralysis which Claxton (2000) describes when professionals are forced to focus too much on the detail of what they are doing in the moment. Teacher initiated TMs, also unexpected, are those in which inspiration strikes resulting in a new approach, a different style of presentation or additional content. These “epiphany moments” as Rufus described them are not prompted by the children but by the teacher’s experience and imagination, and these could be described as teacher realisations rather than direct responses to the children. Rufus went on to describe an incident in which whilst teaching about measurement in mathematics he was suddenly reminded about something he had heard on the radio that morning about cubits and the equity of human arm span and height. He wove this into his teaching of that lesson and a child came to him the following morning excited about having discussed it at home with his parents. It is interesting to note that during the focus group discussion Rufus seemed quite sure that there was little scope for TMs and TMBs in his teaching due to the frequency of internal surveillance, pending an Ofsted inspection. He described conscious attempts to improvise as, “Squeezed in surreptitiously” as though doing something subversive. His checklist of TMBs however, indicates that he was engaged in most of the pre-identified TMBs throughout the week. This suggests that some of these improvisatory behaviours can be sufficiently embedded in everyday practice, or be subtle enough to go unnoticed by the teacher themselves. This sense of doing 35 something that would not be approved of was clear from all three teachers, and will be discussed in more detail later. There was agreement from all three teachers that holding back on these “epiphany” moments, or even rejecting them outright was at times, just as important as making use of them. Rufus again, explained how a “grinding gear change” arising from a sudden thought or idea from the teacher in the moment can disrupt the flow of the lesson and be an impediment, rather than a support, to learning. These crossroads moments seem pivotal to TMBs. The ability to make a quick judgement about embracing or rejecting an idea may be just as important for teachers as the openness to ideas in the first place. The conditions, including physical, emotional and psychological factors, in which TMs arise will be discussed later. The third category of unexpected TMs is that in which extrinsic events, for example sudden snow, an unexpected visitor or an assembly which over runs, forces a change of plan, a restructuring of timetable or a rethink of lesson content. These moments can add, or remove urgency from the pace of classroom discourse, force teachers into periods of improvisation and introduce novelty or disruption into the teaching and learning exchange. Amy illustrated this using the example of a teacher’s judgements following a class assembly or Christmas play performed to parents, “You’re not expecting the next lesson to be brilliant, so you decide we won’t do that, we’ll do something else. You know you’re not going to get any work out of them so you talk about it (the performance), evaluate it, ask how it went and how they feel about it before you can move on.” In an instance such as this, the excitement of the event forces a slowing of the pace of transition to the next lesson. Robert shared a similar example in which he cancelled a literacy lesson on his first morning back after paternity leave to show pictures of his new daughter to the class. His analysis of this decision was as follows, “The children just had to know about the birth, the baby and how it was 36 all going and there was almost no point in doing anything else until we’d got that out of the way. They couldn’t think about anything else at that point.” Asked if he would describe that as a professional judgement, he replied, “Yes! I would exactly describe that as a professional judgement. I think it’s advanced teacher skills in fact.” In both these examples, whilst neither Amy nor Robert regretted their decisions to deviate from their timetables, the desire to justify their actions was apparent. This suggests two things. Firstly, that the replacement activities were in some way subordinate to those originally timetabled and that their legitimacy was therefore questionable, and secondly that their line managers may not consider their deviations to be “advanced teacher skills”. Conditions in which TMs arise: A theme revisited several times throughout the focus group discussion was that of risk taking. Amy illustrated this with an example from her own practice in which, with a few minutes to go before a literacy lesson on writing eye-witness accounts, she decided to wrap a piece of cloth around her waist and put an old curtain on her head and enter her classroom in character as the maid of Samuel Pepys. After a few minutes of role play in character she removed the costume and the lesson progressed more or less as initially planned and the children produced their best writing of the year so far according to Mary. Robert shared a similar example in which he was about to begin a lesson he had planned on writing speeches. “I started by thinking that I’d just tell them they had to write a speech, but then just as I was about to begin I decided it would be much more exciting for them to receive it like a secret mission. So, I explained that Winston Churchill had a sore throat and that they would have to deliver his Christmas address, meanwhile my LSA was busy putting this challenge into ‘top secret’ envelopes for me to give out.’ 37 A risk inherent in these scenarios seems less to do with the actual activities and more to do with the practice of leaving decisions to the last minute. The risk is not just whether the activity will succeed or fail to inspire the children, but also whether an idea for an activity will come at all. Analysis by the teachers of their own and each other’s TMs led Robert to declare that, “There is a fine line between my best and worst lessons.” The implication here was that these quick ideas do not always work, but when they do they make the risk worth taking. This highlights the dynamic nature of teaching and learning and raises an interesting question about the mental and emotional composure of teachers in which they are most likely to have moments of inspiration. Amy and Robert’s evaluation of these instances suggested that fear and adrenaline were important factors. Commenting on Amy dressing up, Robert said: “I bet you wouldn’t have thought of doing that whilst sitting in front of a computer (alluding to planning by filling in boxes on a proforma) and I wouldn’t have thought of my thing (mission in envelopes) had the pressure not been on with only five minutes to go.” According to the experiences of these teachers, leaving certain things deliberately unplanned (leaving the box empty) is one way of setting the conditions in which TMBs may arise. The risk here is that no useful idea will emerge, but also that the inverse may be true; that being thoroughly planned for teaching and knowing each step of the lesson in advance could stifle moments of inspiration. Pollard (2008) is not alone in suggesting that planning should be organic, not static. In both cases, Amy and Robert were not however, completely unplanned. They had plans, but modified them at the last minute in favour of new ideas. It is possible that the security which comes from having a plan, in fact sets the conditions for new ideas to arise. There was agreement between the teachers that this is where experience plays a part. As Amy pointed out, “The more of this type of lesson (lively/spontaneous) you’ve taught, the more likely you are to have a good idea.” 38 This correlates with one response in the group task describing the attributes of the TM teacher for which Rufus, Amy and Robert agreed that a broad range of interests and life experience would be essential. In addition to life and teaching experience, it could also be argued that experience of the sensation of needing a quick idea is a crucial factor. The deliberate act of being unplanned, the necessity to have a plan, the emotional state of fear and the psychological state of openness which accompany unpreparedness may also require rehearsal. So far insights from the research activities about the nature of TMs point to a few key hypotheses. Firstly, TMs and TMBs are an organic response to the dynamics of the classroom, and TMs rely on what both teachers and pupils do. Secondly, that whilst TMs are often unexpected, they can also be encouraged by risk taking and deliberate or strategic unpreparedness on the part of teachers. Thirdly, this is considered to be subversive teacher behaviour. With this in mind, it appears that one factor either encouraging or inhibiting TMBs is teachers themselves; another is expectations from line management. These two factors will be discussed at length in the next sections. Experiencing the Teachable Moment: Creativity and Confidence Drawing on the engagement and experiences of the participating teachers, a definition of the Teachable Moment has evolved to include moments of spontaneity, improvisation, creativity and adjustment. These moments involve responding to events outside of teachers’ control, as well as deliberate attempts to instigate unpreparedness in an attempt to remain open to inspiration. In group task one (appendix C), Rufus, Amy and Robert identified creativity and confidence among five characteristics a teacher would need to improvise effectively in the classroom. Exactly how these attributes contribute to a state of openness and how they can be experienced and harnessed by teachers, warrants further analysis. This section will explore how they relate to TMs and TMBs. One suggestion from Herbert (2010:79) is that any teacher prepared to take a risk should be able to tap into what she calls ‘the creativity of the unconscious’, one explanation for the source of unplanned and unexpected ideas which arise. Research by Runco (2004; 2006; 39 2007), Griffin & McDermott (1998) and Sternberg & Lubart (1996) suggests that some people are more naturally predisposed to improvisation than others. Risk taking, originality, playfulness, sensitivity and a preference for complexity are cited, along with autonomy, independence, non-conformity and openness as characteristics of creative individuals. This hypothesis presents a problem for attempts to theorise about TMB and make recommendations for its development. If teacher behaviour is directly linked to disposition, then attempts to encourage the development of particular behaviours may result in slow or negligible outcomes. Nickerson (1999) contends however that creativity is a product of both nature and nurture and agrees that, whilst tenuous, there is evidence to suggest that creative capacities can be improved. An interesting suggestion emerging from research into creativity by Barron (1963:157) is that ‘The creative individual is one who has learned to prefer irregularities and apparent disorder and to trust himself to make new order.’ A correlation can be drawn here between the creative individual who is comfortable with uncertainty, and the teacher who has chosen to leave the introduction to their lesson partly to chance, as in the examples from Amy and Robert. Both are taking risks, both are relying on certainty coming out of uncertainty and both are trusting that ideas will follow. This moment of uncertainty, described by Amy and Robert as characterised by fear and adrenaline, is a form of adversity. Albert (1978) claimed that creativity arises from adversity. In the case of a TM, the adversity may be no more than an awareness that a planned activity is not resonating with the learners, or that there is no planned activity as yet. In this situation a temporary withdrawal from the problem at hand (Ward & Saunders, 2003) often results in a sudden realisation or insight, as with Archimedes recognising the principle of water displacement or Kekule’s realisation about the structure of Benzene. Whilst not pedagogic in context, these examples do illustrate a psychological framework concerning the way the conscious and unconscious minds work. A hypothesis presented by Ward and Saunders (2003) suggests that creative insights and ideas are often not available to the conscious mind at all and that only by ceasing conscious effort can they be accessed. 40 This is consistent with Robert’s description of how and when ideas come to him in the moment. “For me it’s a bit like Bob Monkhouse on, what was it, Celebrity Squares? He used to have this banter with the audience, they would call out and he would do this improvisation with them and he could only do it live, they tried to rehearse it but he could only do it in the moment. When they asked people to call specific things out he simply couldn’t do it, but in the moment he could always get that old joke out of his head and deliver it.” In this example, the conscious effort which limits creativity is the deliberate attempt to be creative. Robert aligns his experience with that of a comedian; when he tries to be spontaneous he rarely is, but when he operates unconsciously in the moment spontaneity flows. Here an interesting point arises about the reflective journal and video reflection activities. Did the teachers’ deliberate, conscious evaluation of their own TMBs prevent them from accessing creative potential in any given moment? Rufus posed this very question in his reflective journal. Amy described a similar instance in her reflective log, in which she changed her mind about an approach to teaching addition: “Changed the plan before I even started. Thought having a 100 square would help them. On the spot I made up a rhyme about adding 11 (across one, down one...) Thought it might help!” There is a similarity here with the concept of optimal experience. The experience, described explicitly by Csikszentmihalyi and implicitly by Robert and Amy is one in which ideas flow without obstruction. Csikszentmihalyi’s contention that individuals can deliberately organise their consciousness to experience Flow as and when they choose bears some relation to Robert’s assertion that we put ourselves in the position where inspiration can come. Interestingly, the position Robert described was characterised primarily by fear due to the imperative to be inspired, whereas the position in which one experiences Flow is described as a more positive ‘can do’ state of mind. 41 Ward and Saunders’ (2003) definition of creativity is useful to this discussion due to its inclusion of behaviour and contextually appropriateness: ‘Creativity is the result of the convergence of basic cognitive processes, core domain knowledge and environmental, personal and motivational factors which allow an individual to produce an object or behaviour that is considered both novel and appropriate in a particular context.’ (electronic journal – no page number given) A creative teacher based on this definition would not only have original ideas and develop original approaches suitable for the classroom and the particular children they teach, but also behave in creative ways; this could include deliberately leaving space for inspiration in the moment. Attempts to identify types of creativity have led to some interesting distinctions that are helpful in examining the psychology of the TM. Boden’s (1990) conception of psychological creativity, in which creative ideas are novel in the mind of the individual, though not necessarily historically new, describes much of the creativity employed by these teachers in TMs. A particular idea may be tried and tested, perhaps even by the teacher in question, the creativity in the TM therefore becomes knowing exactly when and how to use and adapt it in the current context. This is the role of experience, as identified by Rufus, Amy and Robert, and is similar to Czikszentmihalyi’s (1988) classification of personally creative people who adopt original ideas and perspectives, but whose ideas do not radically alter domains of endeavour. Whilst spontaneity is not essential to creativity, it is characteristic of some creative activity. Johnson-Laird (1988:209) describes creative improvisation as occurring when it is necessary to ‘work rapidly within a framework.’ In the classroom that framework might be the timetable or the lesson plan. He goes on to hypothesize that even within rigid frameworks gaps may appear which can be filled with a rapid choice from the creative individual. This raises an interesting question about the will of the teacher to improvise. According to Johnson-Laird’s suggestion, teachers who choose to remain open to TMs may find opportunities to work around, or perhaps subvert the frameworks within which they operate. This relationship between framework and improvisation is reminiscent of the Giddens’ position on structure and agency. I will pick up this in the next section. 42 One aspect of creativity which is commonly identified and which differs from the spontaneous action of the TM is that of process taking precedence over product. In the TM the quality and effectiveness of teachers’ ideas matter because the creativity is not an end in itself, learning and development is the end and creativity may just be the means. In this sense, there is a degree of objectivity about the TM, as Robert pointed out in the focus group discussion: “Sometimes it just doesn’t work, you have an idea but it turns out to be no more effective than it might have been if you’d done what you originally planned. Or, it could be worse.” The divergence here between creativity as an end and creativity as a tool is an important one. For an artist, the process may be as important, if not more so, as the product itself. The psychology of the TM, as already demonstrated by comments and analysis from Rufus, Amy and Robert, is based on one key imperative: to have ideas that are ultimately useful to children’s learning and development. Parents for example, who are increasingly constructed as consumers of education (Harris & Bijsterveld, 1993; Cizek, 2000; Wilkins, 2011), are unlikely to care that their child’s teacher has been through a creative process if the resulting activity or lesson was uninspiring or contributed little to their child’s learning. Here lies a second risk for the teacher in the TM, that thoughts and ideas which come to them in the moment while they teach may not be inspiring or effective. Hennessey and Amabile (1988) suggest that the state which most readily characterises creative moments is one of intrinsic motivation. Crutchfield (1962) in fact indicated that the work of notably creative people is accompanied by high levels of intrinsic motivation. The idea that an individual will be most open to inspiration and perhaps spontaneity when doing something that they love seems logical. Here again, the work of Czikszentmihalyi comes into focus. The state of Flow is characterised similarly as one in which individuals experience high levels of enjoyment in a given activity. Crucially, Lepper and Greene (1978) have suggested that intrinsically motivated people are more likely to feel freer to take risks. Mcgraw (1978) focussed attention on tasks rather than individuals, and suggested that performance on creative tasks would be adversely affected by extrinsic motivations, but performance on algorithmic tasks would not. There are several important 43 points to note here in relation to TMs. Firstly, teaching requires both creative and methodical activity and requires teachers to think and act both creatively and logically, sometimes simultaneously. Based on Magraw’s work, it may be safe to assume that the many extrinsic drivers which exert themselves on teachers do enhance performance on logical activity. It may also be fair to assume however, that these drivers do little to enhance teachers’ creativity, and in fact may damage it. Robert illustrated this in the focus group when he stated that “Observation (by others) naturally leads to being conservative.” The implication here is that surveillance and scrutiny, as extrinsic drivers, discourages risk taking, prompting more, rather than less, reliance on a script. One aspect of professionalism which the teachers described as damaged by external motivators was their confidence. The confidence to go off script or follow one’s instincts was highlighted as an important trait for an improvising teacher, and damage to that confidence surfaced several times during the focus group discussion, mostly from the two more experienced teachers Rufus and Robert. They described how their confidence had diminished over recent years. Rufus: ”Confidence is what I’ve lost; the confidence to do it (improvise). And lost the confidence that I’m allowed to do it.” Robert: “I’ve lost a lot of confidence in my teaching over the last few years and I just think it’s because of the constant criticism of it. It’s like a downward spiral.” There are two aspects to Rufus’ comment; the loss of confidence in his own judgement and in his sense of not being permitted to exercise that judgement. These two are closely related; individuals’ self-confidence comes in part from the confidence which others invest in them. The influence which others within, and outside of, the school community can have on teacher confidence will be discussed in the next section. 44 Judgement in professional contexts is described by Atkinson (2000:53) as a trust in one’s own ‘ability to come up with an appropriate response to the shifting, unpredictable experiences of life.’ Instances in which individuals show unwillingness to exercise their own judgement are often characterised by ill-defined or understood expectations or incomplete information. One might expect this to be the experience of the novice, and it appears to fit with Dreyfus’(1981) conception of novices as reluctant to exercise discretionary judgement. The difference here is that this lack of confidence was expressed by two experienced professionals, experts in their field. One of the questions which this research raises is whether novice teachers are capable of behaving like expert teachers; whether there are strategies or dispositions which can be developed or adopted by novice teachers which would facilitate the sort of intuitions and spontaneity that expert teachers display. Here though, we see the reverse, expert teachers somehow disabused of their expert capacity for exercising judgement. Atkinson (2000) provides a useful model for analysing why this might be. She suggests three factors which can either enable or disable confidence in one’s judgement: support, direction and structure. Though her model refers to early infant environmental experience and its affect on adult judgement, the model is also useful for examining what adults experience in the present. Support is conceived as ‘positive interest’ from relevant persons, which in the teacher’s case might mean fellow colleagues, line managers and the head teacher. Support is inappropriate when trusting one’s judgement always results in criticism or sanctions. Direction is the extent to which one is supported to be autonomous. It is considered inappropriate when one is never encouraged towards autonomy. Structure is the secure and consistent framework within which one can reflect safely on one’s actions. Environments in which expectations are constantly shifting exert a negative effect on judgment. It is interesting to note that all three components of this model are extrinsic factors which exert themselves intrinsically. This is to be expected when describing infant development, however I believe that in the same way that infants develop dispositions and characteristics through inter-subjectivity and engagement with their environment, interpersonal, inter-professional and environmental engagement can relieve individuals, including experienced adult professionals, of confidences. Of the three components in Atkinson’s model, structure is of particular interest. Rufus and Robert spoke of losing 45 confidence which suggests that they are aware of not having something that they previously had. They also both referred to times in their career when they had felt more confident to exercise judgement than they do currently. This suggests one of three things; either they have changed, the structures and expectations within which they work have shifted, or both. The second of these suggestions is congruous with the ongoing shifts in the political landscape which drives constant change in expectations on the teaching profession, and may go some way to explain why Amy, the least experienced of the three teachers, did not mention diminishing confidence at all. The less time one has spent in the profession, the less changes one has had to adapt to. In the next section I will focus attention on the concept of permission and analyse the teachers’ experiences in light of my earlier exploration of the relationship between structure and agency. Permission to act: structure and agency in Teachable Moments. In the focus group discussion the teachers were asked to identify factors which supported or inhibited them in responding to TMs. In the last section I analysed some of the factors particular to the individual teachers which influenced their behaviour, creativity, risk taking and confidence. It is however, difficult to isolate these elements of agency and examine them separately from the structure within which they may occur. There is no doubt that Rufus, Amy and Robert saw direct links between the structures within which they teach and the way they behave in the classroom, and the issue of perceived permission to exercise judgement and follow their instincts permeated the entire discussion, resurfacing in almost every response. Whilst there are many practices which structure the professional experience of teachers, planning, timetabling and policies for example, Rufus, Amy and Robert focussed predominantly on internal and external surveillance in their responses. Gidden’s conception of structure and agency as described earlier, places them in a relationship in which one facilitates the other. When asked to identify factors which encouraged or discouraged action in the TM, the teachers focussed almost exclusively on 46 those which inhibited them. For Rufus, observation of his classroom practice was the most significant factor, as demonstrated in the following exchange: Rufus: “For me at the moment it’s the word ‘pace’, that’s the current buzz word. I had to go and observe my partner teacher who’s only in her second year of teaching, to look at her pace. For me, it was too fast. Some of them (the children) might have got it, but lots hadn’t, and then she was on to the next thing (clicking fingers to indicate rushing). If you’re doing something on the board and you think to yourself ‘have they all got that? I’m not sure, do I just move onto the next thing or do I pause and make sure? This is where I am very much feeling the pressure.” Researcher: “That’s an interesting point. Is pace class or child specific or is it an absolute?” Rufus: “At the moment for me it has to be an absolute. My line manager said, ‘I came to see pace in this lesson but I didn’t really see any. Next time, I want to see lots of quick questions bang bang bang bang bang!’ This is where I’m losing autonomy, and becoming an automaton.” Here Rufus explains how he is being required to suppress his professional instincts about what children most need in the moment and comply with a school-wide agenda. He went on to explain that pace had been a key area for development highlighted by the Ofsted inspection earlier in the year. As a result, all teachers were now expected to plan and teach lessons using Smart Board software. With this resource the teacher creates slides containing information, examples and tasks in advance and presents them one at a time to the children throughout the lesson. This approach is intended to maintain pace in lessons by limiting opportunities for pausing, recapping or deviating from the planned content. The following exchange explains the rationale for this strategy and illustrates how the environment around Rufus is shifting: Rufus: “There’s a nearby school that is ‘Outstanding’ and that’s what they do. Personally, I feel that we’re spoon-feeding the children. Twenty 47 years ago, I just wrote things on my blackboard during the lesson and I tend to use my interactive whiteboard in the same way, just writing notes and prompts for children. So this is a complete paradigm shift for me.” This is a clear example of how a loss of confidence in one’s own judgement and in one’s sense of permission to be spontaneous can come from the changing expectations of line managers as they look for solutions to the problem of not being considered ‘outstanding’ by Ofsted. When asked to identify the key characteristics of the Teachable Moment School in group task 2, the teachers put Trust at number two out of five, saying: Rufus: “You would feel trusted to do what you do.” Roberts: “Yeah, absolutely.” The insistence on teachers teaching from Smart Board slides in every lesson appears at odds with this idea. What constitutes appropriate practice to Head teachers appears to be as much governed by expectations outside of the school community as from within it, and in some cases perhaps more so. What all three teachers described, to varying degrees and in different ways, was a difficulty reinventing themselves professionally to suit evolving expectations. It could be argued that internalising the practices of the school community as in Bourdieu’s Habitus theory, becomes difficult when the practices change regularly to meet external performance indicators. If Habitus, as conceived by Calhoun, is the structure which facilitates improvisation (1995:304), then regular shifting of that structure might render such improvisation difficult. For Amy, the issue of external scrutiny was also an important one. “We’ve just had Ofsted last week, and the inspector talked to me about pace. My top table are real thinkers and can be so slow, but given time they will write something really good. They’ll talk about it for five or ten minutes before they start writing, they have to take the atmosphere in, but they will finish it and it’ll be really good. But he (the inspector) said, ‘I would like to have seen you pushing the pace more with them, bang bang bang (claps to indicate speed). If I had done that, I would have got half of 48 those children in a right tiz, probably three of them would have cried. He wanted me to bring them back to the carpet for a plenary, but that group actually benefitted from having a slower pace.” Here again, expectations of someone from outside the school and class community are imposed. What comes across clearly here is the role that Amy’s knowledge of the children plays. The potential for undermining teacher confidence and willingness to make professional judgements based on curriculum knowledge and knowledge of the children needs little explanation. This leads to an interesting question raised by Robert about the validity of external scrutiny. “There is an essential flaw with lesson observations, and that is that it is not possible to say for certain, when an observer says ‘you ought to have done this or that, that their suggestion would have been any more effective than what you actually did. The only way to prove this would be to go back in time and re-teach that lesson. No one can actually say ‘it would have been better if...’” Robert introduces a useful point of discussion here about the legitimacy of external judgements. If, as in the case of Amy’s example, teachers’ knowledge of the children influences their judgement regarding factors such as pace, pitch, suitable environments or questions to ask, for example, then the question arises of whether a set of ‘universal’ criteria can be meaningfully employed as a point of reference for labelling practice (Outstanding, Good, Satisfactory etc) or dispensing retrospective advice? The process of disabusing teachers of their confidence to judge appropriate professional action does not however, begin with line managers, head teachers or even Ofsted inspectors observing lessons. Before the process of surveillance commences in a classroom, the apparatus defining criteria for success or failure has been decided and published; these performance descriptors and performance categories drawn up by those outside the profession serve to undermine those in it. The teacher’s task could therefore be seen as fitting their practice to the criteria, possibly at the expense of their instincts, as in the case of Rufus. 49 Sustaining intrinsic motivation in a culture dominated by extrinsic drivers is, according to Deci and Ryan (1985), Deci et al. (1999) challenging. Performance indicators, such as descriptors from Ofsted describing the features of lessons ranging from ‘Excellent’ down to ‘Unsatisfactory’, though revised periodically, until recently have been essentially inert, in that they apply to all schools across the board and rarely change. The difficulty here for the teacher is that responding to an inert rubric may come at the expense of dynamic professional judgements, and this raises the question of whether an inert tool can be used effectively to judge a dynamic process? Robert’s description of his thinking about a lesson on rivers illustrates this: “We were doing rivers, sea to source, and I was going to get them to draw a diagram of the river Severn, talk about and label the features of the river, very boring lesson, but it was going to get something in the books that was going to look good. I was honestly thinking in those terms about the lesson, you know, they would understand the key things (about rivers). But then I thought, the children are going to hate this; the computer suite next door was free so we dived onto Google Earth and all followed the river Severn from sea to source. The children loved it, and they couldn’t believe when they reached the source and zoomed out that they were in the middle of North Wales.” Of particular interest here is Robert’s shift in motivation. The first idea for the lesson could be described as extrinsically motivated, by the need to show evidence of learning in children’s books. The adjustment to the activity the children actually did appeared to be motivated by an intrinsic desire to enthuse the children. Assuming however, that accountability in the teaching profession is necessary, the question becomes, not whether external scrutiny should take place at all, but whether it can take a form which serves the purposes of accountability without damaging teacher confidence, removing teacher agency or undermining teachers’ professionalism. In the next section I will elaborate further on possible forms such a process might take; however one characteristic, that of including teacher judgement as a key assessment criteria, is already 50 partially represented in new Ofsted literature. An examination of some of this literature will further support this discussion. The following criteria are taken from the new Ofsted Framework (2012) and are of particular relevance to teacher action in the moment: ‘Observation of teaching will judge..... the extent to which well judged teaching strategies, including setting challenging tasks matched to pupils’ learning needs, successfully engage all pupils in their learning the extent to which teachers enthuse, engage and motivate pupils to learn and foster their curiosity and enthusiasm for learning the extent to which teachers’ questioning and use of discussion promote learning’ Whilst there is only one direct reference to teacher judgement, and that reference is not explicitly referring to judgement in the moment, all three of these criteria could be interpreted as teacher action based on spontaneous decision making. Drawing on the evidence from their own practice, the sort of adjustments described by Rufus, Amy and Robert just before or during their lessons were integral to ensuring the matching of tasks to individuals and increasing enthusiasm and motivation. It could also be argued that questioning and discussion cannot be a scripted process, since pupil responses are often unpredictable. There is therefore, an implied permission here to exercise professional judgement in the moment. A recent Ofsted publication, Moving English Forward: Action to raise standards in English (2012:13) goes a step further. In describing common myths about good teaching it states the following about pace and flexibility: ‘Pace. There seems to be a belief that the faster the lesson, the better the learning. While pace is important – a slow lesson is likely to lose pupils’ concentration – teachers too often concentrate on the pace of their planned activities rather than the pace of learning. For example, a teacher told an inspector that they had been advised that a starter 51 activity should never last longer than 10 minutes. While this may be a sensible starting point for discussion, the inspector’s view was that a starter activity, like any other activity, needs to last only as long as is needed to ensure effective learning. ‘ And about sticking rigidly to lesson plans: ‘An inflexible approach to planning lessons. School policies sometimes insist that all lesson plans should always follow the same structure, no matter what is being taught. In addition, evidence from the survey suggests that teachers often feel that they should not alter their plans during the lesson. The notion of a three- or four-part structure to lessons with certain key elements, such as a lively starter activity and an opportunity to review learning at the end, is helpful to teachers. However, teachers need to have the confidence to depart from their plans if early indications are, for example, that the pupils know more or less than the teacher had anticipated. The key consideration should be the development of pupils learning rather than sticking rigidly to a plan.’ These two extracts clearly align the exercising of professional judgement in the moment with good professional practice. In doing so, the inert rubric of this assessment tool takes on dynamic features, and not only for the teacher being observed; the observer is equally required to exercise dynamic judgement about the practice they are assessing. The recentness of this revised framework may explain the apparent conception of pace as an absolute expressed in Rufus’ school. The recent inspection in Amy’s school however was carried out using these guidelines which highlights the subjective nature of the assessment process. There is an interesting relationship between structure and agency, and extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. On a simple level, structure can be seen as producing extrinsic motivation and agency intrinsic. Structure and agency however, are not mutually exclusive, they exist, not even simply side by side, but in a complex inter-relatedness and the balance between them seems to be a crucial factor in how, or whether, they complement each other. A similar balance is required of teachers between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. The expectation 52 of politicians, inspectors, head teachers and others who scrutinise professional practice seems to be that teachers will make use of the best of that which motivates them intrinsically (children’s learning, enthusiasm, enjoyment, interest according to Rufus, Amy and Robert) whilst also meeting certain extrinsic standards. Or, perhaps the expectation is simpler than that; that teachers’ intrinsic motivations should be the same as the extrinsic drivers. If this were the case, a teacher who reads the current iteration of performance level descriptors against which their teaching is to be judged would see nothing for which they are not already intrinsically motivated, and nothing which conflicts with their own professional practice. As we have seen, the most recent Ofsted literature includes emerging acknowledgement of the importance of teachers’ professional judgements, and here lies an incongruity. The instrument used to assess teacher capability now requires teachers to exercise their judgement, both before and during lessons; but the apparatus of surveillance, of which that instrument is a tool, discourages them from doing this. Or, to express it in terms of this research, spotting and responding to TMs is a criteria for success in teaching observations, but the process of being observed has the potential to obviate TMB. 53 Before drawing conclusions and making recommendations, and having devoted considerable time to establishing what TMBs are, it is useful to pause for a moment and consider what they are not. Teachable Moment Behaviours are not: A rejection of structures and protocols. A case for the legitimacy of spontaneity and intuition in teaching is a case for professionalism, not laziness. The sensitive teacher, who is aware of their pupils’ needs and responsive to their utterances, body language, understandings or misunderstandings and open to their own inspiration in the moment, works extremely hard in the moment. They may have come to rely on a particular set of skills and dispositions which they make use of in action, but these are in addition to, not instead of, the skills required to prepare for children’s learning in advance of lessons. Capacity for openness and improvisation may lighten the burden of pre-lesson preparation, but it does not replace it. As in Evans’ (2008) concept of Bounded Agency and JohnsonLaird’s (1988) suggestion that structure facilitates creative thinking, the expert teacher uses structures and protocols such as planning and assessment rather like a ship’s captain uses a light house when sailing near hazardous coastline at night; as a point of reference for mapping a route, as a source of knowledge which facilitates judgements and as a safety net to inspire confidence. Similarly, planning for TMB development is not a call to disregarding structures, hierarchies, scrutiny or constructive advice from others. Neither is it part of a liberal agenda. I would argue, as Robert did that “It is advanced teacher skills.” and that TMBs play as important a role in children’s progression, development and learning as do the protocols which can facilitate it. Legitimising laziness. TMBs are demanding. They call on teachers to be ever present and constantly sensitive to the dynamics of the learning environment. TMBs require teachers to know their pupils well, as individuals and as learners. They demand attention to detail and expert knowledge of the curriculum. Openness to new ideas and willingness to improvise, take risks, innovate and adapt plans depend on use of working memory, multi-tasking and creative thinking. 54 Indiscipline. Responding to TMs is not and should not be laissez-faire. Intuitions have to be monitored and tested rigorously (Eraut, 2000). As Rufus, Amy and Robert suggested, the professionalism in judging the TM is a filtration process for ideas and adaptations which arise. The intuitive teacher knows when strategies or approaches are not working and reflects on how or whether to employ them in the future. They maintain a professional inner monologue which helps them to scrutinise their own judgements and decisions in action, as well as before and after lessons. Meaning-making for teachers rather than pupils. During the focus group discussion, when asked to reflect on the value of spontaneous action in lessons, Robert said the following: “I wonder if it’s just good for us though. Is this just something that teachers like to do so that they don’t have to sit a plan for hours?” This is an important question because it sharpens focus on the teacher’s raison d’etre and challenges thinking about the benefit to children of teachers following their instincts. Put another way, the question here is whether teacher autonomy actually supports children’s learning and development. There is little doubt, based on this research, that greater autonomy would enrich the professional lives of Rufus, Amy and Robert; and one could argue that their views on this are representative of teachers generally, but is it enriching for children? I believe the key to this is intrinsic motivation. If, as this study has led me to believe, an appropriate degree of autonomy can unlock intrinsic motivation, then the permission to judge, adapt, improvise, take risks and reflect in action (Schon, 1983) can be of direct benefit to pupil engagement, enthusiasm and consequently their learning. Without discipline however, teacher autonomy could be detrimental to children’s learning, and here the familiar sentiments of Eleanor Roosevelt (1960) resonate; that freedom demands a great deal from human beings, and brings with it responsibility. A risk here for Head teachers and indeed the government, is whether teachers, if granted more autonomy, will also exercise their responsibility to remain disciplined. Without this TMBs could be all of the above. It cannot be stated with any certainty that all teachers, if offered greater autonomy and motivated less by extrinsic factors, would become more intrinsically motivated; however, based on the literature and the experiences shared by 55 Rufus, Amy and Robert, it seems clear that the absence of autonomy in professional life is likely to inhibit intrinsic motivation, and that this could adversely affect children’s school experience. A key question here for head teachers is how teacher autonomy can most effectively be facilitated and managed so as to encourage the best that TMBs have to offer pupils and discourage the pitfalls described above. 56 Conclusions: Validity of TMs and TMBs Robert raised a pivotal point about TMs and TMBs during the focus group discussion. “I can’t make my mind up whether there is such a thing as the teachable moment or whether it’s just such an integral, normal part of teaching that actually, it’s not anything particularly special.” Robert’s thinking here about whether TMs are actually events of particular consequence, or TMBs actions of pedagogic substance, is useful in that it provides a place from which to evaluate the value of improvised, intuitive teacher action. His suspicion that TMs and TMBs are simply a given in everyday practice could stem from the fact that these practices are reasonably embedded in his everyday teaching. Perhaps a teacher more reliant on adherence to lesson plans would view TMs and TMBs as a greater novelty. The question is a valid one nevertheless. Are TMs actually significant enough to children’s learning and development to warrant this degree of analysis? I believe they are, Robert agrees, however improvisation does appear to be a dying art in teaching; as he joked when asked to list the traits of improvising teachers for task 1: “What does the teachable moment teacher look like? They look like a dinosaur because they’re extinct!” Beneath the humour here lies a key observation about what is valued in current everyday practice. Skills and Dispositions of the TM teacher TM teachers: are pedagogically sensitive can think on their feet have confidence to try new ideas know their pupils well 57 excel under pressure are comfortable with open-endedness and uncertainty understand that learners do not all understand the world in the same way trust their instincts take risks understand that a wide range of complex dynamics exist in classrooms understand that learning involves many variables working together know that they cannot control every variable Teachable Moment Behaviour is expert teaching The assertion made by Robert that exercising professional judgement is an advanced teacher skill raises some interesting questions about the way expertise is conceived. His conception is similar to that of Dreyfus, who also characterises expertise in terms of instinct and judgement. This has certain implications for teacher professional development. Firstly, if this is a key facet of expertise, then it is likely that teachers will develop towards this at different rates. Secondly, teachers will develop this expertise to varying degrees. Thirdly, some teachers may never achieve this level of expertise. Where my own conclusion conflicts with Dreyfus’ rubric is in the suggestion that novices cannot possess this expertise. I have met and worked with new teachers who rely comfortably on their instincts and experienced teachers who are comparatively uncomfortable deviating from protocols and plans. These are perhaps exceptions to the rule, however they point to the hypothesis that experience is only one factor influencing teacher expertise. Amy, as a relative novice, acted successfully in the moment to the clear benefit of her pupils. Another conclusion relating to the expert nature of teacher improvisation is its suitability to teaching and learning. Experiences shared by all three teachers point unequivocally to the dynamism and unpredictability of the learning environment. An unpredictable system requires dynamic, flexible and improvised responses. If children always acted and responded to learning stimuli in expected or predictable ways, teachers would rarely, if ever, need to adjust their plans or change tack. Judgements would largely be made in 58 advance and teaching would be a clockwork-like process in which lessons unfolded according to a preordained path. The reflections of Rufus, Amy and Robert demonstrate that a very small proportion of lessons proceed in this way. Dynamic environments require dynamic management. The expertise of the teacher here lies in their ability to respond appropriately to the unpredictability of the classroom. Experience and the Teachable Moment One interesting question raised in the literature on intuition, expertise and creativity is how these capacities and dispositions correlate with experience. Rufus, Amy and Robert suggested that the principle role which experience plays in the TM is providing a repertoire of previous decisions, judgements, actions and activities upon which to call. This does not relegate TMB to simply plucking out old ideas at opportune moments, though a judgement to reuse an effective idea at a particular moment is just that, a judgement. The teachers attributed equal importance to life experience as to teaching experience, therefore this repertoire which experience builds is more than simply a bank of lessons or activities. It may include these things, but it is also more nuanced, more subtle and much more complex. It involves teachers’ ability to connect the present moment with previous moments; to interweave understanding about the ways in which children learn and develop with knowledge of self and relationships with children. It includes a certain comfort with uncertainty and an appreciation that not all variables in the dynamic terrain of the classroom can be controlled by the teacher; something many teachers I have worked with find uncomfortable. Whether TMBs are reliant on experience has implications for continuing professional development and initial teacher education. If making professional judgements, improvising and other associated capacities are experience dependent then recommendations for developing them may prove futile for pre-service and novice teachers. I believe TMBs are to an extent experience related, but not experience dependent, by which I mean that experience is one of many factors influencing behaviour. Others include personality, degree of risk aversion, relationships with pupils, confidence and sense of permission to name a 59 few. These variables, along with many others, form the repertoire into which teachers may delve in TMs. One aspect of experience however which I do believe to be more closely related to TMBs is TMBs themselves. My fourteen years experience in teaching leads me to conclude that the more opportunities teachers seize to rely on their instincts, judgements and intuitions, the more confident they will become in exercising them. Development of Teachable Moment Behaviours: implications for CPD and ITE I have already suggested that an effective way to develop TMBs would be to rehearse them. By putting themselves in situations which require improvisation and judgement in the moment, teachers will experience not only the judgements, intuitions and decisions themselves but also the feelings and conditions associated with risk taking and uncertainty. A key obstacle to this however, is risk aversion encouraged by the current culture of surveillance in the profession. As Robert pointed out, observation and scrutiny brings out conservatism in teachers. In order to develop this expertise teachers need to feel permitted to make mistakes, to make poor judgements and learn from them. The imperative for schools to be on a constant upward improvement trajectory and for head teachers to demonstrate constant pupil progress however, has resulted in the marginalisation of this permission. Newly Qualified Teachers are required to be as proficient as their experienced colleagues on entry to the profession, with little leeway given for taking risks, making mistakes or learning to judge. This tendency, which negates the process of taking risks and learning from experience and places the reflective process in potential jeopardy, also exists in Initial Teacher Education; perhaps the very place where one might expect to find encouragement for the practice of improvising or making and reflecting on judgements. The pressure on students to demonstrate excellence against Professional Standards and increasing pressure on ITE providers to increase their numbers of ‘Outstanding’ teacher training graduates have resulted in further marginalisation of teacher expertise, as defined by Robert, even in the training process. 60 Recommendations: Teachers could be encouraged to research their own practice. The action researcher takes ownership of his or her subject matter, looks for opportunities to improve praxis, implements change and modifies elements within his or her control. As Pignatelli (1993) suggests, if teachers become engineers of research, developing an evidence base for their action, they can make a more significant contribution to the process of defining best practice, if not drive the process. This encouragement could take the form of a compulsory research sabbatical every five or six years (Barber 1994) and could prove to be a more empowering and useful demonstration of expertise than current hoops through which teachers leap to reach higher pay scales, senior management and advanced skills status. A key question for the improvising teacher is how one argues for the value of practices whose outcomes cannot be easily evidenced and which cannot be easily replicated. Teacher action research may be a useful tool here. School inspection could be reconceived as a collaborative process rather than a judgemental one. Whilst emphasis in Ofsted inspections has shifted a little in recent years towards school self-evaluation, the relationship between Her Majesty’s Inspectors and schools remains characterised by audit and managerialism akin to ‘policing’ (Woods & Jefferies, 1998:548), and largely punitive. The balance is still tipped significantly in favour of extrinsic motivation of teachers. As previously discussed, balancing extrinsic and intrinsic influences can be seen as crucial for teachers in sustaining confidence and professional judgement. Over-bearing extrinsic motivators can marginalise, or even extinguish intrinsic drive. There is an apparent contradiction here. Teachers and inspectors have the same goal, the effective education of young people, and yet, as the evidence from Rufus, Amy and Robert indicates, the two are not in partnership. There appears to be a clash of values (Woods & Jeffrey, 1998) and, on the part of the teachers in this study, a significant sense of being distrusted. Reconceived as a partnership between schools and the inspectorate, improvements in teachers’ practice could be secured through discussion, negotiation and reflection rather than summative judgement. Woods and Jeffrey (1996) describe how 61 Ofsted inspections can lead to a gradual change in teacher self-identity, from professional to technician; the professional being trusted to make professional judgements, the technician expected to follow prescribed protocols and do as they are told. Densmore (1987) referred to this as the proletarianization of the profession, from ‘blue collar’ to ‘white collar’. Even a subtle shift in thinking about the purpose of school inspection, from ‘Quality Control’ to ‘Quality Assurance’ has the potential to positively change the dynamic between the inspectorate and the profession. The former closed and suggestive of coercive power, the latter more open. Since local authorities’ and head teachers’ in-house scrutiny approaches tend to mirror those of Ofsted, a greater emphasis on partnership in pursuit of quality teaching from Her Majesty’s Inspectors would be likely to have a knock-on effect on the manner in which teachers are judged by their line managers. Teacher education could do more to acknowledge the dynamic nature of teaching and learning, offering support and guidance to students on how to improvise and make confident professional judgements. Whilst acknowledging that it would be impossible to directly teach ITE students how to think on their feet and make judgements, outside of the classroom context, steps can be taken in university ITE programmes to encourage these behaviours. Firstly, ITE providers could do more to resist the urge to remodel their programmes in the image of the current inspection regime with an over-emphasis on competence-based approaches in which students are required to meet easily-measureable, tick box standards. Pedagogical discourse could be favoured over coverage of assessment criteria and current ‘buzz word’ topics such as ‘pace.’ Secondly, teaching and learning could be discussed as a process of dynamic interaction and exchange, not a series of fixed actions with fixed outcomes. Thirdly, ITE students should be required to teach unplanned lessons and encouraged to engage in reflective evaluation of their own spontaneous judgement and TMBs in the classroom. The balance here between workplace learning and critical reflection, between the affective and social domains, and the cognitive domain Beckett and Hager (2010) is important. Fourthly, providers should offer workshops in developing 62 classroom presence, verbal and non-verbal communication, vocal techniques, interpersonal skills and improvisation to support the development of teacher confidence. 63 References: Albert, R. (1978). Obervations and suggestions regarding giftedness, familial influence and the achievement of eminence. Gifted Child Quarterly. Vol. 22, pp.201-211. Alexander, R. (2004). Still no pedagogy? Principle, pragmatism and compliance in primary education. Cambridge Journal of Education. Vol. 34(1), pp. 7-33. Atkinson, L. (2000). Trusting your own judgement: Or, allowing yourself to eat the pudding. In, Atkinson & Claxton (Eds) The intuitive practitioner: On the value of not always knowing what one is doing. Maidenhead: OUP. Atkinson & Claxton (Eds) (2000). Intuitive practice: On the value of not always knowing what one is doing. Buckingham: OUP. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy in changing societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Barber, M (1994). Power and Control in Education: 1944-2004. British Journal of Educational Studies. Vol. 42(4) pp. 348-362. Barkham, J. and Miller, J. (2008). Support or straitjacket: a tale of three strategies. In Harnett, P (Ed) Understanding primary education London: Routledge. Barron, F. (1963). The need for order and for disorder as motives in creative activity. In, Taylor, C. & Barron, F. (Eds) Scientific enquiry: its recognition and development. New York: Wiley. Beckett, D and Hager, P. (2000) Making judgments as the basis for workplace learning: towards an epistemology of practice. International Journal of Lifelong Education. Vol. 19(4), pp.300-311. BERA (2011). Ethical guidelines for educational research. London: BERA. Blaikie, N. (2000). Designing social research. Cambridge: Polity Press. Boden, M. (1990). The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms. London: Routledge. Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bourdieu, P. (1986) The Forms of Capital. In, Richardson, J. (Ed.) Handbook of Theory and Research for the Sociology of Education. New York: Greenwood Press. Brown, M. (1998) The tyranny of the international horse race. In, R. Slee, G . Weiner & S, Tomlinson. (Eds) School Effectiveness for Whom? Challenges to the school effectiveness and school improvement movements. London: Falmer Press. Burrell, G and Morgan, G. (1979). Sociological paradigms and organisational analysis. Aldershot: Asgate. 64 Calhoun, C. (1995). Critical Social Theory. Blackwell: Oxford Camus, A. (1953). The Rebel. London: Penguin. Cizek, G. (2000). School Politics 101: It’s not really about the children. Education Week. Vol. 20(14), p35. Claxton, G. (2000). The anatomy of intuition. In, Atkinson & Claxton (Eds) The intuitive practitioner: On the value of not always knowing what one is doing. Maidenhead: OUP. Cohen, L., Manion, L. & Morrison, K. (2011). Research methods in education. London: Routledge. Connelly, M. (1972). The functions of curriculum development. Interchange. Vol.3 Nos.2&3. Pp 161-177 Crutchfield, R. (1962). Conformity and creative thinking. In Gruber, H., Terrell, G. & Wertheimer, M. (Eds) Contemporary approaches to creative thinking. New York: Atherton. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The Flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York: Harper and Row. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding Flow. Psychology Today. Vol. 30(4), pp.46-71. De Bot, K. (2007). A Dynamic Systems Theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. Vol. 10(1), pp. 7-21. Deci, E. & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behaviour. New York: Plenum. Deci, E., Koestner, R. & Ryan, R. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psycho-logical Bulletin. Vol. 125(6), pp. 627-668. Densmore, K. (1987). Professionalism, Proletarianization and Teacher Work. In, Popkewitz, T. (Ed) Critical studies in teacher education. London: The Falmer Press. Dreyfus, S. E. (1981). Four models v human situational understanding: Inherent limitations on the modelling of business expertise USAF. Office of Scientific Research. Durkheim, E. (1933). The division of labour in society. New York: The Free Press Ebbs, C. (1996). Qualitative research inquiry: Issues of power and ethics. Education. Vol. 117(2), pp.217-222. 65 Eisner, E. (1985). The Art of Educational Evaluation. London: The Falmer Press Elder, G.H. (1995). Life trajectories in changing societies. In, Bandura, A. (Ed) Self-efficacy in changing societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Engel, U. & Strausser, H. (1998). Global risks and social inequality: Critical remarks on the risk society hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Sociology, Vol.23 pp962-1023. Eraut, M. (2000). The intuitive practitioner: A critical overview. In, Atkinson & Claxton (Eds) The intuitive practitioner: On the value of not always knowing what one is doing. Maidenhead: OUP. Evans, K. (2007). Concepts of bounded agency in education, work, and the personal lives of young adults. International Journal of Psychology. Vol.42(2), pp85-93. Fisher, R. (2004). Embedding the literacy strategy: snapshots of change. Literacy. Vol. 38 (3), pp. 134-140. Flammer, A. (1997). Developmental analysis of control beliefs. In, Bandura, A. (Ed) Selfefficacy in changing societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Foucault, M. (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison. London: Penguin Foucault, M. (1980). Power/Knowledge: Seelcted interviews and other writings 1972-1977. Hemel Hempstead: Harvestor Press Foucault, Michel (1991). Questions of Method. In, Burchell, G.; Gordon, C. & Miller, P. (Eds.) The Foucault Effect: Studies in Governmentality. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Furlong, A. & Cartmel, F. (1997). Young people and social change: Individualisation and risk in late modernity. Buckingham: OUP. Garner, R. (2010). Gove promises teachers the right to inspire pupils. The Independent [online]. Available from: http://www.independent.co.uk/news/education/educationnews/gove-promises-teachers-the-right-to-inspire-pupils-2139731.html [Accessed 12th February 2011]. Gergen, M. & Gergen, K. (2000). Qualitative inquiry: Tensions and transformations. In Denzin, N. & Lincoln, Y. (Eds.) The handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed) Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Giddens, A. (1981). A Contemporary Critique of Historical Materialism. London: Macmillan Giddens, A. (1979). Central problems in social theory: Action, structure and contradiction in social analysis. London: Macmillan Giddens, A. & Pierson, C. (1998). Conversations with Anthony Giddens: Making sense of modernity. Cambridge: Polity Press Giddens, A. (1998). The third way: The renewal of social democracy. Cambridge: Polity Press. 66 Giddens, A. (1986). Action, subjectivity and the constitution of meaning. Social Research. Vol. 53, pp 529-545. Gorard, S. (2001). International comparisons of school effectiveness: the second component of the ‘crisis account’ in England. Comparative Education. Vol. 37(3), pp. 279-296. Grenfell, M. & James, D. (1998). Bourdieu and education: Acts of practical theory. Guilford: Falmer Press Griffin, M. & McDermott, M. (1998). Exploring a tripart relationship between rebelliousness, openness to experience and creativity. Social Behaviour and personality Vol. 26, pp. 347356. Hand, S. (Ed) (1989). The Levinas reader. Oxford: Blackwell. Hargreaves, A. (1994). Restructuring restructuring: postmodernity and the prospects for educational change. Journal of educational Policy. Vol. 9(1), pp. 47-65. Harris, N. & Van Bijsterveld, S. (1993). Parents as consumers of education in England and Wales and the Netherlands: A comparative analysis. International Journal of Law and the Family. Vol. 7, pp178-204. Hartley, D. (1994). Mixed messages in education policy: Sign of the times? British Journal of Educational Studies. Vol. 42, No.3 pp 230-244 Hebdige, D. (1988). Hiding in the light. London: Routledge. Hennessey, B. & Amabile, T. (1998). The role of the environment in creativity. In, Sternberg, R. (Ed) The nature of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press. Heron, J. and Reason, P. (1997). A participatory inquiry paradigm. Qualitative Inquiry Vol. 3(3), pp. 274-294. HMI. (2012). Moving English Forward: Action to raise standards in English. Ofsted: Manchester. Crown Copyright. HMI. (2012). The framework for school inspection: Guidance and grade descriptors for inspecting schools in England under section 5 of the Education Act 2005, from January 2012. Ofsted: Manchester. Crown Copyright. Jeffrey, B. & Woods, P. (1996). Feeling deprofessionalised: The social construction of emotions during an Ofsted inspection. Cambridge Journal of Education. Vol. 26(3) pp.325343. Johnson-Laird, P. (1998). Freedom and constraint in creativity. In, Sternberg, R. (Ed) The nature of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press. 67 Karnieli-Miller, O., Strier, R. & Pessach, L. (2009). Power Relations in Qualitative Research. Quaitative Health Research. Vol.19(2), pp.279-289. Lacan, J. (1973). The four fundamental concepts of psycho-analysis. London: Karnac LeCompte, M., Millroy, W & Preissle, J. (Eds) (1992). The handbook of qualitative research in education. San Diego: Academic Press Inc. Lepper, M. & Greene, D. (1978). The hidden costs of reward. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lindley, R. (1986). Autonomy: Issues in political theory. Basingstoke: Macmillan Mcgraw, K. (1987). The detrimental effects of reward on performance. A literature review and prediction model. In, Lepper, M. & Greene, D. (Eds) The hidden costs of reward. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Murphy, J.W. (1988). Computerization, postmodern epistemology and reading in the postmodern era. Educational Theory Vol. 38, No.2 pp 175-182 Newby, P. (2010). Research methods in education. Harlow: Pearson. Nickerson, R. (1999). Enhancing creativity. In, Sternberg, R. (Ed) Handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ozga, J.T. & Lawn, M.A. (1981). Teachers professionalism and class. Basingstoke: Falmer Press Peters, M. (2001). Poststructuralism, Marxism, and Neoliberalism: Between Theory and Politics. Oxford: Rowman & Littlefield. Pollard, A. (2008). Reflective teaching. London: Continuum. Ratner, C. (2002). Subjectivity and Objectivity in Qualitative Methodology. Qualitative Social Research. Vol. 3(3), article 16. Reason, P. (Ed). (1994). Participation in human inquiry. London: Sage. Roosevelt, E. (1960). You learn by living: eleven keys to a more fulfilling life. Harper & Row: London. Runco, M. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology. Vol.55, pp.657–87. Runco, M. (2006). Reasoning and personal creativity. In, Kaufman, J. & Baer, J. (Eds) Creativity and reasoning in cognitive development. Cambrdige: Cambridge University Press. Runco, M. (2007). Creativity: Theories and themes: Research, development and practice. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Schon, D. A. (1983) The Reflective Practitioner: How Professionals Think in Action. London: Temple Smith. 68 Scott, D. & Morrison, M. (2007). Key ideas in educational research. London: Continuum. Simmel, G. (1971). On individuality and social forms. Chicago: University of Chicago Press Simons, H. & Usher, R. (Eds) (2000). Situated ethics in educational research. London: Routledge Falmer. Sternberg, R. & Lubart, T. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, Vol. 51(7), pp.677-688. Stonach, I. & Maclure, M. (1997). Educational research undone: The postmodern embrace. Buckingham: OUP. Thomas, G. (2009). How to do your research project. London: Sage. Treleaven, L. (1994). Making a space: A collaborative inquiry with women as staff development. In Reason, P. (Ed) Participation in human inquiry. London: Sage. Van Lier, L. (2007). Action-based teaching, autonomy and identity. Innovation in Language Learning & Teaching. Vol. 1(1), pp.46-65 Ward, T. & Saunders, K. (2003) Creativity. In Encyclopaedia of Cognitive Science. Nadel, L. (Ed) London: Nature Publishing Group. Whitehead, J. (1988). Creating a living educational theory from questions of the kind: ‘How do I improve my practice?’ Cambridge Journal of Education. Vol. 19(1), pp.41-52. Wilkins, A. (2011). School choice, consumerism and the ethical strand in talk. Studies in the Cultural Politics of Education. Vol. 32(3), pp.357-370. Woods, P. (1990). Teacher skills and strategies. London: Falmer Press Woods, P. & Jeffrey, B. (1998). Choosing Positions: Living the Contradictions of Ofsted. British Journal of Sociology of Education. Vol. 19(4) pp. 547-570. 69 Appendix A Teachable Moment Checklists and Professional Logs Rufus Amy Robert 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 Appendix B Focus Group Discussion Questions: What were the most common TMs in your experience of this process? Are there any factors which enabled or inhibited you in responding to TMs? What, if anything, is the value of noticing and responding to TMs? How did it feel to watch your back your videoed lesson? What did you notice about yourself? Task 1: Compile a list together of 5 key attributes a teacher would need in order to make the most of TMs. Task 2: What characteristics would the TM school have? Some definitions of what it means to be professional place autonomy at the top of the list. What do you think about this? Have you learned anything about yourselves as teachers through this process? What were the strengths and weaknesses of the process? Appendix C 93 Responses to Task 1: 5 key attributes a teacher would need in order to make the most of TMs. 1. Confidence 2. Creativity 3. Flexibility 4. Broad interests and knowledge 5. Uniqueness 94 Appendix D Responses to Task 2: What characteristics would the TM school have? 1. Allows autonomy 2. Trusts teachers 3. High level of quality resources 4. Environment constantly changing 5. Children learning throughout the environment 95