TShephard Magpies shiny objects segl version 08.07.14

advertisement
1
1
‘The thieving magpie’? No evidence for attraction to shiny objects
2
T.V. Shephard, S.E.G. Lea, N. Hempel de Ibarra
3
Centre for Research in Animal Behaviour, University of Exeter, Perry Road, Exeter, EX4 4QG,
4
UK
5
6
Corresponding Author:
7
Dr Toni Vernelli Shephard
8
T.Shephard@exeter.ac.uk
9
2
10
Abstract
11
It is widely accepted in European culture that magpies (Pica pica) are unconditionally attracted
12
to shiny objects and routinely steal small trinkets such as jewellery, almost as a compulsion.
13
Despite the long history of this folklore, published accounts of magpies collecting shiny objects
14
are rare and empirical evidence for the behaviour is lacking. The latter is surprising considering
15
that an attraction to bright objects is well documented in some bird species. The present study
16
aims to clarify whether magpies show greater attraction to shiny objects than non-shiny objects
17
when presented at the same time. We did not find evidence of an unconditional attraction to
18
shiny objects in either captive or free-living birds. Instead, all objects elicited responses
19
indicating neophobia in free-living birds. We suggest that humans notice when magpies
20
occasionally pick up shiny objects because they believe the birds find them attractive, while it
21
goes unnoticed when magpies interact with less eye-catching items. The folklore may therefore
22
result from observation bias and cultural inflation of orally-transmitted episodic events.
23
3
24
Introduction
25
It is widely believed across Europe that magpies (Pica pica) are unconditionally attracted to, and
26
collect, shiny objects. A Google search on the term 'magpie shiny object' produced 60,100 results
27
in 0.28 seconds (February, 2014). Most of these are descriptions of magpies that make reference
28
to their 'weakness for shiny things' (Winterman, 2008) or reputation for 'stealing jewellery'
29
(BBC, 2003). Although such assertions are usually stated as fact, no references or examples are
30
given. The belief is so ingrained in our culture that one definition given for ‘magpie’ in the
31
Collins English Dictionary is 'a person who hoards small objects' (Collins, 2013). Perhaps the
32
most famous example of this folklore penetrating popular culture is Rossini's opera 'The thieving
33
magpie', first performed in 1817, where a servant girl is executed for a series of silver thefts
34
which were actually committed by a magpie. Despite the folklore’s long history and widespread
35
acceptance, published accounts of magpies stealing or collecting shiny objects are rare and
36
empirical research on the subject is lacking.
37
An extensive internet search uncovered just two published accounts of magpies stealing shiny
38
things: a missing engagement ring that was discovered in a magpie nest (Telegraph, 2008) and a
39
magpie in Rochdale stealing keys, coins and a spanner (a shiny tool) from an automotive garage
40
(Manchester Evening News, 2007). Without empirical research, it is not known whether these
41
birds had an unconditional attraction to shiny objects, or if the folklore has caused observation
42
bias in the human population. In other words, we notice when magpies collect shiny objects
43
because we 'know' they are attracted to shiny objects but we do not notice when they interact
44
with less eye-catching items. A non-specific attraction to objects was demonstrated by Heinrich
45
(1995) with four hand-raised juvenile ravens, birds who are also reputedly attracted to shiny
46
objects. The young birds contacted novel items placed in their familiar aviary significantly more
47
than familiar items; however, their attraction depended solely on the novelty of the item "without
48
relevance to its palatability or shininess."
4
49
Research investigating cognitive abilities and object interaction in magpies is scarce. However,
50
attraction to shiny or bright objects has been documented in several other bird species, with
51
many using them as non-body ornaments, to attract mates or influence parental investment
52
(outlined in Schaedelin and Taborsky 2009). Black kites (Milvus migrans) place them in the nest
53
to act as threats to conspecifics, revealing the viability and conflict dominance of the signaller
54
(Sergio et al. 2011). Some corvid species are known to collect and cache non-food objects and
55
this may be an expression of exploration, although this behaviour does not appear to be colour
56
specific (Jacobs et al. 2014). We performed experiments with captive wild-born magpies and
57
free-living magpies (during breeding and non-breeding periods) to test for an unconditional
58
attraction to shiny objects or any sign of collection of non-food objects, and to investigate
59
possible explanations for such behaviour. In the absence of such an attraction, a neophobic
60
response to objects might be expected; accordingly we recorded behaviour that could indicate
61
either neophobia or ambivalence towards the objects. Neophobia was measured as a reduction in
62
the rate of feeding in the presence of the test objects. Five behaviours, approach/withdrawals
63
(see Greenberg and Mettke-Hoffman, 2001), walk-bys, fly-overs, jumping jacks (see Heinrich,
64
1988) and neck extensions, were classified as ambivalent behaviour, and ambivalence scores
65
were calculated as the sum of the numbers of all these behaviours exhibited in a session. Full
66
operational definitions of these behaviours are given in Table S1.
67
68
Methods
69
Captive Study
70
Eight magpies were obtained from three wildlife rescue centres in the UK. All birds were
71
deemed to be suitable for release back into the wild at the time of acquisition. They were housed
72
in an aviary on the roof of the laboratory. The accommodation provided a shed and a flight cage
5
73
(Fig. S1). There was a large, hinged hatch in the wall of the shed adjacent to the flight cage. A
74
large tray filled with soil was situated in the flight cage (outside of the test arena) so the birds
75
could cache food or other objects. A second large tray filled with water was provided for
76
bathing. The birds were colour banded for individual identification. All birds that arrived in a
77
group were housed as a group. They were locked in the shed overnight, and while test materials
78
were being set out; during experimental sessions, birds not being tested were also locked in. At
79
all other times, all birds had free access to both the shed and the flight cage.
80
On arrival at the laboratory, the birds were habituated to feeding from two plastic trays situated
81
at the opposite ends of the main aviary (test arena, Figure S1). The shiny-object tests formed part
82
of a random sequence of novel object tests that followed habituation; the other tests involved
83
presenting birds with familiar shaped trays in novel colours (black, silver and red or blue) and
84
novel shaped trays (8cm deeper) in the familiar colour. In all tests, the trays contained
85
mealworms. In the shiny objects test, two piles of six zinc-plated steel woodscrews were placed
86
in the aviary, each 20cm from a feeding tray. One pile contained silver screws and the other blue
87
screws. This test was only performed once with each bird. The screws were 7cm long. Before
88
the study began, half of the screws were painted blue with matt aerosol paint. The remaining
89
screws were left in their original shiny silver colour.
90
All sessions were videotaped with a tripod-mounted video camera situated inside an observation
91
hide located approximately 20m from the flight cage. The tests were performed between May
92
and August 2010.
93
94
Field Study
95
The field experiment was conducted at eight sites on the university campus, where magpies are
96
accustomed to regular human activity, allowing observations to be conducted from a close
6
97
proximity (<20m). To prevent pseudoreplication, the sites were separated by a minimum of
98
500m, about twice the maximum extent of territories defended by breeding magpies across
99
northern Europe (Birkhead 1991, p. 43). Each study site had a resident pair of magpies,
100
identified as the same pair in each session by their habituation to the observer and to the feeding
101
protocol, as well as their defence of the feeding area when other magpies approached. This
102
provided a total of 16 birds in the field study, but, as the two birds at each site could not be
103
reliably individually identified they are treated as one unit for analyses of mean feeding latency
104
and frequency and the mean ambivalence score.
105
The experiment took place in two phases. The first series of tests were performed at six sites
106
during the non-breeding season, from August to September 2011. The same tests were then
107
repeated at four of the same sites and two new sites throughout the breeding season, February to
108
April 2012. The new sites were required as the birds from two summer sites did not re-engage in
109
the breeding season.
110
The test objects were screws as used in the captive study, small foil rings (3cm diameter) and a
111
small rectangular piece of aluminium foil (7cm x 5cm). Half of the screws and rings were
112
painted blue with matt aerosol paint, and the rest left in their original shiny silver colour, as was
113
the piece of aluminium foil.
114
Before the beginning each phase, food was provided in two loose piles on the ground daily at
115
each study site for two weeks, and the observer recorded data as in the intended tests. The birds
116
were considered to be habituated when all of the food provided was taken in the presence of the
117
observer and within 20 minutes of provisioning. Food continued to be provided throughout the
118
study, with test objects placed next to the food during test sessions. Once the birds were
119
habituated, three control sessions were conducted to determine the birds' feeding motivation and
120
whether they had a side preference (right or left side of the feeding area). Two equal portions of
7
121
nuts were placed in loose piles on the ground approximately three metres apart, and at roughly
122
equal distance from the observer and from tree cover. Figure S2 illustrates these procedures.
123
Shiny object tests were conducted once three successful control sessions had been completed
124
(success was defined as the birds appearing within 15 minutes and feeding from the piles). Two
125
loose piles of nuts were placed on the ground in the same locations as in the control sessions but
126
with the addition of two piles of objects (screws or rings), each placed 30cm from a nut pile. One
127
object pile contained four silver objects and the other four blue objects. During the breeding
128
season, a further test was conducted with a rectangular piece of aluminium foil placed on the
129
ground next to one nut pile and secured with a small hair pin (Fig. S2). In the non-breeding
130
phase two object tests were performed at each site, one with screws and one with rings. In the
131
breeding phase a minimum of eight tests were performed at each site, with multiple exposures to
132
the rings and screws and one test with the foil rectangle. The tests were spaced out to incorporate
133
early nest-building, final nest-building and incubation and were performed in a random
134
sequence. In both phases, the sequence of tests varied across sites. Control sessions lasted for 30
135
minutes or until the food was depleted, whichever came first. All test sessions lasted for 30
136
minutes, even if the food was depleted sooner, to give the birds ample time to investigate the
137
objects. If no magpies arrived in the first 15 minutes of an experimental session it was
138
abandoned and its data were discarded.
139
140
Binoculars (10x magnification) were used to confirm the number of food items taken in each
141
visit. All sessions were videotaped with a tripod-mounted video camera situated 15 to 20m from
142
the food and positioned to give a panoramic view of the feeding area. The video footage was
143
viewed to measure feeding latency and feeding frequency, which were considered to be
144
measures of neophobia, and to record ambivalent behaviours. One person coded the sessions
145
from the non-breeding phase and another coded those from the breeding phase, and also recoded
8
146
three sessions from the non-breeding phase. Pearson's correlations and comparisons of means
147
confirmed consistent coding: the correlations between coders were 1.00, 0.99 and 0.99 for
148
feeding latency, feeding frequency and ambivalence scores respectively, P<.05 in all cases, and
149
the Means ±se for the two observers were 246s ±202 and 245s ±202 for feeding latency, 7.33
150
±1.09 and 8.67 ±1.61 for feeding frequency, and 11 ±5.86 and 12 ±6.49 for ambivalence score.
151
152
Statistical procedures
153
Data from the captive test and the foil test were analysed using Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests.
154
General Estimating Equations (GEE) were used to analyse the remaining field data to
155
accommodate the non-normal distribution and the use of repeated measures (Garson, 2012). The
156
most appropriate model type, working correlation matrix structure and subset of predictors were
157
chosen based on the Goodness of Fit statistics QIC (quasi-likelihood under independence
158
criterion) and QICC (a corrected version that rewards parsimony). Feeding latency data were
159
analysed using identity-inverse Gaussian regression whereas feeding frequency and ambivalence
160
data were analysed using negative binomial regression. Least Significant Difference tests were
161
used to examine pairwise contrasts. Spearman Correlation Coefficients were used to explore
162
whether ambivalent behaviour changed with repeated exposure to the objects in the field study.
163
GEE analyses on feeding latency, feeding frequency and ambivalence scores were first run with
164
the control sessions split into multiple phases, each comprised of the three sessions preceding a
165
test. No qualitative differences between the control sessions across the different phases were
166
found so their data were combined and treated as one control condition. This resulted in a better
167
model fit for each of the analyses. All analyses were carried out using SPSS v20.
168
9
169
Results
170
Captive study
171
None of the eight birds made contact with any object, shiny or blue. The presence of the objects
172
did not deter them from feeding from the trays next to the objects. Feeding latencies in this test
173
were not significantly different to the preceding control session (means ±se: control = 42s ±13,
174
screw test = 23s ±19; W = 16, N = 8, P = 0.313). Additionally, there was no significant
175
difference in feeding latency between the trays next to silver and blue screws (means ±se: silver
176
= 325s ±161, blue = 370s ±223; W = 4, N = 8, P = 0.844). The birds exhibited little ambivalence
177
in the presence of the screws, with scores in this test lower than the other novel object tests
178
conducted during the study (mean score ±se: shiny object test = 3.13 ±1.34, other tests in captive
179
study ranged from 8.88 ±2.70 to 25.88 ±7.97).
180
Field study
181
Magpies only made contact with a shiny object twice in 64 tests. Both incidents occurred at the
182
same site, once in the non-breeding phase and once during early nest-building. One silver ring
183
was picked up and immediately discarded in both instances. No other object interactions
184
occurred at any site.
185
Figure 1 shows that birds in the field appeared to find the test objects aversive, with significant
186
differences in feeding latencies and frequencies for the food piles across three conditions
187
(control, ring test, screw test; feeding latency: χ2 = 23.52, df = 2, P <0.001; feeding frequency: χ2
188
= 12.68, df = 2, P = 0.002). Pairwise contrasts between conditions confirm that magpies fed from
189
the piles significantly faster and took significantly more nuts in the control condition than when
190
objects were present. In the foil test, magpies showed a preference for the control pile over the
191
pile next to the foil rectangle, feeding from the control pile sooner (W = 1, N = 6, P = 0.080;
192
Figure 1a) and taking significantly more nuts from it (W = 0, N = 6, P = 0.041; Figure 1b). The
10
193
aversion to feeding from food piles beside objects remains when the non-breeding and breeding
194
phases are examined separately (test*season interaction: feeding latency: χ2 = 5.87, df = 2, P =
195
0.053; feeding frequency: χ2 = 1.28, df = 2, P = 0.527).
196
Figure 1 also shows that screw colour (shiny or blue) did not affect magpies’ reactions to the
197
food piles during either phase (breeding and non-breeding). The birds took significantly longer
198
to feed from piles beside blue rings in the breeding season (test*colour*season interaction:
199
feeding latency: χ2 = 14.16, df = 4, P = 0.007; Figure 1a) but there was no difference in the non-
200
breeding phase. Ring colour did not affect feeding frequency in either phase (test*colour*season
201
interaction: feeding frequency: χ2 = 5.09, df = 4, P = 0.278, Figure 1b). Colour comparisons were
202
not confounded by a positional bias (mean feeding latency ±se: right pile = 350s ±49, left pile =
203
298s ±40, χ2 = 1.62, df = 1, P = 0.203; mean feeding frequency ±se: right pile = 4.65 ±0.31, left
204
pile = 4.90 ±0.41, χ2 = 2.62, df = 1, P = 0.106).
205
Figure 2 shows that the pattern of ambivalent behaviour also implies that the birds were wary of
206
the objects. There was a significant difference in ambivalence scores across the four conditions
207
(control, ring test, screw test, foil test; χ2 = 27.5, df = 3, P <0.001). Pairwise contrasts between
208
conditions reveal the magpies displayed significantly more ambivalence in the ring and screw
209
tests than in the control sessions. Ambivalence scores decreased with repeated exposure to the
210
objects (N = 6, n = 163, r = -0.187, P = 0.017).
211
212
Discussion
213
This study exonerates Rossini’s magpie, finding no evidence to suggest the species is
214
unconditionally attracted to shiny objects. The birds either ignored or avoided both shiny and
215
blue objects, with little difference observed between breeding and non-breeding seasons. There
216
were two interactions with shiny rings at one site, but these occurred after the food was depleted
11
217
rather than when the objects were first presented, suggesting investigations into a potential food
218
item rather than unconditional attraction.
219
Rather than being attractive to magpies, the objects appeared to induce neophobia in most of the
220
birds in the field - demonstrated by increased feeding latency, decreased feeding frequency and
221
more ambivalent behaviour when objects were present than during control sessions. The free-
222
living birds reacted this way to both shiny and blue objects, as well as to different shaped
223
objects. The decline in ambivalence scores with repeated exposure to the objects suggests the
224
birds did habituate to their presence over time. This agrees with descriptions of object neophobia
225
in other corvid species (Greenberg 2003; Greenberg and Mettke-Hofmann 2001) and we have
226
investigated it more fully in other studies (Vernelli, 2013). Birds in captivity did not show any
227
object-related avoidance. Similar differences between wild and captive populations have been
228
observed in kea (Huber & Gajdon 2006) and spotted hyenas (Benson-Amram, Weldele &
229
Holekamp 2013). They may reflect the artificial nature of captive settings where animals are
230
confined in close proximity to novel stimuli, whereas wild animals are free to avoid fear-
231
provoking objects.
232
We suggest the widely-held belief that magpies are unconditionally attracted to shiny objects
233
results from cultural generalisation of anecdotal evidence, demonstrating how biases in
234
observations of animals may determine the broad public perception of species and their impact
235
on the environment.
236
237
Acknowledgements:
238
We thank RSPCA, Secret World Wildlife Rescue and Crows are Us for providing rescued
239
magpies for the captive study. TVS was funded by the University of Exeter Research
240
Studentship. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
12
241
242
References
243
244
BBC (2003, 4th July) Magpies. Retrieved March 26th, 2014, from
245
http://news.bbc.co.uk/dna/place-lancashire/plain/A1094230
246
Bensom-Amram S, Weldele ML & Holekamp KE (2013) A comparison of innovative problem-
247
solving abilities between wild and captive spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Anim Behav
248
85:349-356.
249
250
Birkhead TR (1991) The Magpies: The ecology and behaviour of Black-billed and Yellow-billed
Magpies (1 ed.). London: T & AD Poyser.
251
Collins (2014) English dictionary: Definition of magpie. Retrieved March 26th, 2014 from
252
http://www.collinsdictionary.com/dictionary/english/magpie?showCookiePolicy=true
253
Garson GD (2013) Generalized Linear Models / Generalized Estimating Equations, 2013
254
Edition. Asheboro, NC: Statistical Associates Publishers.
255
Greenberg R (2003) The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative
256
behaviour of birds. In SM Reader & KN Laland (Eds) Animal Innovation (pp 175-196).
257
Oxford: OUP.
258
259
Greenberg R & Mettke-Hofmann C (2001) Ecological aspects of neophobia and neophilia in
birds. In V Nolan Jr & CF Thompson (Eds) Current Ornithology 16: 119-178.
260
Heinrich B (1988) Why do ravens fear their food? Condor 90: 950-952.
261
Heinrich B (1995) Neophilia and exploration in juvenile common ravens, Corvus corax. Anim
262
263
264
Behav 50: 695-704.
Huber L & Gajdon GK (2006) Technical intelligence in animals: the kea model. Anim Cog
9:295-305.
13
265
266
267
Jacobs IF, Osvath M, Osvath H, Mioduszewska B, von Bayern AMP & Kacelnik A (2014)
Object caching in corvids: Incidence and significane. Behav Proc 102:25-32.
Manchester Evening News (2007, 15th February) Magpie swoops to steal keys and tools.
268
Retrieved March 26th, 2014, from http://www.manchestereveningnews.co.uk/news/greater-
269
manchester-news/magpie-swoops-to-steal-keys-and-tools-1045325
270
Schaedelin FC , Taborsky M (2009) Extended phenotypes as signals. Biol Rev 84:293-313.
271
Sergio F, Blas J, Blanco G, Tanferna A, Lopez L, Lemus JA, et al. (2011) Raptor nest
272
273
decorations are a reliable threat against conspecifics. Science 331:327-330.
The Telegraph (2008, 28th August) Magpie steals woman's engagement ring and buries it in nest
274
for three years. Retrieved March 26th, 2014, from
275
http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/newstopics/howaboutthat/2637365/Magpie-steals-womans-
276
engagement-ring-and-buries-it-in-nest-for-three-years.html
277
278
279
280
281
Vernelli T (2013) The complexity of neophobia in a generalist foraging corvid: the common
magpie (Pica pica). Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Exeter.
Winterman, D (2008, 28th March) Why are magpies so often hated? BBC News Mag. Retrieved
March 26th, 2014, from http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/magazine/7316384.stm
14
282
Figure captions
283
284
Figure 1. Feeding behaviour of free-living magpies (6 sites, 202 total trials). Birds (a) fed more
285
quickly and (b) took more food items when no objects were present. The two columns in control
286
condition represent the left and right food piles. Error bars show the standard error. *P <0.05,
287
**P <0.005.
288
289
Figure 2. Ambivalence scores (±se) for field-tested magpies (6 sites, 202 total trials). Birds
290
showed more ambivalence when test objects were present. *P = 0.050, **P ≤0.026.
291
15
292
Figure 1. Feeding behaviour of free-living magpies (6 sites, 202 total trials). Birds (a) fed more
293
quickly and (b) took more food items when no objects were present. The two columns in control
294
condition represent the left and right food piles. Error bars show the standard error. *P <0.05,
295
**P <0.005.
296
297
16
298
Figure 2. Ambivalence scores (±se) for field-tested magpies (6 sites, 202 total trials). Birds
299
showed more ambivalence when test objects were present. *P = 0.050, **P ≤0.026.
300
Download