CHAPTER SEVENTEEN MODELS OF READING COMPREHENSION FOR PRIMARY SCHOOL STUDENTS SONIA PEČJAK,1A SVJETLANA KOLIĆ VEHOVEC2 AND ANJA PODLESEK1B 1. Introduction Investigation of reading comprehension has a long research tradition in psychology. Its importance has, however, grown even more within the frame of international reading literacy studies, e.g., PIRLS (Mullis, Martin, Gonzales, & Kennedy, 2003) and PISA (Štraus, Repež, & Štigl, 2007). Reading skills and abilities have always been important in the educational context, and even more nowadays in the era of information society. These skills represent an effective means of acceptance, organization, and usage of information in different areas. Thus, reading skills and abilities to comprehend written materials have become an important cross-curriculum competency of learning to learn, which influences one’s educational achievement, as the bulk of information in educational situations is transferred through written materials. The aim of our study was to develop a model of reading comprehension which would include different psychological characteristics of students, and which can be promoted in students by the teacher in order to achieve the competence “learning to learn”. 1 Faculty of Arts, University of Ljubljana, Ljubljana, Slovenia, Professor of educational psychology (sonja.pecjak@ff.uni-lj.si). B Associate professor (anja.podlesek@ff.uni-lj.si). 2 Full Professor, Faculty of Arts, University of Rijeka, Rijeka, Croatia (skolic@ffri.hr). A 308 Chapter Seventeen 1.1 What is reading comprehension? Kintsch (1998) defined reading comprehension as a combination of processes which arise from text, and integrative processes of that text in a reader's knowledge structure which is indicated in an interrelated net of concepts. Most of these processes are automated. Conscious control is necessary only when the information in the text interferes with the reader's background knowledge or when the reader does not possess sufficient knowledge to form a mental representation of the text. Oakhill and Cain (2004) see effective text comprehension also as an integrated and coherent representation of a text’s contents. Namely, the reader has to integrate the meanings of individual words, sentences and paragraphs, extract the text’s key ideas, and make inferences in order to understand the text. The key factors in a person's achievement in reading comprehension are: (i) various environmental characteristics and features, especially school environment (e.g., reading and writing instructions, enhancing students’ reading motivation) and family environment (e.g., the number of books a family owns, the frequency of parents’ reading to children, parents’ reading habits in general), and (ii) psychological characteristics of students (cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, and emotional). We therefore continue with a more detailed presentation of the students’ (meta)cognitive and motivational-emotional variables that have great educational potential. These variables can be significantly developed in students by a teacher’s systematically training them and thus improving reading comprehension in students. Variables that cannot be influenced at all or to a minor extent only (e.g., intelligence, working memory capacity and inference competence) were not included in the model. 1.2 Cognitive factors of reading comprehension Researchers report numerous factors that are significantly connected with reading comprehension and an individual's reading literacy. Numerous studies have investigated the connection between an individual's reading comprehension variables, such as vocabulary (Nagy, Diadikoy, & Andersen, 1993), use of cognitive and metacognitive strategies (Meyer, Brandt, & Bluth, 1980; Greasser, 2007) or specific strategies, e.g., summarizing (Van der Broek, Tzeng, Risden, Trabesco, & Basche, 2001) and reading comprehension. Each of these variables is closely connected with reading comprehension in younger and older students. Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 309 2. Students' vocabulary Students’ vocabulary indicates a knowledge of word meaning and, as defined by Beck and McKeown (1991), includes a lexical knowledge of words and the concepts related to those meanings. According to empirical studies, vocabulary is one of the most powerful predictors of reading comprehension: e.g., a child's vocabulary size in the preschool period as a predictor of reading comprehension in early period of schooling (Muter al., 2004; Share & Leiken, 2004); student’s vocabulary in the 3 rd and the 6th grade as predictors of sixth-grade pupil's reading comprehension (Aarnoutse and van Leeuwe, 1998). Some authors (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 1991; Perfetti, 1994) believe that vocabulary directly influences comprehension, whereas others point out that limited vocabulary does not necessarily lead to comprehension difficulties (e.g., Pany, Jenkins, & Schrek, 1982), or, as Cain, Oakhill, and Lemmon (2004) stressed, poor reading comprehension is a consequence of interaction between poor vocabulary and poor summarizing ability. The majority of lexical knowledge models distinguish between passive/receptive and active/productive vocabulary (Laufer & Goldstein, 2004). Researchers generally agree that passive/receptive vocabulary normally precedes active knowledge. Passive knowledge is related to listening and reading and reflects our ability to comprehend words that we hear or read. At the level of vocabulary, passive word knowledge refers to the ability to perceive the word form and recall its meaning. In contrast, active word knowledge is linked to speaking and writing, and refers to the ability to recall a pronounced or written word form for the meaning we want to express. Quellette (2006) established that only active vocabulary is a good predictor of reading comprehension, while, for example, passive or receptive vocabulary correlates more with decoding speed than with reading comprehension. This means that it is necessary to precisely define the dimension of the vocabulary that is measured. However, Didović and KolićVehovec (2009) compared the active and passive word knowledge and its relation to text comprehension in 11- and 15-year-old students and found that passive word knowledge was a stronger predictor of text comprehension (beta = .81 and .69, respectively) than active word knowledge (beta = .51 and .43, respectively). 3. Metacognitive knowledge of reading Metacognitive knowledge refers to a person’s declarative knowledge about the interactions between person, task, and strategy characteristics (Flavel, 310 Chapter Seventeen 1979). Metacognitive knowledge about reading includes a person’s knowledge about their reading, different types of reading tasks, and reading strategies (Alexander, Graham, & Harris, 1998; Baker & Brown, 1984; Paris, Lipson, & Wixson, 1983). Baker and Brown (1984) emphasised the knowledge and regulation that students exercise over reading strategies. Anderson and Armbruster (1982) found that poor readers do not skim, reread, integrate information, plan ahead, and make inferences, as often as more skilled readers do. Differences in metacognitive knowledge were also found between different age groups. Myers and Paris (1978) examined the metacognitive knowledge of children between 8 and 12 years of age and found that the older children knew more about text structure, various goals and using reading strategies to construct meaning and to resolve comprehension failures than the younger children. Pazzaglia, Beni, and Caccio (1999) found a significant improvement occurred in knowledge about goals and strategies from 8 to 13 years, and Kolić Vehovec and Bajšanski (2003) in metacognitive knowledge about reading strategies from 11 to 14 years. Furthermore, Kolić Vehovec and Bajšanski (2001) established that metacognitive knowledge is a significant predictor of elementary school students’ reading comprehension at different developmental stages. Knowledge and use of different (meta)cognitive reading strategies help students learn from texts effectively. In order to learn effectively, a student must activate his background knowledge, which helps him understand new texts. Sometimes a student does not know how to relate his background knowledge to new information, which does not allow comprehension or at least interferes with the understanding of the text. The knowledge of learning strategies that contribute to the attainment of a learning goal and conscious use of such strategies help identify relevant information from text, recall long-term knowledge, monitor and direct the use of these strategies in order to model comprehension. Numerous interventional studies show that planned training can improve students’ reading comprehension. Cromley and Azevedo (2007) reported that 51 interventional studies gave positive results. 4. Summarizing strategy Summarizing represents one of the most important after-reading strategies which indicate one's actual stage of reading comprehension. While summarizing, a reader must be able to identify the main ideas in a text, assess the importance of particular information, and incorporate the relevant information into a coherent entity – an abstract (Anderson & Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 311 Armbruster, 1982; Friend, 2001). Johnston and Afflebach (1985) claimed that the ability to identify the main ideas in a text is the essence of reading comprehension. Text summarizing requires omitting unimportant information, as well as condensing and organizing text information. Summarizing is more effective when students use their own words to form the connections between the concepts and relate the concepts to their own prior knowledge (Wittrock, 1990). Many studies have found that summarizing improves students’ comprehension and helps students monitor their comprehension (Armbruster, Anderson, & Ostertag, 1987; King, 1992; Symons, Richards, & Greene, 1995; Wittrock, 1990). Wittrock (1990) suggested that the process of generating summaries helps readers build relations between the concepts contained in a text, as well as link these concepts to prior knowledge. Others have suggested that summarizing improves comprehension by promoting self-monitoring during reading (Brown & Day, 1983; Palincsar & Brown, 1984; Paris & Lindauer, 1982), which may signal comprehension breaks and invite readers to initiate fix-up strategies to repair breaks in comprehension (Winne & Hadwin, 1998). Thiede and Anderson (2003) demonstrated that summarizing of texts enhances comprehension by improving metacomprehension accuracy and increasing the effectiveness of self-regulated study. Yussen (1982) found out that children’s ability to state the main topic of a text increases markedly from 2nd to 6th grade. Younger students and poor readers often demonstrate difficulties with summarizing strategy. Correlation coefficients between reading comprehension and the ability to summarize are in the range from .20 to .30 for elementary school students, and around .31 for secondary school students (Kolić Vehovec & Bajšanski, 2001). 4.1 Motivational-emotional factors of reading comprehension In studies investigating the factors connected with reading comprehension, motivational factors are consistently recognized to be important. They influence the intensity, perseverance, and direction of one's reading behaviour. Schneider (2001) emphasized that motivational factors explain differences in reading achievement of highly competent students. Along with Eccles’ (1983) statement that beliefs, values and goals can be conceptualized as different questions students can ask themselves, Wigfield (1997) stated that the questions most directly related to motivation for reading are: Can I be a good reader? and Do I want to be a good reader and why? The answer to the first question captures the children’s ability beliefs or perceived competency, and the answer to the 312 Chapter Seventeen second question captures the perceived task value, including interest value, as well as intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. 4.2 Perceived competency Perceived competency has been one of the most widely investigated elements among motivational factors in the last decade. This concept is congruent with Bandura’s self-efficacy (1997), defined as an individual's belief that one is capable of performing in a certain manner to attain certain goals. Congruent with this definition, perceived reading competency is a person's belief in their own reading capacities or belief to be able solve reading tasks and attain goals by reading. The difference between the two concepts is that there is no comparison with others included in self-efficacy, while the latter is included in the concept of perceived self-efficacy (e.g., a student is convinced that he or she will be able to solve a reading task well through hard work, but compared to other students he or she does not perceive him or herself as a good reader). Perceived reading competency is positively related to: (i) the task choice – the more one feels competent, the higher the challenge reading tasks represent for them and so they tend to choose more difficult tasks and use deeper learning strategies (Dweck, 1999; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002); (ii) effort and perseverance – the more one is confident in oneself, the greater the amount of effort one is willing to invest in reading comprehension and will do it with more perseverance; and (iii) achievement – higher competency is directly related to better reading achievement (Conlon, Zimmmer-Gembeck, Creed, & Tucker, 2006; Gambrell, Palmer, Codling, & Mazzoni, 1996). Different studies point to a direct connection between perceived reading competency and reading comprehension (Chapman & Tunmer, 1995; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). Pečjak, Bucik, Gradišar and Peklaj (2006) found that perceived reading competency is significantly related to reading efficiency of third (r = .44) and seventh grade primary school students (r = .21). Similarly, Conlon et al. (2006) reported a significant and even higher correlation between perceived competency and reading comprehension in 12-year-old students (r = .53). 4.3 Interest Among the motivational factors connected with reading comprehension, reading interest is commonly mentioned. Deci (1998) established a positive correlation between reading comprehension and reading interest and Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 313 intrinsic motivation for reading. Usually, researchers distinguish between personal/content and situational interest (e.g., Schiefele, 2001). Situational interest is defined as a content-specific motivational variable that is determined by emotional and value beliefs in connection with the content of reading. In this manner, emotional beliefs are associated with enjoyment while reading certain contents, and value beliefs with one’s estimation of the importance of reading. Schiefele and Krapp (1996) established that content interest is connected with deeper learning approaches/strategies which include recall of main ideas and coherence of recalled ideas. Schiefele (1996) also found a significant connection between personal interest and reading comprehension at a concurrent control of cognitive factors (r = .27). Guthrie et al. (1998) found out that the amount of reading (as a personal interest indicator) predicted reading comprehension in groups of 3rd, 5th and 10th grade students, even when a number of variables that correlate with reading comprehension (prior knowledge, reading efficacy) were controlled. Hidi (1990, 2001) showed in her meta-analytical studies that reading interest had an important effect on reading comprehension at the level of cognitive organization of reading material and was also important for memorizing and recall. Similar are the findings by Renninger (1992), while others, for example Alexander, Murphy, Woods, Duhon, and Parker (1997), suggest that this connection is probably due to the fact that interest prolongs learning time and stimulates the use of deeper comprehension strategies. Namely, Shiefele (1991, 2001) established that interest is the factor that motivates readers to get involved in reading and to try to achieve better comprehension. Interest is the activator of the strategies that influence deeper processing. Altogether, interest is also significantly connected with other motivational factors, such as involvement (flow) and enjoyment. Both result in comprehension. In contrast with these findings, Köller, Baumert, and Schnabel (2000) did not find a significant correlation between interest and reading achievement at controlled concurrent background knowledge. 4.4 Intrinsic motivation Intrinsic motivation refers to being motivated and curious to carry out an activity for its own sake. Thus, intrinsic motivation has some parallels with the interest aspect of task value as defined by Eccles et al. (1983). One aspect of intrinsic motivation is becoming completely involved in the current activity, which Csikszentmihalyi (1990) describes as flow. Many readers have experienced flow while reading a book, losing track of time and self-awareness. Intrinsic motivation is also described in Oldfather´s 314 Chapter Seventeen concept (1992) of continuing impulse to learn, which is characterized by intense involvement, curiosity and a search for understanding. Applying this concept to reading, it could be said that a reader’s engagement in reading will be greatly facilitated when they are intrinsically motivated to read. 4.4 Emotions while reading Reading comprehension is also affected by children’s attitudes toward reading, generally defined as an individual´s feelings about reading (Alexander & Filler, 1976). These feelings should influence how much individuals involve themselves in reading. Therefore, attitudes toward reading should relate to reading motivation (Matthewson, 1994). However, the effect of emotions and mood on text comprehension is probably indirect through their influence on different cognitive and metacognitive factors. In this manner, emotions would result in reading comprehension through their effect on working memory (Ellis & Moore, 2000). Negative emotions preoccupy working memory in such a manner that an individual is more concerned with his own negative emotions than with cognitive task demands (Ellis & Ashbrook, 1988; Seiber & Ellis, 1991). Depressed-mode state can produce poorer recall in numerous situations, including variations in awareness (Hertel & Hardin, 1990) and elaborative encoding (Potts, Camp, & Coyne, 1989). Moreover, the attitude towards reading is an important factor in reading achievement (McKenna, 2001; McKenna & Kear, 1990; McKenna, Kear, & Ellsworth, 1995). However, it has been emphasized that attitudes are a very complex factor – they have emotional connotations as well as cognitive components and are experience-based. In their longitudinal study, Kush and Watkins (1996) established that a positive attitude towards reading significantly decreases during schooling. Although the decline is greater for boys than for girls, in both genders there is also a decrease in recreational and educational reading. Even more important are findings from studies that use regression analysis procedures to determine the relation between different variables and reading comprehension. A relation between reading comprehension and metacognition, learning strategies, background knowledge, and decoding speed was found by Artlet, Schiefele, and Schneider (2001) for secondary school students and by Ehrlich, Kurtz-Costes and Loridant (1993) for elementary school students. Guthrie, Wigfield, Metsala, and Cox (1999) found a connection between students’ background knowledge, vocabulary and reading comprehension. Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 315 5. Study In our study, we tried to ascertain how cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, and emotional factors affect reading comprehension in younger and older primary school students (5 th and 9th grades). Following different theoretical findings and empirical evidence from studies which investigated the role of specific factors in reading comprehension, we proposed a model of an indirect effect of motivational and emotional variables (reading interest, perceived competency, flow and reading emotions) on reading comprehension through (meta)cognitive variables and of a direct effect of cognitive and metacognitive variables (vocabulary, summarizing, and metacognitive knowledge) on reading comprehension. Alexander et al. (1997) suggested that the effect of interest on text comprehension is probably due to the fact that interest prolongs learning time and stimulates the use of deeper comprehension strategies. Accordingly, we proposed that interest affects vocabulary and summarizing skill/strategy. Since perceived reading competence is related to effort and perseverance (Dweck, 1999; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002), we predicted its significant effect on gaining vocabulary and metacognitive knowledge and developing summarizing skills. We also proposed a direct effect of perceived competence on reading comprehension only for competency, based on the empirical evidence reported by different studies (Chapman & Tunmer, 1995; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). Taking into account the proposition that emotions affect reading comprehension through their effect on working memory (Ellis & Moore, 2000), we propose that emotions may have an effect on summarizing and metacognitive knowledge. In the reading comprehension model for older primary school students, flow was also included as an aspect of intrinsic motivation. We propose that reader engagement facilitated by flow will enhance vocabulary and metacognitive knowledge. The effect of cognitive and metacognitive variables on text comprehension was already well documented and described in the introduction. The proposed reading comprehension models for younger and older students are presented in Figure 17-1. 316 Chapter Seventeen Figure 17-1. Proposed reading comprehension model for primary school students. 5.1 Participants A total of 885 students from nine Slovenian and Croatian primary schools participated in the study. The first sample included 380 fifth grade students from Slovenia and fourth grade students from Croatia (students enter primary school in Slovenia at 6 years and in Croatia at 7 years). The students were the same age (M = 10.7 years) and had been taught to read for equally long periods of time because students start learning to read in the second grade in Slovenia. The second sample included 505 students from the finishing year (of primary school) - ninth/eighth grade students from Slovenian and Croatian primary schools. The average age was 14.6 years. No significant differences in reading comprehension were found among the Croatian and Slovenian students neither in the younger students (Slovenian students: M = 9.3; SD = 2.81, Croatian students: M = 9.7; SD = 2.83, F(298) = 1.462; p = .228) nor in the older students (Slovenian students: M = 10.57; SD = 2.25, Croatian students: M = 10.95; SD = 2.51, F(379) = 2.135; p = .145). Therefore, we treated the Croatian and Slovenian students as a joint sample. 5.2 Procedure Reading comprehension was measured using two exploratory texts. Vocabulary was measured by Hershel's (1963) Test of Reading Vocabulary: Level 4 for the younger students and Level 8 for the older students. It comprises 40 tasks. The students have to answer the questions by choosing the appropriate word from among five choices. The following is a sample item: “The opposite of near is: A – here, B – beneath, C – now, Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 317 D – far, E – inside”. The test time is limited to 10 minutes. The internal consistency alpha coefficient was .86. Summarizing was assessed with three short texts: Water Pollution (120 words), Traffic (99 words), and Healthy Food (110 words). The students are asked to summarize the main ideas from the texts after reading them. The criteria for the answers were adapted from Kozminsky and Graetz (1986) and Friend (2001). Three semantic units, which represented the main ideas of the text, were identified in each text. The number of points received for each text equaled the number of correctly summarized units. Two independent raters assessed the students' abstracts. If the scores differed, the raters had to reach a consensus on their scoring. The internal consistency for this test was .86. Metacognitive Knowledge Questionnaire was constructed according to Gunning’s questionnaire (1996) and Schmitt’s questionnaire of metacognitive knowledge (1990) for the purposes of this study. It measures students' metacognitive knowledge about reading in general and academic reading in particular, especially the awareness of using different reading comprehension monitoring strategies. The questionnaire has 14 items with multiple-choice questions. Students have to choose the answer that in their opinion best describes the main purpose of reading, and they have to display a knowledge of different reading and learning strategies. Reading Motivation was measured using the Reading Motivation Questionnaire for Young Students (RMQYS) (Peklaj, Bucik, 2003 as cited in Pečjak et al. 2006). It comprises 28 items. The students use a 3-point pictorial scale to assess how valid a statement is for them (1– not at all true for me; 3 – always true for me). An exploratory factor analysis revealed three factors: (i) interest (Cronbach alpha = .81); (ii) (in) perceived competency (Cronbach alpha = .72); and (iii) self-efficiency in oral reading (Cronbach alpha = .63). The items are scored according to the manual, with higher scores implying a more expressed motivational factor. Only the first two factors were included in the model due to a lower reliability of the third one. The adapted version of The Motivation for Reading Questionnaire ([MRQ]; Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997), composed of 40 items, was used for the older students. They were asked to assess the items on a 5-point Likert scale (1- not at all true for me; 5 – always true for me). Factor analysis showed three factors: interest for reading (Cronbach alpha = .88); flow (Cronbach alpha = .88); and perceived competency (Cronbach alpha = .67). Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS) by McKenna and Kear (1990) measures the emotional dimension of reading. The questionnaire contains 20 items yielding two scores: (i) a recreational reading score (Cronbach alpha = .87); and (ii) an academic reading score (Cronbach alpha 318 Chapter Seventeen = .86). The students rated their emotions during reading on a 4-point scale (1 – very bad, 4 – very good). There was only a total reading score included in the study - - the sum of the recreational and the academic reading scores. Path analyses were performed using LISREL 8.8 to analyze the relations between variables and to verify whether our data fit the proposed model. Because most of the variables were not normally distributed, we carried out calculations on their normalized scores. After eliminating students with missing data on one or more variables from the samples, the data from 323 younger and 415 older students were analyzed separately. Covariance between the variables was entered into a model, and the maximum likelihood method was used for estimating the parameters. We allowed for the correlation between exogenous variables and for correlated errors in intermediary variables. 5.3 Results The aim of our study was to determine how the selected cognitive, metacognitive, motivational and emotional factors affect reading comprehension in younger and older primary school students. A model of the direct and indirect effects of these factors on reading comprehension was proposed (see Figure 17-1). The goodness of fit of the proposed model was examined separately for the younger and the older students. The results of path analyses are two most parsimonious models, one for the younger (Figure 17-2) and one for the older students (Figure 17-3). The arrows show the direction of the effect between variables, and the values on the arrows correspond to standardized regression coefficients, which show the predictive power of each predictor, if participants are equalled with regard to other variables. Regression coefficient shows the amount of change in standard deviation of a criterion variable if the value of the effecting variable increases for one standard deviation. In Figure 17-3 all standardized regression coefficients are statistically significant at the level of p < .05. Coefficients show satisfactory fit of the proposed model to the data, χ 2(15) = 10.90; p = .09; RMSEA = .05, 90% confidence interval for RMSEA = .00 –.110 AGFI = .96; NNFI = .97. Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 319 Figure 17-2. A reading comprehension model for fifth grade students. The factors included in the model explained 37% of variance of reading comprehension among the younger students. The direct effects of all (meta)cognitive factors on reading comprehension appeared significant vocabulary (.46), summarizing (.17), and metacognitive knowledge (.15). As in many previous studies (e.g., Aarnoutse & van Leeuwe, 1998; Beck & McKeown, 1991; Pečjak, 1989; Perfetti, 1994), the extent of vocabulary was also confirmed to be the most powerful predictor of reading comprehension in our study. Somewhat weaker was the relation between reading comprehension and the ability to summarize. Still, mastering the strategies of summarizing gives students an ability to detect important information within a large amount of information at the micro level and to combine them into a meaning (information at the macro level). This represents the key process of reading comprehension (Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978). The results of other studies showed that a knowledge of reading strategies which can lead the reader through the text and help them monitor the process of comprehension improves their understanding of the text contents (e.g., Alexander, Graham, & Harris, 1998). However, students at the age of 10 have not yet mastered the summarizing skill well enough to improve comprehension to any greater degree. As Pintrich and Zusho (2002) discussed, when children start using the strategy they do not really benefit from it. After they have gained a considerable amount of practice and experience, they are able to use the strategy in a more adaptive and successful way. Further on, we found that reading interest, as a motivational factor, has an indirect influence on reading comprehension through vocabulary (.25). Emotions, on the other hand, have an indirect effect on reading comprehension through summarizing. Simultaneously, we found close 320 Chapter Seventeen intercorrelations between emotions during reading and reading interest (.66). These results show the importance of emotions in the process of reading, while they influence (meta)cognitive factors directly (through summarizing), as well as indirectly (through interest). Such high intercorrelations with interest confirm the emotional dimension as one of the structural dimensions of reading interest. Reading interest had a significant connection only with vocabulary (.25). This means that students with greater reading interest have a broader vocabulary, which was confirmed in the previous studies as well. Namely, greater reading interest leads to more frequent reading, and if they read more often, readers absorb a larger amount and more diverse reading material which they want to comprehend. Reading different reading materials leads readers to expand their (at least receptive) vocabulary. Perceived competency was not directly related to (meta)cognitive variables. The obtained intercorrelations were low and not statistically significant, which leads us conclude that perceived competence in younger students is more a consequence of their successful text comprehension than its cause. This could also be a sign of a reader’s attributions of achievement in reading comprehension, which could be important factors of competency. Teachers have an important role in this respect. If teachers do not reflect on the reading achievement/comprehension with their students, if they do not emphasize the importance of vocabulary and metacognitive knowledge especially, then their students will not judge their own reading effectiveness/efficiency through the meaning of those factors. Figure 17-3. A reading comprehension model for ninth grade students. Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 321 Figure 17-3 illustrates the final model of the relations between metacognitive, cognitive, motivational, and emotional variables to reading comprehension of the ninth grade students. All standardized regression coefficients were statistically significant at the level of p <.05. The model shows adequate goodness of fit to the data, χ2(12) = 12.04; p = .14; RMSEA = .04, 90% confidence interval for RMSEA = .00–.07; AGFI = .96; NNFI = .98. When the model was applied to the older students, the goodness of fit was somewhat poorer – we were able to explain 23% of variance in reading comprehension. Among the variables directly related to reading comprehension, two were (meta)cognitive – vocabulary and metacognitive knowledge. Vocabulary (with a path coefficient of .40) was found to be a solid predictor of good reading comprehension, and poor vocabulary can result in reading comprehension difficulties. This assumption is also supported by some other studies, which included samples of older primary school students (e.g., Pečjak, 1989; Sternberg, 1987; Thorndike, 1973; Walczyk & Taylor, 1996). Metacognitive knowledge (.17) was another important factor of reading comprehension in the older students. This indicates that being aware of different strategies and using them before, during, and after reading is probably even more important for older students than for younger ones, as they are exposed to much more diversified reading material, have to attain higher reading goals or complete various reading tasks which demand the use of different strategies. We can conclude that expanding students’ vocabulary and their knowledge of different reading strategies could result in better reading comprehension at the end of primary school. It is also worth noting that other authors established a higher correlation between specific learning strategies and reading comprehension of a specific kind of text/tasks than between general learning strategies and reading comprehension of a text in general (Artlet, 2000; O'Neal & Abedi, 1996). Although students aged 15 are capable of using strategies, they still might not do so, because of the decline in their intrinsic motivation for learning (Pintrich & Zusho, 2002). Among the motivational variables, a strong intercorrelation (.64) was found between reading interest and flow. Flow (involvement, Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997) represents an extremely powerful form of intrinsic reading interest. This high intercorrelation also confirms that reading interest and flow are two very similar constructs. Furthermore, a strong connection was found between motivational and emotional variables (between emotions during reading and reading interest r = .55; between emotions during reading and flow r = .69). 322 Chapter Seventeen An interesting finding was that of all motivational-emotional factors and (meta)cognitive factors only flow and competency were significantly related to other variables: the path coefficient from flow to vocabulary was .21, from flow to metacognitive knowledge .15, and from competency to summarizing .13. The correlations between flow and (meta)cognitive factors probably indicate that there are reciprocal relationships between these variables. A reader involved in reading to such an extent that time and space are irrelevant for them will read more and will therefore acquire a richer vocabulary. This type of reading is also accompanied by more positive emotions (high intercorrelation between emotions and flow: .69). Interactive functioning of cognitive and emotional factors enhances reading motivation and results in better reading comprehension (Schallert & Reed, 1997). In our study, interest in reading was not connected with (meta)cognitive factors or with reading comprehension, contrary to other empirical studies which confirmed such a relationship (Artlet, Schiefele, & Schneider, 2001; Pečjak & Gradišar, 2002; Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997). The most plausible reason was a high intercorrelation of interest and flow (.64), meaning that interest can have an indirect effect on vocabulary and metacognitve knowledge and, with the latter two, on reading comprehension through flow. On the other hand, our findings are supported by the study by Ainley, Hillman, and Hidi (2002), who found that only low or moderate positive correlations existed between measures of personal (more general) and content (more specific) reading interest. In their model, students’ personal interest was not a significant predictor of content interest. This is why teachers should create learning situations that trigger students’ situational interest (such as choosing between two similar texts), and provide them with feedback that would help students maintain their attention and promote positive emotions. All of this could result in greater personal interest (Hidi, 2006). 6. Conclusions and pedagogical implications Some important conclusions and pedagogical implications can be derived from our study. We confirmed the model of motivational-emotional and (meta)cognitive factors of reading comprehension. The proposed model, however, better explained the variance in reading comprehension in the younger than in the older students (37% of variance in reading comprehension in the younger students vs. 23% of variance in reading comprehension in the older students). Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 323 The variables included in the model were those we proposed to be easier for the teachers to systematically develop in the classroom. Therefore, we find it is sensible to develop vocabulary and, more importantly, metacognitive awareness and (summarizing) strategies in younger students and not only in the final years of primary school. It seems as if students’ characteristics which teachers will find difficult if not impossible to influence (e.g., general intelligence, inference generation, working memory capacity) have a more important role in reading comprehension. For example, Rončević Zubković (2008) found that intelligence, short-term and working memory explained 50% of variance of text comprehension in students aged 14. We established that motivational and emotional factors are strongly interconnected; therefore, it seems that one should not divide these factors artificially but rather treat them as a single entity. According to our model, vocabulary had the strongest effect among the cognitive factors. Based on these findings, we believe that increasing students’ vocabulary may lead to greater reading comprehension. It is therefore important that teachers enable their students to enlarge their active and receptive vocabulary (general and more specific, which is important for the comprehension of written materials in different courses). Teachers can also systematically promote vocabulary to help students at risk, for example, students from socially unprivileged environments. Studies show that vocabulary is a factor that prevents such students from comprehending written material (Dickinson, McCabe, Anastasopoulos, Peisner-Feinberg, & Poe, 2003). The deficit, however, persists through the first years of schooling (Biemiller, 2005). Although a direct connection between summarizing and reading comprehension was only significant for the younger students and an indirect effect for the older students, it is also important for teachers and school psychologists to stress the importance of learning different reading strategies to achieve better reading comprehension and improve academic achievement. Moreover, it is even more important to model the use of these strategies and to create such learning situations in which students would benefit from the use of these strategies, and consequently become more willing to use them. Since competency affects summarizing, it is important for teachers to create conditions in which all students could experience success in reading. Competency was the only motivational factor that was directly connected with reading comprehension in the older students. Although the path coefficient was not statistically significant, this connection shows the importance of grading reading task and text appropriately when working with students who have reading difficulties. 324 Chapter Seventeen Teachers should help these students attain reading goals and strengthen their reading competency. Individualized work can include adaptation of reading goals, choosing different reading materials, and different teaching methods. Since a lack of motivation in the older students was found to be an important factor of delay in metacognitive knowledge and reading strategy use important for learning and academic success, teachers should become aware of this fact and put greater effort into motivating students to read h by offering higher elementary school students a variety of texts that could attract their interest and induce positive emotional states during reading. Developing positive attitudes towards reading will stimulate students to read with engagement and make better use of reading strategies, such as summarizing, ultimately developing the strategic reading necessary for successful at school (Kozminsky & Kozminsky, 2001). Strategic reading reflects metacognition and motivation, because readers need to know the strategies and be willing to use them. It is the basis for the self-regulation of learning that develops autonomy and control by the student who monitors, regulates, and directs the actions toward learning goals, expanding expertise, and self-improvement (Zimmerman, 2000). Self-regulated students monitor the efficacy of their learning strategies and replace inefficient strategies with different ones. The role of teachers in developing efficient readers is crucial, because teachers provide competent models for reading strategy use and important motivators for students’ engagement in strategic behaviours. References Aarnoutse, C., & van Leeuwe, J. (1998). Relation between reading comprehension, vocabulary, reading pleasure, and reading Frequency. Educational Research and Evaluation, 4(2), 143–166. Ainley, M., Hillman, K., & Hidi, S. (2002). Gender and interest processes in response to literary texts: situtational and individual interest. Learning and Instruction, 12, 411–428. Alexander, P.A., Graham, S., & Harris, K.R. (1998). A perspective on strategy research: Progress and prospects. Educational psychology Review, 10, 129-154. Alexander, P. A., Murphy, P. K., Woods, B. S., Duhon, K. E., & Parker, D. (1997). College instruction and concomitant changes in students' knowledge, interest, and strategy use: A study of domain learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 125–146. Alexander, H. & Filler, R.C. (1976). Attitudes and reading. Newark, DL: International Reading Association. Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 325 Alfassi, M. (2004). Reading to learn: Effects of combined strategy instruction on high school students. Journal of Educational Research, 97, 171–184. Anderson, T.H. & Armbruster, B.B. (1982). Reader and text-studying strategies. In W. Otto & S. White (Eds), Reading Expository Material (pp. 219-242). London: Academic Press. Armbruster, B. B., Anderson, T. H., & Ostertag, J. (1987). Does Text Structure/Summarization Instruction Facilitate Learning from Expository Text? Reading Research Quarterly, 22(3), 331–346. Artlet, C. (2000). Strategisches Lernen. Münster: Waxmann. Artlet, C., Schiefele, U., & Schneider, W. (2001). Predictors of reading literacy. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 16(3), 363–383. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman. Baker, L., Brown, A.L. (1984): Metacognitive skills and reading. U P.D. Pearson (Ur.), Handbook of reading research, 353-394. New York: Longman. Beck, I., & McKeown, M. (1991). Conditions of vocabulary acquisition. In R. Bar, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research: Vol 2 (pp. 789–814). White Plains, NY: Longman. Biemiller, A. (2005). Size and sequence in vocabulary development: Implications for choosing words for primary grade vocabulary instruction. In A. Hiebert, & M. Kamil (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice (pp. 223–242). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brown, A. L. & Day, J. D. (1983). Macrorules for summarizing texts: The development of expertise. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 22(1), 1–14. Cain, K., Oakhill, J. (2004). Reading Comprehension Difficulties. V T. Nunes, P. Bryant (eds.). Handbook of children's literacy (pp. 313-338). Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer Academic Publisher Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence on reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 671–681. Chapman, J. & Tunmer, W. (1995). Development of young children’s reading self-concepts: An examination of emergent subcomponents and their relationship with reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 154–167. Conlon, E. G., Zimmmer-Gembeck, M .J., Creed, P. A., & Tucker, M. (2006). Family history, self-perceptions, attitudes and cognitive abilities 326 Chapter Seventeen are associated with early adolescent reading skills. Journal of research in reading, 29(1), 11–32. Cromley, J. G., & Azavedo, R. (2007). Testing and Refining the Direct and Inferential Mediation Model of Reading Comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 311–325. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper and Row. Deci, E. L. (1998). The relation of interest to motivation and human needs – The self-determination theory viewpoint. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, K. A. Renninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and learning (pp. 146– 162). Kiel: Institute for Science Education, University of Kiel. Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, M. D. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 465–481. Didović, M., & Kolić Vehovec, S. (2009). Različiti aspekti poznavanja riječnika i razumijevanja teksta kod učenika osnovne škole [Different aspects of vocabulary knowledge and text comprehension in elementary school students]. Psihologijske teme, 18(1), 99-117. Dweck, C. (1999). Self-theories: The role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press Eccles, J.S. (1983). Expectancies, values and academic behaviors. In J.T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motives (pp. 75-146). San Francisco: Freeman. Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132. Ehrlich, M.F., Kurtz-Costes, B. & Loridant, C. (1993). Cognitive and motivational determinants of reading comprehension in good and poor readers. Journal of Reading Behavior, Vol 25(4), 365-381. Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In K. Friedler, & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behaviour (pp. 25–43). Toronto, Canada: Hogrefe. Ellis, H. C., & Moore, B. A. (2000). Mode and memory. In T. Daglesish, & M. Power (Eds.), The handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 34-46). Chichester, England: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd. Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245. Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 327 Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail, & W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on development of memory and cognition (pp. 3–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (1993). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. Friend, R. (2001). Effects of strategy instruction on summary writing of college students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26(1), 3–24. Gambrell, L. B., Palmer, B. M., Codling, R. M., & Mazzoni, S. A. (1996). Assessing motivation to read. The Reading Teacher, 49(7), 518–533. Graesser, A. C. (2007). An introduction to strategic reading comprehension. In D. McNamara (Ed.), Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, interventions, and technologies (pp. 3-26). Mahwah, NJ: LEA. Gunning, T. G. (1996). Creating reading instruction for all children. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Metsala, J. L., & Cox, K. E. (1999). Motivational and cognitive predictors of text comprehension and reading amount. Scientific studies of reading, 3(3), 231–256. Hershel, T. M. (1963). Test of Reading Level 3 – Elementary Form. In T. M. Herschel (Ed.), The 20th Yearbook of the National Council on Measurement in Education. Ann Arbor, MI: SGSR. Hertel, P.T., & Hardin, T. S. (1990). Remembering with and without awareness in a depressed mood: Evidence of deficit of initiative. Journal of experimental Psychology: General, 119, 45–59. Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental resource for learning. Review of Educational Research, 60(4), 549–571. Hidi, S. (2001). Interest, reading, and learning: theoretical and practical consideration. Educational Psychology Review, 13(3), 191–209. Hidi, S. (2006). Interest: A unique motivational variable. Educational research review, 1, 69–82. King, A. (1992). Comparison of self-questioning, summarization, and note taking review as strategies for learning from lectures. American Educational Research Journal, 29, 303-323. Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A Paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a Model of Text Comprehension and Production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394. Kolić Vehovec, S., & Bajšanski, I. (2003). Children's metacognition as predictor of reading comprehension at different developmental levels. In 328 Chapter Seventeen Proceedings of the 12th European conference on Reading (pp. 216–222). Dublin: Reading Association of Ireland. Köller, O., Baumert, J., & Schnabel, K. (2000). Zum Zusammenspiel von schulischen Interesse und Lernen im Fach Mathematik: Längschnittanalysen in der Sekundarstufen I und II. In U. Schiefele, & W. P. Wild (Eds.). Interesse und Lernmotivation. Untersuchungen zur Entwicklung, Förderung und Wirkung (pp. 163–181). Münster: Waxmann. Kozminsky, E. & Kozminsky, L. (2001). How do general knowledge and reading strategies ability relate to reading comprehension of high school students at different educational levels? Journal of Research in Reading, 24(2), 187–204. Kush, J. C., & Watkins, M. W. (1996). Long-term stability of children’s attitudes toward reading. The Journal of Educational Research, 89(5), 315−319. Laufer, B. & Goldstein, Z. (2004). Testing vocabulary knowledge: Size, strength and computer adaptiveness. Language learning, 54(3), 399– 436. Matthewson, G.C. (1994). Model of Attitude Influence upon Reading and learning to Read. In R.B. Ruddell, M. Rapp Ruddell, H. Singer. Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading (pp.1131-1161). Newark, Delaware: IRA McKenna, M. C. (2001). Development of reading attitude. In L. Verhoeven, & C. E. Snow (Eds.), Literacy and motivation: reading engagement in individuals and groups (pp. 135–158). Mahwah: LEA. McKenna, M. C., & Kear, D. J. (1990). Measuring attitude toward reading: A new tool for teachers. Journal of reading, 43, 626–639. McKenna, M. C., Kear, D. J., & Ellsworth, R. A. (1995). Children's attitude toward reading: a national survey. Reading Research Quarterly, 30(4), 934−956. Meyer, B. J. F., Brandt, D. H., & Bluth, G. J. (1980). Use of top-level structure in text: Key for reading comprehension of nine grade students. Reading Research Quarterly, 16(1), 72–103. Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J., & Kennedy, A. M. (2003). PIRLS 2001 International report. Chestnut Hill, MA: PIRLS International Study Center. Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, Rimes, Vocabulary, and Grammatical Skills as Foundations of Early Reading Development: Evidence From a Longitudinal Study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665–681. Myers, M. & Paris, S. G. (1978) Children's metacognitive knowledge about reading. Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 329 Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(5), 680–690. Nagy, W. E., Diadikoy, L., & Anderson, R. (1993). The acquisition of morphology: learning the contribution of suffixes to the meanings of derivatives. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 155–170. Oldfather, P. (1992). Sharing the Ownership of Knowing: A constructivist Concept of Motivation for Literacy learning. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Reading Conference, San Antonio, Texas, 2.12.1992 Palincsar, A. S. & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension – fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 117–175. Pany, D., Jenkins, J. R., & Schreck, J. (1982). Vocabulary instruction: effects on word knowledge and reading comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly, 5, 202–215. Paris, S. G., Lipson, M. Y., & Wixson, K. K. (1983). Becoming a strategic reader. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8(3), 293–316. Paris, S. G., & Lindauer, B. K. (1982). The development of cognitive skills during childhood. In B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 333-349). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Pazzaglia, Beni and Caccio (1999). The role of working memory and metacognition in reading comprehension difficulties. V T. E. Scruggs in M. Mastropieri (ur.), Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities, Vol. 13 (str. 115-134). Greenwich, Conn.: JAI Press. Pečjak, S. (1989). Tehnike za izboljšanje bralne učinkovitosti: Eksperimentalni program hitrega branja. Magistrsko delo (neobjavljeno). [Techniques for improving reading efficacy: experimental programme of quick reading]. Ljubljana: Filozofska fakulteta v Ljubljani. Pečjak, S., & Gradišar, A. (2002). Bralne učne strategije [Reading learning strategies]. Ljubljana: Zavod RS za šolstvo. Pečjak, S., Bucik, N., Gradišar, A. & Peklaj, C. (2006). Bralna motivacija v šoli: merjenje in razvijanje [Reading motivation in school: measurement and development]. Ljubljana: Zavod RS za šolstvo. Perfetti, C. A. (1994). Psycholinguistics and reading ability. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 849-894). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Pintrich, P.R. & Zusho, A. (2002). The development of academic selfregulation: The role of cognitive and motivational factors. In A. Wigfield & J.S. Eccles (Eds.). Development of achievement motivation (pp. 249-284). San Diego: Academic Press. 330 Chapter Seventeen Potts, R., Camp, C. & Coyne, C. (1989). The relationship between naturally occurring dysphoric moods, elaborative encoding, and recall performance. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 197–205. Quellette, G. P. (2006). What's meaning got to do with it: the role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554–566. Renninger, K. A. (1992). Individual interest and development: Implications for theory and practice. In K. A. Renninger, & S. Hidi (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 161-196). Hillsdale; NJ: Erlbaum. Rončević Zubković, B. (2008). Uloga radnog pamćenja i strategijskog procesiranja u razumijevanju pri čitanju kod djece [The role of working memory and strategic processing in children’s reading comprehension]. Unpublished PhD thesis. University of Zagreb, Croatia. Schallert, D. L., & Reed, J. H. (1997). The pull of the text and the process of involvement in one's reading. In J. Guthrie, & A. Wigfield (Ed.), Promoting Literacy through Integrated Teaching, (pp. 68–85). Chicago: International Reading Association. Schiefele, U., & Krapp, A. (1996). Topic interest and free recall of expository test. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 141–160. Schiefele, U. (1996). Motivation und lernen mit Texten. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Schiefele, U. (2001). The role of interest in motivation and learning. V J.M. Collis, S. Messick (ur.), Intelligence and personality: Bridging the gap in theory and measurement (str. 163-193). Hillsdale; NJ: Erlbaum. Schmitt, M. C. (1990). A questionnaire to measure children's awareness of strategic reading processes. The Reading Teacher, 43, 454–461. Schneider, W. (2001). Giftedness, expertise, and (exceptional) performance: A developmental perspective. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Mönks, R. J. Sternberg, & R. F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Research and Development of Giftedness and Talent (pp. 165-178). London: Elservier Science. Seibert, P. S., & Ellis, H. C. (1991). Depression and implicit memory: A commentary. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 587–591. Share, D. L., & Leikin, M. (2004). Language impairment at school entry and later reading disability: Connections at lexical versus supralexical levels of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(1), 87–110. Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary in learned from context. In M. G. McKeown, & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89–105). Hillsdale, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum. Symons, S., Richards, C., & Greene, C. (1995). Cognitive strategies for reading comprehension. In E. Wood, V. E. Woloshyn, & T. Willoughby Models of Reading Comprehension for Primary School Students 331 (Eds.), Cognitive strategy instruction for middle and high schools (pp. 66-87). Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books. Štraus, M., Repež, M., & Štigl, S. (Ed.). (2007). Nacionalno poročilo PISA 2006: naravoslovni, matematični in bralni dosežki slovenskih učencev [National PISA report 2006: science, mathematical, and reading achievements of Slovenian students]. Ljubljana: Nacionalni center PISA, Pedagoški inštitut. Thiede, K. W. & Anderson; M. C. M. (2003). Summarizing can improve metacomprehension accuracy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28(2), 129–160. Thorndike, R. L. (1973). Reading as reasoning. Reading Research Quarterly, 2(2), 135–147. Van den Broek, P., Tzeng, Y., Risden, K., Trabasso, T. & Basche, P. (2001). Inferential questioning: Effects on comprehension of narrative texts as a function of grade and timing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 521–529. Walczyk, J. J., & Taylor, R. W. (1996). How do the efficiencies of reading subcomponents relate to looking back in text? Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 537–545. Wigfield, A. (1997). Reading motivation; A domain-specific approach to motivation. Educational Psychologist, 32,59-68. Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of children’s motivation for reading to the amount and breadth of their reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 420−432. Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. (1998). Studying as self-regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 277–304). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wittrock, M.C.(1990). Generative processes of comprehension. Educational Psychologist, 24, 345–376. Yussen, S.R. (1982). Children’s impressions of coherence in narratives. In B.A. Hutson (ed.). Advances in Reading/Language Research. Vol 1 (pp. 245-281). JAI: Press Inc. Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. Handbook of self-regulation. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich & M. Zeidner (Eds). Handbook of self-regulation. (pp. 13-39). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press. Summary 332 Chapter Seventeen Reading comprehension is an indicator of reading literacy and it is also significantly related to the process of learning and students’ academic outcomes. There are different cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, and emotional factors that interactively contribute to reading comprehension. Our study included 885 fifth and ninth grade primary school students. We proposed two confirmatory models of reading comprehension, one for the younger and one for the older students. Several factors were included in the models: (meta)cognitive factors (vocabulary, summarizing, and metacognitive reading knowledge); motivational factors (reading interest and reading competency); and emotional factors (feelings during reading). We confirmed both models for the younger and the older students. The models showed direct effects of (meta)cognitive factors and indirect effects of motivational-emotional factors on students' reading comprehension. Important implications for educational practice are discussed.