The role of teacher support for the gender gap in students’ behavioral engagement Abstract This study examines gender differences in students’ behavioral engagement from a student, teacher and observer perspective and the role of student perceptions of teacher support. Participants are 385 students (7th grade) and their teachers. Results indicated that boys’ behavioral engagement is lower than girls’ is, from student, teacher and observer perspective. Boys’ perception of teacher support is also lower. Furthermore, teacher support (autonomy, structure and involvement) can partially explain the gender gap in engagement. Finally, it is shown that teacher support can be a protective factor for boys’ engagement (as measured from student perspective). These findings add to the explanation of the gender gap in student engagement and open perspectives for further investigating boys’ lower perception of all teacher support dimensions. 1 Introduction Boys are generally less successful in terms of school achievement than girls are and they report higher dropout rates as well (Lam et al., 2012, Lamote et al., 2013). Furthermore, student engagement acts as a mediator between teacher support and school achievement (Dotterer & Lowe, 2011; Roorda, Spilt, Koomen & Jak, 2011). This paper highlights the connection between teacher support and student engagement and gender differences in this matter. Student engagement is a multidimensional construct which consists of a cognitive dimension (selfregulated learning & goal orientation), behavioral dimension (conduct, participation and initiative) and emotional dimension (interest, identification with school) (Fredricks, Blumenfeld & Paris, 2004). Especially for behavioral engagement, gender differences are favoring girls (Martin, 2007; Skinner, Kinderman & Furrer, 2009; Wang & Eccles, 2012). For investigating teacher support, self-determination theory (SDT) suggests autonomy support, structure and involvement as important instructional styles. These are often measured by studentreport questionnaires. Some studies find no gender differences in the perception of teacher support (DeWit, Karioja & Rye, 2010; Sierens et al., 2009). Other researchers report that girls perceive higher autonomy support and structure (Vansteenkiste et al., 2009) and higher affective support (Oelsner, Lippold, & Greenberg, 2011), while boys experience higher control (Soenens et al. 2012). On the one hand, no gender differences have been found in the relationship between teacher support and student engagement (Assor et al., 2005; Hafen et al., 2012; Lam et al., 2012). On the other hand, each of the three instructional styles seem to have a larger effect on the engagement of boys than on the engagement of girls and could even protect boys from the risk of being disengaged (Marks, 2000; Roorda et al., 2011; Katz et al., 2006; Geist & King, 2008; Suldo et al., 2009; Van de gaer et al., 2008). Furthermore, many researchers point to the benefits of using multiple perspectives to measure engagement (e.g.: student self-report, teacher report, interviews, observations) in order to counter shared method variance and to capture the complexity of behaviors in specific contexts. 1 1.1 Aims Two main research goals are addressed in this study. Firstly, we will investigate whether gender differences in the perception of teacher support can explain the gender gap in student engagement. Secondly, we will examine whether teacher support could reduce this possible gender gap in student engagement. 2 2.1 Method Participants Participants were selected from 6 schools in Flanders, Belgium. At these schools, from September to November 2012, students (N = 385; 58 % boys, 42 % girls) completed questionnaires under the supervision of the project researchers. At 3 of these 6 schools, the engagement of 10 randomly selected students per class (N = 156; 62 % boys, 38 % girls) was observed during 6 Dutch language classes, with a total of 12 observations per student. Dutch language teachers also rated the engagement of the observed students. At the other 3 schools, Dutch language teachers rated the engagement of 10 randomly selected students per class. 2.2 Measures (1) Students reported their gender (i.e. sex) at the beginning of the questionnaires. (2) Student perception of teacher support for Dutch language was assessed by means of the short version of the Teacher As Social Context Questionnaire (TASC-Q, Belmont, Skinner, Wellborn, & Connell, 1988). (3) Teacher perception of students’ behavioral engagement was measured by means of the subscale Cooperative Participation (7 items; α = .88) of the Teacher Rating Scale of School Adjustment (TRSSA; Birch & Ladd, 1997). (4) For measuring student report of behavioral engagement, students also filled out the subscale Cooperative Participation (7 items; α = .88) of the Teacher Rating Scale of School Adjustment (TRSSA; Birch & Ladd, 1997, Valiente et al., 2012). (5) The Leuven Involvement Scale (Laevers, 1994) was used to measure students’ activity-specific engagement during Dutch language classes by means of 2-minute intervals. The intraclass correlation coefficient between 4 observers who rated 15 students was excellent (ρICC=.91). 2.3 Data analyses To address the aims of this study, the following analyses were conducted. To begin with, descriptive statistics and correlations between the variables were calculated and t-tests for detecting gender differences for all the variables were performed. Furthermore, a mediation analysis was conducted for examining whether gender differences in the perception of teacher support can explain the gender gap in student behavioral engagement. Finally, the possible protective role of teacher support for boys’ behavioral engagement was investigated by means of moderation analysis. 2 3 Results Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and correlations of all variables measuring student engagement and teacher support. Girls reported higher engagement and higher perceptions of teacher support. Next, boys’ lower perception of teacher support seemed to explain their lower behavioral engagement, with partial mediation found for teacher, observer and student perspective on behavioral engagement, except for involvement, which did not mediate the relationship between sex and observer report of engagement. This could mean that the gender gap in student engagement can be partially explained by gender differences in the perception of teacher support. Interesting for future research is to investigate this lower perception of boys and higher perception of girls concerning teacher support. Do teachers interact differently with boys than with girls and do gender stereotypes in education actually exist? Furthermore, teacher support can be a protective factor for boys’ behavioral engagement. However, these results were only found when student perspective of behavioral engagement was considered to be a dependent variable. Students perceive that teacher support can make a difference for their engagement, but teachers and external observers do not seem to notice that. These findings indicate that it is important to consider different perspectives on engagement. Furthermore, it is possible that because teacher support is only measured by means of student perception, a stronger connection is found between this teacher support and student perception of student engagement. Finally, it is noteworthy that the main relationship between teacher support and behavioral engagement is quite strong for both genders and that improving teacher support can enhance engagement for both boys and girls. 3 References Assor, A., Kaplan, H.,Kanat-Maymon, Y.,& Roth, G.(2005). Directly controlling teacher behaviors as predictors of poor motivation and engagement in girls and boys: The roles of anger and anxiety. Learning and Instruction, 15, 397–413. Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, but relevance is excellent: Autonomyenhancing and suppressing teacher behaviours in predicting student’s engagement in school work. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 261–278. Belmont, M., Skinner, E., Wellborn, J., & Connell, J. (1988). Teacher as social context: A measure of student perceptions of teacher provision of involvement, structure, and autonomy support (Tech. Rep. No. 102). University of Rochester, Rochester, NY. Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher–child relationship and children's early school adjustment. Journal of School Psychology, 35, 61–79. Dotterer AM, Lowe K. (2011) Classroom context, school engagement, and academic achievement in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(12):1649–1660. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Review of educational research, 74, 59-109. Fredricks, J. A. & McColskey, W. (2012). The Measurement of Student Engagement: A Comparative Analysis of Various Methods and Student Self-report Instruments. In S.L.Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 763-782). New York: Springer. Freudenthaler, H., Spinath, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2008). Predicting school achievement in boys and girls. European journal of personality, 22, 231-245. Geist, E.A., King M. (2008). Different, Not Better: Gender Differences in Mathematics Learning and Achievement. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 35 (1), 43-52. Hafen, C. A., Allen, J. P., Mikami, A. Y., Gregory, A., Hamre, B. & Pianta, R. C. (2012). The pivotal role of adolescent autonomy in secondary school classrooms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(3), 245-255. Katz, I., Assor, A., Kanat-Maymon, Y., & Bereby-Meyer, Y. (2006). Interest as a motivational resource: Feedback and gender matter, but interest makes the difference. Social Psychology of Education, 9, 27-42. Laevers, F. (Ed.) (1994). Defining and assessing quality in early childhood education. Studia Paedagogica. Leuven, Leuven University Press. Laevers, F., & Laurijssen, J. (2001). Well-being, involvement, and satisfaction of pupils in nursery and elementary school: A guide to systematic observation and survey. Internal report, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Center for Nursery and Elementary Education. Lam, S.-F, Jimerson, S., Kikas, E., Cefai, C., Veiga, F. H., Nelson, B., Hatzichristou, C., Polychroni, F., Basnett, J., Duck, R., Farrell, P., Liu, Y., Negovan, V., Shin, H., Stanculescu, E., Wong, B.P.H., Yang, H., Zollneritsch, J. (2012). Do girls and boys perceive themselves as equally engaged in school? The results of an international study from 12 countries. Journal of School Psychology, 50, 77-94. 4 Lamote, C., Speybroeck, S., Van Den Noortgate, W., Van Damme, J. (2013). Different pathways towards dropout: the role of engagement in early school leaving. Oxford Review of Education, 39 (6) Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high school years. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 153–184. Marsh, H. W., Martin, A. J., & Cheng, J. H. (2008). A multilevel perspective on gender in classroom motivation and climate: Potential benefits of male teachers for boys? Journal of educational psychology, 100, 78-95. Martin, A. (2007). Examining a multidimensional model of student motivation and engagement using a construct validation approach. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 413-440. Meece, J. L., Glienke, B. B., & Burg, S. (2006). Gender and motivation. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 351–373. Reeve, J. (2002). Self-determination theory applied to educational settings. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination (pp. 183–203). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Roorda, D.L, Koomen, H.M.Y., Spilt, J.L. & Oort, F.J. (2011). The influence of affective teacher–student relationships on students’ school engagement and achievement: A meta-analytic approach. Review of Educational Research, 81(4), 493–529. Schmidt, J. A., Shernoff , D. J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Individual and situational factors related to the experience of flow in adolescence: A multilevel approach. In A. D. Ong & M. v. Dulmen (Eds.), The handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 542–558). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Sierens, E., Vansteenkiste, M., Goossens, L., Soenens, B., & Dochy, F. (2009). The interactive effect of perceived teacher autonomy-support and structure in the prediction of self-regulated learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 57-68. Skinner, E., Kinderman, T., & Furrer, C. (2009). A motivational perspective on engagement and disaffection: Conceptualization and assessment of children’s behavioral and emotional participation in academic activities in the classroom. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69 , 493–525. Soenens, B., Sierens, E., Vansteenkiste, M., Goossens, L., & Dochy, F. (2012). Psychologically controlling teaching: Examining self-regulated learning and achievement outcomes and antecedents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 108-120. Suldo, S. M., Friedrich, A. A., White, T., Farmer, J., Minch, D., & Michalowski, J. (2009). Teacher support and adolescents' subjective well being: A mixed methods investigation. School Psychology Review, 38, 67−85. Valiente, C., Swanson, J., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. S. (2012). Kindergartners’ temperament, classroom engagement, and student–teacher relationship: Moderation by effortful control. Social Development, 21 (3), 558-576. Van de gaer, E., Pustjens, H., Van Damme, J. & De Munter, A. (2008). Mathematics participation and mathematics achievement across secondary school: the role of gender. Sex Roles, 59, 568-585 5 Van Heddegem, I., Gadeyne, E., Vandenberghe, N., Laevers, F., & Van Damme, J. (2004). (LOA-rapport nr. 20) [Longitudinal research in elementary school: Observational measure school year 2002–2003 (LOA report no. 20)]. Leuven, Belgium: Steunpunt ‘Loopbanen doorheen Onderwijs naar Arbeidsmarkt’, Cel ‘Schoolloopbanen in het basisonderwijs’ (SiBO). Williams, M., Burden, R., & Lanvers, U. (2002). 'French is the language of love and stuff': student perceptions of issues related to motivation in learning a foreign language. British educational research journal, 28, 503-528. 6 Table 1 t-tests and correlations for the variables of teacher support and behavioral engagement 1. 2. 3. 4. Girls N = 163 N= 161 N= 162 N = 160 Autonomy Structure Involvement Engagement (student report) 5. Engagement N= 93 (teacher report) 6. Engagement N = 59 (observer report) Note. **p < .01, *p < .05 Boys N= 227 N= 227 N= 227 N = 225 t -5.02** -5.77** -5.24** -4.99** N= 146 -3.13** N = 97 -2.59* 1 - 2 .71** - 3 .58** .59** - 4 .54** .49** .49** - 5 .28** .27** .22** .39** 6 .30** .32** .16 .34** - .33** - Table 2. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses for moderation effects of the teacher support dimensions and gender on the perceptions of behavioral engagement. Predictors Step 1 Autonomy Sex Step 2 Autonomy x sex Step 1 Structure Sex Step 2 Structure x sex Step 1 Involvement Sex Step 2 Involvement x sex Student report Δ R² B .31** .29** .10** .02** .-16** .26** .25** .10* .01* -.12* .26** .23** .11** .01* -.10* Behavioral engagement Teacher report Δ R² B .10** .15** .12* .00 -.02 .09** .14** .16* .00 .03 .07** .10** .14* .00 -.05 Observer report Δ R² B .12** .20* .18* .00 -.08 .12** 20** .16 .00 -.08 .06* .09 .20* .00 .06 7 Figure 1. Mediation effects of all teacher support dimensions on the relation between student sex and behavioral engagement (student report, teacher report, observer report). **p<.01. Autonomy Structure .29** .37** Sex (.11**) .10** .25** .44** Engagement (student report) Sex (.11**) .10** Engagement (student report) Involvement .23** .43** Sex (.10**) .11** Engagement (student report) Autonomy Structure .15** .37** Sex (.06**) .13* .14** .44** Engagement (teacher report) Sex (.06**) .12* Engagement (teacher report) Involvement .10* .43** Sex (.04*) .14* Engagement (teacher report) Autonomy Structure .20** .37** Sex (.07**) .18* .20** .44** Engagement (observer report) Sex (.09**) .16* Engagement (observer report) Involvement .08 .43** Sex (.04) .19** .19 Engagement (observer report) 8