Educational Psychology|Part 1: Human Development|Chapter 3: Personal, Gender, Social, and Moral Development Selections from Chapter 3 (pp. 99-106) 1. Moral Development: Acquiring a sense of Right and Wrong A. Piaget's Theory of Moral Development 1. Morality is Following the Rules a. 1st phase of moral development called heteronymous morality, or morality that is subject to rules imposed by others. b. Until approximately age 7, children generally believe that rules are inflexible mandates provided by some higher authority. Breaking a rule, they think, will automatically lead to punishment. c. Young children tend to make moral judgments on the basis of how much harm their actions will cause d. Moral Realism - Piaget's first developmental phase of morality, at which children see rules as absolute. 2. Morality is a Social Agreement a. After approximately age 8, children are able to understand that rules and laws are not absolute, but rather are formed by the agreement of groups of people. b. Morality of Cooperation - Piaget's second developmental phase of morality, at which children understand that people both make up rules and can change them. c. Piaget referred to this second phase of moral development as autonomous morality, the level at which children understand that people make up the rules and can change the rules. B. Kohlberg's Theory of Moral Development: Levels and Stages 1. Moral Dilemmas - Ambiguous situations in which no one decision is clearly, morally right. Level 1: Preconvention Morality Level 2: Conventional Morality Level 3: Post conventional Morality Stage 1: Punishment and obedience are an individual's main concerns. Rules are obeyed because of the threat of punishment for infractions. Stage 3: Moral reasoning is guided by mutual interpersonal expectations and conformity. People try to do what is expected of them. Stage 5: People recognize and try to balance the importance of both social contracts and individual rights Stage 2: Individual adopts an orientation of individualism and exchange. Rules are followed if they are in the individual's best interest. Deals and compromises with others are sometimes used to solve problems Stage 4: Individuals place importance on the social system, including laws, and on fulfilling obligations. Stage 6: Individuals adopt an orientation toward universal principles of justice, which exist regardless of a particular society's rules. B. Level 1 1. Preconvention Morality - First level of moral development, as proposed by Kohlberg. Individual behavior and moral decisions are based primarily on egocentric concerns--expectations of reward or punishment. 2. Children between about ages 7 to 10. Educational Psychology|Part 1: Human Development|Chapter 3: Personal, Gender, Social, and Moral Development C. Level 2 1. Conventional Morality - Second level of moral development, as proposed by Kohlberg. Individual behavior and moral decisions are guided by interpersonal expectations and conformity to internalized social rules. 2. Ages 10 to 16 D. Level 3 1. Postconventional Morality - Third level of moral development, as proposed by Kohlberg. Individual behavior and moral decisions are based on an internal set of moral absolutes, which may or may not agree with social rules. 2. Generally not achieved before the age of 16 C. Gilligan's Alternative to Kohlberg's Theory 1. Women tend to have a different conception of morality than do men. 2. Whereas men tend to focus on abstract, rational principles such as justice and respect for the rights of others, women tend to view morality more in terms of caring and compassion 3. Empathy - Ability to feel how another person feels. D. Evaluating Kohlberg's Theory 1. Psychologists have found that moral development in many situations seems to proceed roughly along the lines of Kohlberg suggested, even in other cultures, such as Turkey and Israel 2. Criticisms: a. The scoring of the scenarios is somewhat subjective, and can lead to errors of interpretation b. Stages of moral development seem to be less domain general than Kohlberg's theory suggests. c. Kohlberg's own finding that people can regress in their behavior points out the weak link that often exists between thought and action. d. Although some investigators have found cross-cultural support for Kohlberg's theory, others have found that in certain circumstances what is viewed as a "higher" level of morality differs form the value system suggested by Kohlberg. 2. Implications for Teaching A. Expect a level of moral thought and behavior that is appropriate to the child's age 1. Expert teachers know they can fail to challenge students at a level appropriate to their development if they set their expectations either too high or too low B. Help challenge students' moral reasoning by holding classroom discussions of moral dilemmas 1. These explorations of moral issues may arise from the academic subject matter or from inside or outside the classroom. Kohlberg & Kramer believed discussion of moral dilemmas could help a child develop expertise in moral reasoning by exposing the child to thinking at higher stages C. Use self-assessment to help assess your own level of moral development so that you can better understand how you perceive the thinking and behavior of your students. 1. Teachers need to understand themselves and their own thinking D. Realize that no one theory of moral development is universally excepted 1. All of the theories appear to apply only to some-rather than to all-individuals E. Encourage and develop thinking that is not just moral, but wise Educational Psychology|Part 1: Human Development|Chapter 3: Personal, Gender, Social, and Moral Development 1. Such thinking has as its aim the attainment of a common good-what is ultimately in everyone's best interests F. How teachers Might Apply The Implications at Different Educational Levels Implications Preschool and Primary Grades Elementary School Middle School Expect a level of moral thought and behavior that is appropriate to the child's age Young children often consider consequences more important than intentions in judging others. Teachers might use everyday examples, such as spills or playground accidents, to help point out to students that causing some damage on purpose is different than doing so unintentionally Students in the upper elementary grades and in middle school are often in the second of Kohlberg's moral levels, using conventional moral reasoning. They may enjoy setting some of the rules of the classroom "society" in class meetings High school teachers can challenge students to consider whether there are any universal principles. Subjects such as literature and social science make an easy context for moral discussion, but even science classes, for example, offer students opportunities to ponder the existence of global rules Discussions of moral dilemmas might help children develop their moral reasoning skills Younger children's decisions are generally based on egocentric concerns about their own punishment or rewards. Teachers should challenge young students to sometimes see how individual actions affect other people. Stories or real-life examples from the classroom could be used to provide opportunities for Class meetings provide opportunities to bring up moral dilemmas students are actually facing and encourage discussion of possible choices Current events in many subjects, such as scientific advances leading to cloning or discoveries about the "true" nature of historical figures or events, can provide the basis for discussions of values and ethics in many different classes In some school districts, it may be acceptable to encourage students to discuss issues that are being brought to their minds by the changes of puberty, such as personal sexual standards. Be sure to check the district's guidelines before initiating any discussions of High School Educational Psychology|Part 1: Human Development|Chapter 3: Personal, Gender, Social, and Moral Development taking another's perspective Teachers should be aware of cultural and gender influences on morality Children whose cultural backgrounds value interdependence may be uncomfortable away from their families for the first time. Teachers may be able to help by encouraging family members to visit, or even volunteer regularly, at school and by initiating frequent contact with families, designed to promote cooperation in helping the child adjust to school sensitive topics Cooperative learning tasks, such as team science or social science projects, encourage students who are oriented to independent achievement to also learn to value working for the sake of the group. Some classes may wish to participate in civic projects that show students how their individual knowledge could be sued to help their whole community. For example, one junior high school science class, on learning that bats eat biting insects, installed bat houses in a local park to make the park more comfortable for the whole community. In some schools, competition for grades may become fierce as students strive to impress college acceptance committees. Teachers may wish to point out the value of study groups for cooperatively boosting the achievements of all members.