Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 Chapter 1 Natural Breeding Conditions and Artificial Increases in Testosterone Have Opposite Effects on the Brains of Adult Male Songbirds: A Meta-analysis T Smulders This document has been modified for the purposes of IT skills courses run by Rebecca McCready and Sue Vecsey (LTMS), Newcastle University, UK, with permission from the author. 1.1 Abstract A meta-analysis of the literature shows that in adult male songbirds: 1. brain mass, 2. telencephalon volume and 3. n. rotundus (a thalamic visual nucleus) volume increase from the nonbreeding season (low testosterone) to the breeding season (higher testosterone). These effects can at least partially be mimicked by photoperiod manipulations in captivity. In contrast, an artificial testosterone (T) titer increase by chronic implants yields the opposite results: telencephalon, n. rotundus, and n. pretectalis volumes are lower in Ttreated animals than in controls. These results suggest that artificial testosterone manipulations do not necessarily mimic the effects of natural variations in hormone levels and that results from experiments using T implants to mimic natural hormonal effects should be interpreted with caution. Key Words: song system; avian hippocampus; brain mass; androgens; i Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 corticosteroids; telencephalon; n. rotundus; n. pretectalis; n. spiriformis medialis; oscines. ii Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 Table of Contents Chapter 1 Natural Breeding Conditions and Artificial Increases in Testosterone Have Opposite Effects on the Brains of Adult Male Songbirds: A Meta-analysis ..................................... i 1.1 Abstract ................................................................................................................................................... i 1.2 Introduction ......................................................................................................................................... 1 1.3 Methods ................................................................................................................................................. 2 1.3.1 Data Analysis ............................................................................................................................. 2 1.4 Results .................................................................................................................................................... 3 1.4.1 Seasonal Changes in Wild-Caught Birds ......................................................................... 3 1.5 Discussion ............................................................................................................................................. 3 1.5.1 Seasonal Changes in Brain Mass ........................................................................................ 3 1.6 Conclusions .......................................................................................................................................... 4 1.7 References ............................................................................................................................................ 5 List of Figures List of Tables iii Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 1.2 Introduction Much research has been directed toward understanding seasonal behaviors and the associated seasonal changes in the underlying neuroanatomy in birds. This research includes such behaviors as foodhoarding, nest-searching in brood parasites, and singing. Nuclei of the song system, which control learning and production of song in oscine songbirds, often are larger during the breeding season than during the nonbreeding season [Ref 1]. This varitation is partly due to the rise in testosterone during the breeding season, but other photoperiodically regulated factors (e.g., melatonin and possibly thyroid hormone; recently reviewed by Tramontin and Brenowitz, 2000) also contribute. The hippocampal formation in birds also changes seasonally, and this seems to be related to a seasonal need for better spatial memory. In food-hoarding birds, it is larger during the hoarding season than during the rest of the year [Ref 2]. In brood-parasitic cowbirds, the hippocampal formation is larger during the breeding season, but only in the sex(es) that actively looks for host nests. In this system, the underlying mechanisms have not yet been studied in detail. When investigating changes in neuroanatomy, especially brain region volumes, it is usually necessary to also measure control areas: regions of the brain that are not expected to vary as a result of the experimental manipulation or condition. Typical control measures are the volume of the entire telencephalon, or some variant such as total brain mass or telencephalon width, as well as the volumes of small, easily delineated nontelencephalic nuclei. Sometimes significant differences have also been found in these control regions. Most often, these differences are assumed to be “nonspecific background varitation” and are controlled for statistically when investigating the differences in the areas under study. 1 Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 In this paper, I investigate the changes in the most commonly used control regions in songbird neurobiology using the quantitative review techniques of meta-analysis. One of the common problems with meta-analyses is the “publication bias:” it is difficult to find negative results in the literature because it is difficult to publish negative results. This is not a problem in the current investigation, however, since the regions under investigation were not the primary targets of the original papers. Therefore, any consistent and significant patterns across studies would be especially meaningful. The regions that are included in this analysis are whole brain mass and the volumes of the entire telencephalon (Tel), nucleus rotundus (Rt) (a thalamic visual nucleus), nucleus pretectalis (Pt) (a thalamic visual nucleus), and nucleus spiriformis medialis (SpM) (a thalamic sensorimotor nucleus) [Ref 3]. Whereas several parts of the Tel contain androgen receptors, no androgen receptors have been described in any of the thalamic nuclei under investigation in this paper (Balthazart, Foidart, Wilson, and Ball, 1992; Nastiuk and Clayton, 1995). 1.3 Methods 1.3.1 Data Analysis I used Stouffer’s method for combining hypothesis tests [Ref 4]. First, I tabulated the P values associated with the relevant comparisons and the directions of all differences, whether they were statistically significant or not. Then, all two-tailed P values were transformed to one-tailed P values in the most commonly occurring direction of the difference for that structure. These P values were then transformed to the corresponding Z scores using the inverse function of a Gaussian distribution. Stouffer’s Zc was then calculated as where zi represents the individual z value for each study included in the analysis, and N is the total number of studies included. The one-tailed P value (Pc) resulting from this 2 Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 combined Z score is then used as an indicator of the significance of the effect across all the tests of the same hypothesis. Effects are considered significant if Pc , 0.05. In addition, I calculated a common metric for the effect size (d) for each study (whether significant or not) as the difference between two groups as measured in units of standard deviations (SD). 1.4 Results 1.4.1 Seasonal Changes in Wild-Caught Birds represents all the studies from which I obtained data on seasonal changes in male songbirds. “Birds collected during the breeding season have brains that are on average 1.05 (6 0.42 (SEM)) SD heavier (i.e., brain mass is larger) than those of birds collected in the nonbreeding season (Zc 5 23.547, Pc 5 0.0002)”. “Black-capped chickadees are seasonal food hoarders, which have a larger hippocampal formation (HF) and septum in the fall”. “These structures (and possibly other associated structures) make up a large part of the brain and this would definitely influence total brain size”. . Summary of All the Studies Investigating Seasonal Changes under Natural Conditions 1.5 Discussion 1.5.1 Seasonal Changes in Brain Mass In birds too, it is likely that at least part of the increase in brain mass during the breeding season is the result of increased water content. Evidence for this comes from the fact that most studies from which we collected data used perfusion-fixed brains, which were then cut on a freezing microtome or on a cryostat. For this procedure, brains are typically 3 Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 cryoprotected by immersion in a high concentration of sucrose (typically 20–30%). Such a high osmolarity solution will, in addition to adding sucrose to the tissue (which cryoprotects it), equalize the total osmolarity inside and outside the tissue. If the tissue osmolarity is lower in the breeding season (i.e., if there is more water relative to solutes in the brain tissue), the brains should lose more water (i.e., more weight) when immersed in sucrose if collected during the breeding season than during the nonbreeding season. This is exactly what we found in two independent data sets: one from black-capped chickadees and one from dark-eyed juncos. In both cases, during the breeding season, immersion in sucrose resulted in a significant decrease in brain weight, while during the nonbreeding season, the change was much smaller (). . Change in brain mass as a consequence of cryoprotection. The percentage (6SEM) change in brain mass before and after immersion in a 30% sucrose solution for several days is plotted against the time of year when the brains were collected and processed [Web Ref]. (A) Brains from adult black-capped chickadees (male and female) from upstate New York, captured at five different times across the year. . Change in brain mass in castrated adult male dark-eyed juncos with changing photoperiodic condition. 1.6 Conclusions A quantitative review of the literature shows that there are overall changes taking place in the brains of adult male songbirds across the year and with artificial testosterone treatment. Under natural conditions, brains tend to be larger during the breeding season 4 Hormones & Behavior (2002) 41(2):156-69 than during the nonbreeding season. Artificial elevations of T levels, however, seem to decrease the volumes of these same brain areas. These patterns now require careful direct experimental testing, both to verify the patterns (using experimental designs with high statistical power) and to elucidate the possible underlying mechanisms. These results lead to three important cautionary insights for researchers in the field of behavioral neuroendocrinology. First of all, artificial hormone treatments are not necessarily a good mimic of natural hormone changes, however well we think we know the system. Second, care should be taken when interpreting the outcomes of behavioral experiments in which birds were implanted with exogenous testosterone, since the behavioral implications of changes in nontargeted brain regions are not known. Finally, these brain regions should not be used as statistical controls or “standards” when investigating the effects of testosterone on other brain nuclei, such as the song system, since they may distort the actual data. Appendix 1.7 References 5