THE RELATIONSHIP OF REPORTED PARENT DISCIPLINING PRACTICES TO PHYSICALLY ABUSIVE BEHAVIOR AMONG PARENTS OF AFRICAN, ASIAN, LATINO, AND WHITE AMERICANS Bahareh Abhari B.A., University of California, Davis, 2008 B.S., University of California, Davis, 2008 THESIS Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in PSYCHOLOGY at California State University, Sacramento SPRING 2011 THE RELATIONSHIP OF REPORTED PARENT DISCIPLINING PRACTICES TO PHYSICALLY ABUSIVE BEHAVIOR AMONG PARENTS OF AFRICAN, ASIAN, LATINO, AND WHITE AMERICANS A Thesis by Bahareh Abhari Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Phillip D. Akutsu, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader Gregory Kim-Ju, Ph.D. __________________________________, Third Reader Kelly Cotter, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date ii Student: Bahareh Abhari I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator ___________________ Jianjian Qin, Ph.D. Date Department of Psychology iii Abstract of THE RELATIONSHIP OF REPORTED PARENT DISCIPLINING PRACTICES TO PHYSICALLY ABUSIVE BEHAVIOR AMONG PARENTS OF AFRICAN, ASIAN, LATINO, AND WHITE AMERICANS by Bahareh Abhari The current study examined mediation models to explain differences in sex and ethnicity in retrospective reports of parental physically abusive behavior for Asian, African, Latino, and White American college students. In addition, relationships between parenting styles, mother and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting and parent disciplining methods, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and four corporal punishment items were explored. One corporal punishment item, shaking, partially mediated the relationship between ethnicity and reports of physically abusive behavior. _______________________, Committee Chair Phillip D. Akutsu, Ph.D. _______________________ Date iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Foremost, I would like to express my deepest gratitude towards my thesis committee chair and advisor, Dr. Phillip Akutsu, for his continued support, advice, and mentorship not only during this project, but throughout my academic career at CSU Sacramento. His encouragement and guidance have greatly assisted the completion of this thesis. I would also like to extend my thanks towards Dr. Kelly Cotter and Dr. Gregory Kim-Ju for joining my thesis committee and providing helpful feedback throughout the project. I owe a special thanks to Dr. Larry Meyers for his advice about statistical analyses for my thesis. In addition, I’d like to thank all of the past and current members of the research lab, Ho Man Cheung, Timothy Fechter, Ting Ting Lee, Jeannie Scroggins, Brittany Purdy, and Jennifer Wong for assistance with collecting data and especially their continued moral support during this project. Finally, I’d like to thank my dance team, friends, parents, siblings, and my cousins who have encouraged me to succeed in my efforts. v TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................. v List of tables ...................................................................................................................... vii Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................... 1 Negative Outcomes of Physical Abuse ....................................................................... 3 Factors Associated with Physical Abuse .................................................................... 7 Summary ................................................................................................................... 39 Present Study ............................................................................................................ 41 2. METHOD ..................................................................................................................... 46 Participants................................................................................................................ 46 Measures ................................................................................................................... 47 Procedure .................................................................................................................. 52 3. RESULTS ..................................................................................................................... 55 Demographic Information and Study Variables ....................................................... 55 4. DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................. 134 Summary and Interpretation of Findings in the Context of Previous Literature .... 134 Limitations in the Current Study ............................................................................ 147 Clinical Implications ............................................................................................... 149 Future Directions in Research................................................................................. 152 References ....................................................................................................................... 155 vi LIST OF TABLES 1. Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations of Mother and Father Authoritarian, Authoritative, and Permissive Parenting Styles…………………………….. 60 2. Table 2 Descriptive Statistics per Ethnic Group for Total Sample, Participant Sex, Family of Origin Socioeconomic Status, and Age…………………….. 61 3. Table 3 Estimated Marginal Means for Males and Females in Mother/Father Parenting Styles……………………………….…………………………….. 62 4. Table 4 Estimated Marginal Means for Ethnic Groups in Mother/Father Parenting Styles……………………………….…………………………….. 63 5. Table 5 Estimated Marginal Means for Males and Females in Nonphysical Forms of Parent Discipline…………………….………………………………........ 68 6. Table 6 Estimated Marginal Means for Ethnic Groups in Nonphysical Forms of Parent Discipline……………………………….…………………………..... 69 7. Table 7 Logistic Regression Results with Ethnicity Predicting Spanking with or without Object……………………………….……………………………..... 75 8. Table 8 Logistic Regression Results with Ethnicity Predicting Pinching and Shaking…………………………………….………………………………… 77 9. Table 9 Percentage of Each Sex and Ethnic Group Experiencing Spanking With Bare Hand, Spanking with Object, Pinching and Shaking………………….. 79 10. Table 10 Estimated Marginal Means for Males and Females in Physically Abusive Behavior……………………………….…………………………... 80 11. Table 11 Estimated Marginal Means for Ethnic Groups in Physically Abusive Behavior…………………………………….………………………………. 81 12. Table 12 Multiple Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Nonviolent Discipline…………….…………………………….. 84 13. Table 13 Multiple Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Psychological Aggression…………….……………………….... 87 vii 14. Table 14 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Spanking with Bare Hand…………….……………………….... 90 15. Table 15 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Spanking with an Object…………….………………………….. 93 16. Table 16 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Pinching……………………………….………………………… 94 17. Table 17 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Shaking……………………………….………………………..... 97 18. Table 18 Multiple Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior…………….…………………….. 100 19. Table 19 Multiple Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline Predicting Psychological Aggression……………………………….……………….... 106 20. Table 20 Logistic Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline and Psychological Aggression Predicting Spanking with or without Object….. 107 21. Table 21 Logistic Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline and Psychological Aggression Predicting Pinching and Shaking…………….... 109 22. Table 22 Multiple Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline and Psychological Aggression Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior………. 111 23. Table 23 Multiple Regression Results with Corporal Punishment Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior…………………….……………………….... 117 24. Table 24 Multiple Regression Results with Mother/Father Authoritarian Parenting and Parent Discipline Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior… 121 25. Table 25 Multiple Regression Results with Mother/Father Authoritative Parenting and Parent Discipline Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior… 129 26. Table 26 Summary of the Relationship between Mother/Father Authoritarian, Authoritative, and Permissive Parenting Styles, Parent Discipline, and Physically Abusive Behavior……………………………………….………………..... 141 viii 27. Table 27 Summary of the Relationships between Nonviolent Discipline, Psychological Aggression, Corporal Punishment, and Physically Abusive Behavior…………………………………….……………………………… 144 ix 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Physical abuse is the second most common form of maltreatment after neglect by caregivers of children in the United States. As reported by the National Child Abuse and Neglect Data System (NCANDS), the majority of these children were abused by their biological parents and in recent years, most of the abused children were reported to be first-time victims (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services [DHHS], 2007). In the Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS-4), more than half of the 835,000 children who were found to be abused by Child Protective Services (CPS) were determined to be physically abused (Sedlak et al., 2010a). The negative experience of physical abuse can have dire consequences for many children. For example, 61% of child fatalities in the NCANDS study (U.S. DHHS, 2007) had experienced some form of physical abuse. Unfortunately, only 62% of the referrals for suspected abuse made to Child Protective Service agencies were officially investigated in 2007 (U.S. DHHS, 2007). Many children may be overlooked when such investigations take place. It is also possible that many children who are being physically abused are never referred to CPS. As such, possible risk factors for physical abuse need to be investigated to assist in identifying which children may be at greater risk for physical abuse. It is suggested that physical abuse may be associated with certain demographic variables. For example, while most of the children who were physically abused are relatively young in age, physical abuse has been reported for older children. Specifically, 2 22% of the children who were physically abused in the NCANDS study (U.S. DHHS, 2007) were young teenagers. The child’s sex may also be associated with physical abuse, with boys being physically abused more than girls (Ackerman, Newton, McPherson, Jones, & Dykman, 1998; Mathurin, Gielen, & Lancaster, 2006). Another trend that is commonly discussed is the over-representation of certain ethnic groups in reported physical abuse cases (Fluke, Yuan, Hedderson, & Curtis, 2003; Hughes, 2006; Klevens & Leeb, 2010); Lee, Guterman, & Lee, 2008; Zolotor, Theodore, Chang, Berkoff, & Runyan, 2008). The NIS-4 revealed that African American children had a greater risk of experiencing physical abuse than White or Latino American children (Sedlak, McPherson, & Das, 2010a). While socioeconomic status (SES) has previously been found to be an important factor in predicting physical abuse (Fergusson, Boden, & Horwood, 2008, Sedlak, McPherson, & Das, 2010b; Springer, Sheridan, Kuo, & Carnes, 2007; Strassberg, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1994), ethnic differences in SES have diminished in recent years (Sedlak, McPherson, & Das, 2010b). Specifically, ethnic differences only existed in two income categories in the most recent NIS, the very lowest (<$15,000-24,999) and highest (>$100,000). It is possible that other factors besides SES that are closely related to ethnic group membership may be associated with physical abuse. One of these factors may be ethnic differences in parenting styles and the role these differences play in parents’ decision to use specific types of parent practices such as the use of nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment, (Garbarino, 1977; Gershoff, 2002; Gil, 1970; Kadushin & Martin, 1981; Straus, 2000. 3 These ethnic differences in parenting styles may reflect different cultural norms and beliefs that are espoused by various ethnic groups. These differences in styles could lead to the use of different parent disciplining techniques. In the following sections, a review of the mental health literature will be conducted on the physical abuse of children, parenting styles, parent disciplining practices, and the negative outcomes of such parent behaviors. This review will examine possible risk factors for physical abuse, such as parent disciplining practices, that may differ across the sexes and ethnic groups and how these relate to parents who may engage in physically abusive behaviors. There will be a discussion of past definitions of these constructs relating to physical abuse and corporal punishment to assist in comparing the results of current and past studies on parent disciplining practices and physical abuse. Finally, directions for future research in this field of study will be discussed. Negative Outcomes of Physical Abuse Previous studies have widely supported the short and long term negative outcomes of physical abuse for children in substantiated cases of physical abuse and physically abusive behaviors in general populations. The types of physically abusive behaviors that were investigated varied from study to study so it is very important to note what types of behaviors were examined in each study to determine any conclusive findings pertaining to “physical abuse.” Studies that focus on immediate or short term negative effects of physical abuse reveal a number of negative consequences for children who experience this abuse. 4 Reports of physical abuse that have been identified by human service agencies, or are defined by pushing, grabbing, or slapping by parents have been associated with increased mental health problems such as delinquent and aggressive behaviors for child victims (Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990; Feldman, Salzinger, Rosario, Alvarado, Caraballo, & Hammer, 1995; Johnsona et al., 2002; Maikovich, Jaffe, Odgers, & Gallop, 2008). Physical abuse defined as behaviors ranging from verbal threat of physical punishment to the actual use of a deadly weapon were associated with higher rates of posttraumatic stress disorder among child populations (Ackerman et al., 1998; Kiser, Heston, Millsap, & Pruitt, 1991). Child Protective Services and clinic referred children who were abused were also found to report with internalizing problems such as depression (Allen & Tarnowski, 1989; Litrownik, Newton, & Landsverk, 2005) and lower self-esteem (Allen & Tarnowski, 1989; Kazdin, Moser, Colbus, & Bell, 1985). Physical abuse in childhood seems to also have long term negative consequences for many adults who were victims of child abuse. For adults who were abused as children, physical abuse was defined as having marks on one’s body, having an object thrown at them, being pushed, grabbed, slapped, or kicked, or reporting a high frequency of physical punishment by their parents. The negative outcomes or consequences of physical abuse as children for adult victims included increased mental health problems such as posttraumatic stress disorder (Barbo, 2004), depression and anxiety (Edwards, Holden, Felitti, & Anda, 2003; Fletcher, 2009; Wainwright & Surtees, 2002), substance dependence, conduct/anti-social personality disorder, suicidal ideation and attempts 5 (Fergusson et al., 2008), and self-injurious behaviors (Yates, 2006). For physical abuse defined as being slapped, shoved or having things thrown at them, adult survivors of such child abuse were also found to report with increased physical health problems such as high blood pressure, cancer, ulcers, and high cholesterol (Springer et al., 2007). Representation of Sex in Physical Abuse Cases The literature on physical abuse and sex differences seems to suggest there may be a difference in the rates of physical abuse among boys and girls. While some studies have found that boys and girls are equally represented in physical abuse cases (Mersky, Berger, Reynolds, & Gromoske, 2009; Sedlak et al., 2010a; Springer et al., 2007), less information is known about situations where children have experienced some physically abusive behavior by their parents, that was not reported to child protective agencies. It is suggested that in studies examining physically abusive behavior in general samples, males may report experiencing higher levels in childhood than females (Thompson, Kingree, & Desai, 2004). It is possible that certain parent disciplining practices may increase the likelihood of physical child abuse. Since the child’s sex may be associated with these disciplining techniques, this makes the child’s sex an important factor to include in future studies. Representation of Ethnicity in Physical Abuse Cases One finding that has been detected in child abuse cases is the over-representation of ethnic minorities in physical abuse cases reported to child protective agencies. The NIS-4 study in 2010 found for the first time that African American children experienced 6 higher rates of physical abuse than White American children. However, studies conducted before the NIS-4 found similar rates of physical abuse among ethnic groups (Ackerman, et al., 1998; Fluke et. al., 2003; Hughes, 2006; Klevens & Leeb, 2010; U.S. DHHS, 2007). The reasons for these inconsistent findings are unknown at this time. It is possible that other factors that are highly correlated with physical abuse, such as low socioeconomic status, may be overrepresented in ethnic minority groups. It could also be that ethnic minority children are referred or investigated more frequently by CPS agencies. Over-representation of ethnic minorities has also been detected by researchers that study physical abuse in the general population (Afifi, Brownridge, Cox, & Sareen, 2006; Kennedy, 2003; Lee et al., 2008, Turner & Muller, 2004). For example, one study found that ethnic minority status which was defined as being Asian, African, Latino, and Native American, predicted a higher level of physical abuse (Zolotor et al., 2008). However, it is important to recognize some of the limitations of these studies. For example, sample sizes of ethnic minorities in these studies tend to be small so they are often grouped together to represent a single aggregate group of “ethnic minority” or “non-White group.” It may be useful to keep these ethnic groups separate when studying physical abuse to better understand whether ethnic differences do exist in reported physical abuse and if specific behaviors of parents, such as disciplining practices, may be contributing to physically abusive behaviors. 7 Factors Associated with Physical Abuse Detrimental outcomes for both children and adult children who are victims of physical abuse have led researchers to seek possible predictors of physical abuse to perhaps reduce the occurrences of physical abuse among children in the future. One factor, parents’ use of corporal punishment, has been associated with child physical abuse and has been frequently researched and debated in past literature. Unfortunately, less attention has been given to how parents’ use of corporal punishment relates to the use of nonphysical parent disciplining practices and how these alternative practices relate to parents’ use of physically abusive behaviors. Comparative investigation of parent discipline and how it may vary across different ethnic groups may help clarify the relationship between ethnicity, parenting styles, parent discipline, and physical abuse. Parenting Styles Before a discussion of parenting disciplining practices can be initiated, some contextual factors must be taken into consideration. A limitation of previous research is the study of parenting disciplining practices without consideration of how parents may come to the decision of using certain disciplining practices. One way of considering these contextual factors for parental discipline, is to examine general parenting styles. Three types of parenting styles based on the dimensions of parent warmth and demandingness (Baumrind, 1984) have commonly been examined among child and adolescent outcome studies (Buschgens, van Aken, Swinkels, Ormel, Verhulst, & 8 Buitelaar, 2010; Fletcher, Walls, Cook, Madison, & Bridges, 2008; Park & Bauer, 2002; Simons & Conger, 2007). These parenting styles include authoritarian style (low warmth, high demandingness), authoritative style (high warmth, high demandingness) and permissive style (high warmth, low demandingness). A fourth parenting style was developed by Maccoby & Martin (1983) which includes uninvolved/neglectful parenting (low warmth, low demandingness). However, one limitation of the measurement of this parenting style is that parents may be less likely to report that they are low on both of these parenting dimensions. In addition, the uninvolved/neglectful parenting style is not commonly researched. Examining these general parenting styles may give some insights into how parents may choose particular disciplining practices to support the parenting style that is preferred in the household. In addition, information about parenting styles may facilitate a better understanding of possible ethnic differences in parent discipline. Parenting styles and child sex. Studies have shown that parents may use different parenting styles depending on their child’s sex. However, the results for studies in the field have been somewhat mixed. For example, in a study of 10-12 year olds, girls were found to experience higher levels of one dimension of parenting, warmth, which may be associated with authoritative or permissive parenting styles (Buschgens et al., 2010). A study of undergraduate college students supports this finding, with women reporting more permissive parenting than men, in childhood (Dominguez & Carton, 1997). However, other studies suggest boys may be the recipients of greater parental warmth. For example in a sample of adolescent children, boys reported a lower level of 9 authoritarian parenting than girls, suggesting higher parental warmth for boys (Dornbusch, Ritter, Leiderman, Roberts & Fraleigh, 1987). Similarly, among college students, men reported higher level of permissive parenting as children than women, (McKinney & Renk, 2008; Klein et. al., 1996). In contrast, others have reported that adolescent boys and young men experience higher levels of both authoritarian and authoritative parenting than adolescent girls or young women suggesting higher levels of parental demandingness for boys (Furnham & Cheng, 2000; Flett et al., 1995; Klein et al., 1996). Finally, some researchers have suggested that parents use similar parenting styles with boys and girls (Simons & Conger, 2007). As such, the findings in the preceding studies have not been clear about how parents may use different parenting styles with their sons and daughters. However, these results suggest there may be some sex differences in the mother’s and father’s parenting styles concerning the child’s gender and this may be an important factor to consider when examining corporal punishment and physical abuse by parents. Parenting styles and ethnic group membership. Past studies have found that ethnic groups may differ in the types of parenting styles that are practiced in the home. For example, Asian and African American parents were found to report with higher levels of authoritarian parenting than White or Latino Americans (Fite, Stoppelbein, & Greening, 2009; Jackson-Newsom, Buchanan, & McDonald, 2008; Radziszewska, Richardson, Dent, & Flay, 1996; Steinberg, Mounts, Lamborn, & Dornbusch, 1991). Other studies also found that ethnic minority parents in general report a more 10 authoritarian style of parenting than White Americans (Dearing, 2004; Kaufman, Geston, Santa Lucia, Salcedo, Rendina-Gobioff, & Gadd, 2000; Steinberg et al., 1991). While some studies found that ethnic minority parents were less likely to support authoritative parenting styles than White American parents (Radziszewska et al., 1996; Steinberg et al., 1991), other studies found that authoritative parenting was the preferred method of parenting reported by ethnic minority parents (Bluestone, Tamis-LeMonda, 1999; Bradley, 1998). In contrast, other reports found that ethnic minority parents and White American parents were similar in their support of authoritative parenting styles (Kaufman et al., 2000; Mupinga, Garrison, & Pierce, 2002; Steinberg, Blatt-Eisengart, & Cauffman, 2006). Less information is known about possible ethnic differences in permissive parenting. However, since the dimensions used to measure parenting styles, warmth and demandingness, overlap across these three parenting styles (Baumrind, 1984), it may be possible that ethnic differences in this type of parenting style may exist as well. Parenting styles, discipline, and physical abuse. Researchers suggest that parents who engage in a particular style of parenting may also make greater use of certain types of disciplining techniques to support their preferred method of parenting. For example, authoritarian parents were less likely to endorse the use of corporal punishment to discipline a child than permissive parents, but they were more likely to support the use of corporal punishment than authoritative parents (Paquette, Bolte, Turcotte, Dubeau, & Bouchard, 2000). Some researchers have examined the use of parent corporal punishment and psychological aggression in their definitions of authoritarian style 11 parenting (Frias-Armenta & McCloskey, 1998; Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 2001). These findings are consistent with the idea that authoritarian parents often demand strict compliance from their children, and such compliance is often achieved through corporal punishment (Bean & Roberts, 1981; Day & Roberts, 1983; Gershoff, 2002; Larzelere, & Kuhn, 2005). Since there is intolerance for misbehavior among these parents, some researchers suggest this rigid type of thinking may lead to physical abuse as well. For example, abusive and non-abusive parents in a meta-analysis of 33 studies were found to significantly differ in their levels of authoritarian and authoritative parenting (Wilson, Rack, Shi, & Norris, 2008). Specifically, abusive parents showed more authoritarian parenting characteristics, while non-abusive parents showed more authoritative parenting characteristics. A similar finding was reported in another sample of abusive parents of adolescents (Pelcovitz, 1984). There is less information about the specific types of discipline used by authoritative parents. Given that authoritative parents are higher in parental demandingness and warmth, they may be less likely to use coercive disciplining methods such as psychological aggression and corporal punishment and more likely to use nonviolent disciplining methods such as time-out. However, there is little research which has actually examined this relationship between authoritative parenting style and disciplining practices. In contrast, permissive parents are characterized by high parental warmth, and low demandingness and such parents may be less likely to use all forms of discipline. However, there is a suggestion that permissive parents may be prone to 12 occasional impulsive attempts at controlling child behavior, which may be abusive in nature (Pelcovitz, 1984). In the psychology literature, parenting styles have been defined using two dimensions, parental warmth and demandingness. As discussed previously, these two parental dimensions may be related to the decision to use some disciplining practices over others. Additionally, some studies have suggested that some parenting styles, such as authoritarian or permissive parenting, may lead to attitudes that make it more likely for parents to use physically abusive behaviors. As such, it may be important to include parenting styles as a contextual factor in a study of parent discipline to facilitate a better understanding of how parents choose from different disciplining techniques, particularly among different ethnic groups. Information about parenting styles may also help explain possible relationships between this factor and physically abusive behavior, particularly among authoritarian and permissive parents. Parent Disciplining Practices Parents have a wide variety of disciplining techniques available to them when deciding to discipline their children, which may be influenced by their general parenting style. These techniques can include nonviolent forms of discipline such as verbal reasoning, revoking of privileges, time-outs, and psychologically aggressive forms of discipline such as yelling or threatening, and corporal punishment such as spanking with a bare hand, spanking with an object, slapping, and pinching. It is suggested that lack of success with the more positive or socially accepted forms of disciplining practices may 13 lead to progressively more intrusive disciplining practices such as psychological aggression and corporal punishment (Gil, 1970). Finally, parents may engage in physically abusive behaviors as an attempt to control child behavior. Many studies have attempted to link parent corporal punishment use with physical abuse or have suggested that physical abuse is on the extreme end of a continuum of all forms of physical punishment (Gershoff, 2002; Gil, 1970; Giles-Sims, Straus, & Sugarman, 1995; Jaffe, Caspi, Moffitt, & Taylor, 2004; Kadushin & Martin, 1981; Straus, 2000; Zolotor et al., 2008), but these studies have failed to take into consideration that parents may use alternative disciplining practices. It may be possible that access or use of nonphysical disciplining practices may reduce the use of corporal punishment or the likelihood of physically abusive behaviors. Previous studies have also failed to determine if culture or ethnicity plays a critical role in determining what types of parent disciplining practices are commonly accepted among parents of different ethnic groups. This information would be useful in order to clarify the relationship between the disciplining practices that parents decide to use and their association with physical abuse. It is possible that overrepresentation of ethnic minorities in physical abuse cases may be better explained by ethnic group differences in parent disciplining practices. Nonphysical forms of discipline. Nonphysical forms of discipline can include both strategies that are commonly taught in parent training programs such as the use of rewards, reasoning, and time-out and psychologically aggressive disciplining practices such as yelling, the use of threats, and name-calling to control child behavior. It is 14 possible that success with the use of nonphysical forms of discipline may reduce the occurrence of more physical disciplining practices or physically abusive behaviors. Nonviolent discipline. Nonviolent discipline is commonly described in past literature as parent behaviors such as verbal reasoning/explaining, sending a child to his/her room, the use of time-out, grounding, and removing privileges (Amuwo, Fabian, Tolley, Spence, & Hill, 2004; Barkin, Scheindlin, Ip, Richardson, & Finch, 2007; Caughy & Franzini, 2005; Jackson et al., 1999; Trickett & Kuczynski, 1986). Outcomes of this type of parent discipline are not well studied. Many effective parent training programs tend to teach the use of nonviolent disciplining techniques in controlling child behavior (Boyle, Sanders, Lutzker, Prinz, Shapiro, & Whitaker, 2010; Smagner & Sullivan, 2005; Webster-Stratton, & Hancock, 1998). These programs have been found to be effective in promoting more positive or socially accepted parenting practices. For example, the uses of these positive parenting techniques in Parent-Child Interaction Therapy were found to reduce physically abusive parenting in White, African, Latino, and Native Americans (Hakman, Chaffin, Funderburk, & Silovsky, 2009). It is unclear, however, if using nonviolent disciplining practices less is related to increased use of other psychologically aggressive or physically violent disciplining practices. Some evidence suggests the use of nonviolent discipline is positively associated with using psychological aggression (Wissow, 2001). Studies that have examined the relationship between the use of nonviolent discipline and more physical forms of punishment have provided mixed findings (Gest, Freeman, Domitrovich, & Welsh, 2004; Jackson et al., 1999; Thompson 15 et al., 2009; Wissow, 2001). Some of these studies have shown the use of nonviolent discipline is positively associated with using corporal punishment, while other studies have shown the opposite or a negative relationship between these two parenting behaviors. It is also uncertain whether the use of less nonviolent disciplining practices is directly related to a greater risk of physically abusive parenting. One study showed that verbal reasoning was used less by physically abusive parents compared to nonabusive controls (Trickett & Kuczynski, 1986). However, some evidence suggests that a history of physical abuse by parents is not significantly related to parent use of nonviolent discipline (Jackson et al., 1999). Further research is needed to clarify the role of nonviolent discipline for parents who are found to be physically abusive. Nonviolent discipline and child sex. Previous studies have shown that parents may use nonviolent discipline at different rates for boys and girls. Specifically, some evidence suggests a gender difference (Barkin et al., 2007; Jackson et al., 1999) with boys more likely to lose a privilege than girls. When considering sex differences in other disciplining methods, this finding may suggest that, in general, boys are more likely to be disciplined than girls regardless of the discipline type. However, the opposite finding was shown in a study examining positive parenting, including the use of explaining what the child did wrong. Women were found to report this disciplining technique more than men (Gamez-Guadix, Straus, Carrobles, Munoz-Rivas, & Almendros, 2010). However, there is also evidence that there may be no sex differences concerning this type of discipline by parents (Padilla-Walker & Carlo, 2004). Nonviolent discipline is an 16 accepted and positive form of discipline however, less information is known about its use among boys and girls. Considering the more definitive sex differences in other physical disciplining methods, it may be beneficial to further explore this disciplining method as an alternative technique. Nonviolent discipline and ethnic group membership. There is some evidence to suggest the use of nonviolent disciplining techniques may vary by ethnicity. For example, African Americans and less acculturated Latino Americans were found to use time out less than White Americans and that less acculturated Latinos use the revoking of privileges in comparison with White Americans (Barkin et al., 2007). In contrast, another study has reported that there were no ethnic differences between White, African, and Latino Americans in their most preferred methods of parenting which were verbal praise and reasoning with their children (Medora, Wilson, & Larson, 2001). Ethnic differences in attitudes about nonviolent discipline show different results. For example, African American parents did not differ in their endorsement of verbal explaining, removing privileges, or using time out from White American parents, but Latino American parents endorsed verbal explaining more and time out/removing privileges less than African and White American parents (Caughy & Franzini, 2005). There is less information about the rate of nonviolent discipline among Asian Americans, but some evidence shows that Asian Americans may believe that practices such as time-outs and revoking privileges are associated with Western styles of disciplining and not associated with traditional Asian styles of disciplining (Kim & Hong, 2006). It may be that Asian 17 Americans are less willing to use nonviolent disciplining methods because of culturally different norms concerning parenting and discipline. Overall, past literature seems to support possible ethnic differences in nonviolent discipline, but it is unclear what specific ethnic differences exist in parent disciplining techniques. Psychological aggression. Psychological aggression has many different names in the literature, but encompasses similar operational definitions across various studies. Psychological aggression is often referred to as “verbal abuse,” “verbal aggression,” and “verbal punishment,” and includes behaviors such as yelling/screaming, threatening punishment or spanking, name-calling/insulting, and swearing to control child behavior (Barkin et al., 2007; Berlin et al., 2009; Caughy & Franzini, 2005; Jackson et al., 1999; Joiner, Sachs-Ericsson, Wingate, Brown, Anestis, & Selby, 2007; Moore & Pepler, 2006; Padilla-Walker & Carlo, 2004; Straus & Field, 2003). The use of psychological aggression has been associated with negative child outcomes such as increased aggression and decreased emotional responsiveness (Berlin et al., 2009), and increased depression, and anxiety (Miller-Perrin, Perrin, & Kocur, 2009). There may be some association between psychological aggression and other parent disciplining practices. It is not well known how psychological aggression is related to nonviolent discipline, but there is some suggestion that they are negatively correlated (Padilla-Walker & Carlo, 2004). The relationship between psychological aggression and corporal punishment seems to be clearer. The use of psychological aggression may be positively related to the use of corporal punishment (Berlin et al., 2009; Fung & Lau, 2009; Jackson et al., 1999, 18 Miller-Perrin et al., 2009; Wissow, 2001). There is also some evidence to suggest that use of psychological aggression by parents is associated with using physically abusive punishment (Miller-Perrin et al., 2009; Thompson et al., 1999). Given the lack of information about psychological aggression, it would be beneficial to investigate this technique further when studying alternative disciplining practices and physical abuse. Psychological aggression and child sex. Mixed findings have been reported about sex differences in parents’ use of psychological aggression. Some studies of minor children suggest that boys and girls are equally likely to receive psychological aggression, including yelling, threats, and name-calling, from their parents (Barkin et al., 2007; Jackson et al., 1999; Padilla-Walker & Carlo, 2004; Trickett & Kuczynski, 1986). Alternatively, other studies suggest that men may report higher psychological aggression in childhood than women (Gamez-Guadix et al., 2010). The use of psychological aggression may be closely tied to the use of corporal punishment, and sex differences in corporal punishment seem to be clearer. It may be possible that psychological aggression is a precursor for corporal punishment use and this disciplining technique may be important to consider in studies of sex differences in parent discipline. Psychological aggression and ethnic group membership. Past research suggests that psychological aggression may vary by ethnicity. For example, less acculturated Latino Americans were found to yell at their children less often than White Americans, but White Americans and African Americans were not found to differ in this parenting practice (Barkin et al., 2007). A similar result showed White Americans and African 19 Americans using similar rates of psychological aggression, but both used more psychological aggression than Latino Americans (Lee et al., 2008). Conversely, African Americans tend to yell or use negative comments more than White or Latino Americans (Berlin et al., 2009). In contrast, it was found that name-calling was used more by White Americans than African Americans (Jackson-Newsom, Buchanan, & McDonald, 2008). Others suggest that psychologically aggressive forms of discipline do not vary by ethnicity (Straus & Field, 2003). Given these limited findings, it is difficult to discuss any definitive conclusions about psychologically aggressive parenting behaviors among different ethnic groups. Future research in this area may facilitate the investigation of relationships between ethnicity, psychological aggression, and physical abuse. Nonphysical parenting practices are important to consider because if they are not available as disciplining alternatives in particular cultural groups, parents may use more physical disciplining practices to manage child behavior. Past research has focused on the association between corporal punishment and physical abuse. Little is known about parents’ use of other disciplining practices in combination with their use of corporal punishment. If there is an association between corporal punishment by parents and physical abuse, future research in the field of physical abuse should attempt to consider other forms of parent discipline in addition to corporal punishment use. It is possible the use of nonviolent discipline or psychological aggression may be effective forms of discipline and may reduce the use of more intrusive forms of punishment like corporal 20 punishment or parenting behaviors that are considered to be physically abusive by CPS standards. Corporal punishment. One limitation in the study of corporal punishment is that it is not clearly differentiated from physically abusive behaviors by parents in past research. These two variables do not have a consistent definition across studies in mental health literature. Specifically, nonabusive forms of corporal punishment are often included with physically abusive types of behaviors in operational definitions of corporal punishment for some studies and the reverse is true for some studies on physical abuse. For some researchers, this problem in developing better operational definitions for corporal punishment versus physically abusive parenting behavior may be due to the use of a very commonly employed measure, the Parent-Child version of the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTSPC) (Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore, & Runyan, 1998). The CTSPC contains a physical punishment subscale that includes both abusive and nonabusive corporal punishment behaviors. While the authors have acknowledged the presence of both abusive and nonabusive corporal punishment items in the subscale, they do not specify a standardized way of determining or defining which of these items may be categorized into an abusive and nonabusive component. They suggest that the subscale may be divided into two components or may be used as a single, complete subscale. This ambiguity has allowed each researcher in the past to interpret the scale differently and define corporal punishment and abusive disciplining practices according to their own operational definitions. 21 To help resolve this problem, it has been suggested that researchers should examine previous reports on the frequency of a corporal punishment behavior such as spanking to help differentiate between ordinary corporal punishment and corporal punishment which is excessive and has become physical abuse. For example, Whipple & Richey’s (1997) study on the specific rates of parental disciplining behaviors may help to provide some direction in clarifying the difference between abusive and nonabusive spanking by parents. They reported that physically abusive mothers spanked their children more than nonabusive mothers. Specifically, what made a socially-accepted corporal punishment technique such as spanking “physically abusive” behavior was the excessiveness of its use. They also clearly defined that the difference between nonabusive and abusive spanking in their study was the mother spanking a child more than 5.73 times per day. However, a major limitation of this study was the use of an abusive parent population to compare with a nonabusive parent sample to determine what constituted abusive spanking. Another limitation of this study is that only spanking behavior was measured in the definition of corporal punishment. It is possible that parents who use corporal punishment may use multiple corporal punishment behaviors to discipline their children. Other researchers emphasize that specific behaviors may help to differentiate corporal punishment from physical abuse. Most studies use a composite average of the rates of several behaviors and these specific behaviors can vary from study to study. For example, many studies investigating the use of corporal punishment include common 22 definitions such as spanking on the bottom with a bare hand, spanking on the bottom with an object, shaking for children over the age of two, and pinching (Deater-Deckard et. al., 1996; Dietz, 2000; Giles-Sims et al., 1995; Leary, Kelley, Morrow, & Mikulka, 2008; Miller-Perrin et al., 2009; Straus & Stewart, 1999; Whipple & Richey, 1997; WhitesideMansell, Bradley & McKelvey, 2009). These disciplining behaviors have never been used in any previous operational definitions of physical abuse in the mental health literature. However, many studies investigating the outcomes of physical abuse have used operational definitions of specific abusive behaviors such as hitting with a fist, kicking, choking, beating up, and burning a child or adolescent (Afifi et. al., 2006; Calder, McVean, & Wang, 2010; Maker, Shah, & Agha, 2009; Zolotor et. al., 2008). These abusive behaviors have not usually been used in any definitions of corporal punishment behaviors in the psychology literature. However, there is a list of other parental behaviors that have been evenly categorized or defined as corporal punishment (Amato & Fowler, 2002; Gershoff, 2002; Lansford, Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 2004; Maikovich et al., 2008; Mathurin, et al., 2006; McCabe, Clark & Barnett, 1999; Strassberg et al., 1994) or physical abuse (Afifi et al., 2006; Edwards et al., 2003; Fletcher, 2009; Johnsona et. al, 2002; Springer et al., 2007; Thompson et. al., 2009). These behaviors include hitting on other parts of the body with an object, slapping of the head/ears, slapping of the arms or legs, and throwing/knocking down a child. Categorizing these behaviors sometimes as corporal punishment and sometimes as physical abuse presents a problem for two reasons. One problem is that studies using 23 such inconsistent definitions about corporal punishment and physically abusive behaviors would be very difficult to compare to one another. Another problem is that it would be difficult to determine any conclusive information about whether corporal punishment is related to physically abusive parenting if specific behaviors used to define these two constructs are used interchangeably across these two definitions. Future studies in the field of corporal punishment and physical abuse of children should attempt to standardize definitions across these types of studies, especially with the use of the CTSPC. This would allow studies in each field to be more easily compared with one another and more conclusive results can be found in regards to the possible negative outcomes of corporal punishment and physical abuse. A more standardized definition would help future studies that attempt to find a relationship between corporal punishment and physical abuse. Corporal punishment and physical abuse. Common views about the use of corporal punishment for disciplining have not been well established in the United States. This may be due to general attitudes of parents that corporal punishment is sometimes necessary to manage children’s behavior (Straus, 2000). Corporal punishment may be an effective tool in decreasing noncompliance (Bean & Roberts, 1981; Day & Roberts, 1983; Gershoff, 2002; Larzelere, & Kuhn, 2005) which may contribute to these general attitudes. It is possible that due to these attitudes and the effectiveness of corporal punishment, it is a widely and frequently used disciplining technique. For example, it was found that more than half of the mothers from the National Longitudinal Study of 24 Youth (NLSY) spank their 3-5 year old children (Giles-Sims et al., 1995). Ninety-four percent of caregivers from the Gallup Organization survey (1995) use corporal punishment with their young children and although this rate declines as a child becomes older, a third of the parents were still using corporal punishment with their adolescent children (Straus & Stewart, 1999). Aside from parent attitudes and behavior, parents may not reject corporal punishment as a disciplining technique because laws in the United States have been somewhat unclear about what is acceptable discipline for parents to use. This is in contrast to a growing trend in other countries that have passed laws to prohibit corporal punishment to discipline children. For example, twenty-nine countries including Norway, Croatia, Romania, and Costa Rica have instituted a ban on corporal punishment by parents and twenty-three other countries are in the process of adopting similar laws prohibiting the use of corporal punishment (Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children [GIEACPC], 2010). Although the passage of these laws may not necessarily reflect the residents’ attitudes and behaviors on corporal punishment, it is a legal action undertaken by these nations’ policymakers to eliminate the occurrence of this behavior by parents. In these countries, protecting children from corporal punishment is viewed as part of a larger human rights agenda to protect all people from interpersonal violence (GIEACPC, 2009). To support this case, child advocates in these countries have cited research on the negative physical and psychological consequences (e.g., increased depression and posttraumatic stress disorder) for victims of interpersonal violence such as 25 intimate partner violence, violence experienced while incarcerated, and being physically bullied (Abada, Hou, & Ram, 2008; Lee, Pomeroy, & Bohman, 2007; Wolff & Shi, 2010). It is suggested that corporal punishment by parents is a form of interpersonal violence that can have negative physical and psychological outcomes. For example, research has shown that recipients of corporal punishment reported with more depression and externalizing behaviors (Eamon, 2001; Lansford et al., 2004; Larzelere, Klein, Shumm, & Alibrando, 1989; Mulvaney & Mebert, 2007; Strassberg et al., 1994) than those who have not received this form of punishment. This information may assist policymakers in implementing appropriate and clearer laws in the U.S. regarding what disciplining techniques are acceptable for parents to use. Opposite to most parents’ views about corporal punishment, researchers have often discouraged parents from using corporal punishment, citing the negative psychological outcomes associated with this disciplining technique such as depression and lower grades (Amato & Fowler, 2002; Bryan & Freed, 1982; Lynam, Loeber, Stouthamer-Loeber, 2008), externalizing behavior problems (Deater-Deckard et al., 1996; Gershoff, 2002; Strassberg et al., 1994), and socioemotional problems (Eamon, 2001), for children and adult children. Others believe that the risks of using corporal punishment for disciplining are even greater because they may increase the risk for physical abuse by parents (Garbarino, 1977; Gershoff, 2002; Gil, 1970; Kadushin & Martin, 1981; Straus, 2000). It is important to note that some researchers suggest that it is unlikely for a parent to deliberately consider physically abusing their child to control 26 child behavior. They believe the decision to use corporal punishment by the parent as punishment for a child may escalate into abusive practices when ordinary corporal punishment is not found to be effective. In a study of child physical abuse in sixty-six families, Kadushin and Martin (1981) concluded: There is little evidence in these interviews that unwarranted malice, deliberate sadism, or deep-rooted, persistent animosity toward the child prompted parental abuse. Invariably, there was some behavior on the part of the child which served to instigate an interactional, stimulus-response chain of interdependent actions culminating in abuse. (p. 141) In another investigation of 1,380 cases of child abuse, over half of these cases involved an attempt to discipline the child by a parent or guardian. These findings suggest that perhaps prior disciplining strategies by the parent may not have been effective and that the discipline later became abusive (Gil, 1970). Non-abused samples may show similar results. In a general sample of adult children, the use of corporal punishment was related to also experiencing physical abuse (Miller-Perrin et al., 2009). While these studies suggest a possible link between corporal punishment and experiencing physical abuse, a causal relationship has not been determined. Specific corporal punishment behaviors endorsed by a parent may give insight into the possible occurrence of physical abuse by this same parent. It was found that children who were spanked on the bottom with a bare hand were two times as likely to have experienced physically abusive behaviors from their parents and children that had 27 experienced spanking on the bottom with a hard object were eight times more likely to experience physically abusive behaviors from their parents (Zolotor et al., 2008). Currently, it is not clear if the use of corporal punishment behaviors is a significant predictor of physical abuse by parents. If there is a significant relationship between corporal punishment and physical abuse, it is possible that certain types of corporal punishment behaviors may be better predictors of physical abuse than other forms of corporal punishment. Corporal punishment and child sex. Differences in corporal punishment suggest that more boys tend to be punished with spanking, slapping, pinching, shaking, and hairpulling than girls (Bryan & Freed, 1982; Giles-Sims, et. al., 1995; Jackson et al., 1999; Lansford et al., 2004; Mathurin et al., 2006; Mcloyd & Smith, 2002; Straus & Stewart, 1999). However, other researchers have found that boys and girls are corporally punished with spanking at similar rates (Afifi et. al., 2006; Lytton & Romney, 1991; Strassberg, et. al., 1994; Trickett & Kuczynski, 1986). Studies that examine a wide range of corporal punishment behaviors tend to show that there may be a sex difference in how children are punished. It is possible that more common forms of punishment, such as spanking do not vary by child’s sex. The results of these studies suggest that sex differences may exist in some corporal punishment behaviors however, these have not been previously explored. Corporal punishment and ethnic group membership. Some cultural or ethnic groups may believe the use of corporal punishment is not detrimental to children. In 28 certain situations, some cultural groups may find that corporal punishment is acceptable especially when it takes place within positive parent-child relationships (Baumrind, 1996a; Horn, 2004; Lansford et al., 2004; Polaha, Larzelere, Shapiro, & Pettit, 2004), while others believe that corporal punishment leads to negative outcomes, including physical abuse regardless of child’s ethnic background (Gershoff, 2002; Matta, 2002; Mcloyd & Smith, 2002; Whiteside-Mansell et al., 2009). Comparative studies among children of different ethnic groups have led researchers to believe that stronger ethnic identification with certain ethnic groups, specifically identifying as African American, and adopting their cultural practices may serve as a protective factor for any negative effects of corporal punishment for child targets. It is not clear why being strongly identified or feeling a part of a certain ethnic or cultural group would serve as a protective factor from the negative consequences of corporal punishment. Some suggest that specific characteristics of a cultural group such as social acceptability of attitudes about corporal punishment may make the experiencing of corporal punishment less detrimental (Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1996; Haskett, Allaire, Kreig, & Hart, 2008; Horn, 2004; Lansford et al., 2004; Polaha et al., 2004). However, most of these studies have focused on psychological outcomes and not the risk for physical abuse. Future studies should investigate this possible protective factor among different ethnic groups in relation to risk of physical abuse. It was suggested earlier that specific parenting practices such as nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment may significantly differ 29 across ethnic groups because of possible differences in generally acceptable cultural practices of disciplining children. Several studies have suggested that the disciplining practice of corporal punishment does vary across ethnic groups both in attitudes and in reported behaviors. It is important to take these possible differences into consideration when it may appear that an ethnic group is over-represented in cases of physical abuse. Investigating differences in parent disciplining methods may help to better explain why over-representation of certain ethnic groups in physical abuse cases may occur. Corporal punishment attitudes. Attitudes about corporal punishment may differ across ethnic groups because of cultural beliefs about parent disciplining. For example, White American mothers are significantly more likely than African American mothers to believe that corporal punishment is necessary if they believe that the child is intentionally misbehaving (Burchinal, Skinner, & Reznick, 2010). Studies in the past have found significant differences across ethnic groups in attitudes about corporal punishment. Specifically, research suggests that Asian and African Americans may hold a more positive view of corporal punishment than White and Latino Americans. For example, immigrant ethnic groups in the U.S. endorse corporal punishment (as defined by the authors as beating, burning, and scratching their children) (Hong & Hong, 1991). Specifically, these authors found that Asian Americans rated these corporal punishment behaviors by parents as less severe than Latino or White Americans. Additionally, Asian and African American mothers favored the use of corporal punishment more than White and Latino American mothers (Jambunathan, Burts & Pierce, 2000). Consistent with this 30 study, African American mothers believed spanking was a more effective method for disciplining than White American mothers and both African and White American mothers believed spanking was a more effective disciplining technique than Latino American mothers (Caughy & Franzini, 2005). In a separate study, Flynn (1998) found both gender and ethnicity differences in the endorsement of spanking by African and White Americans. Specifically, African Americans and men were more likely to support spanking as a form of punishment than Whites and females, respectively. White American women endorsed only verbal reasoning to discipline a misbehaving child, but African American women endorsed verbal reasoning and spanking a misbehaving child to correct his/her behavior (Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates & Pettit, 1995 as cited in Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997). African American students were also found to be more accepting of spanking, pulling, pinching, and slapping as a means for disciplining by parents than Latino or White American students (Ibanez, Borrego, Pemberton, & Terao, 2006). Collectively, these studies seem to suggest a certain trend in parental attitudes about corporal punishment across different ethnic groups. These studies suggest that Asian and African Americans may have a more positive view of corporal punishment than Latino and White Americans. Given that attitudes and behavior are often significantly related, this information may lead us to believe that there are possible ethnic differences in the rates or incidence of certain corporal punishment behaviors. Corporal punishment behaviors. Past studies have suggested that corporal punishment behaviors may vary across ethnic groups. For example, it was found that 31 being hit with or without an object, slapped on an extremity/face/head, pinched, and being shaken occurred more for African American children than White American children (Straus & Stewart, 1999). This finding supports the results of a previous study which found that African American children were more likely to be hit with or without an object and slapped on an extremity/face/head than White American children (DeaterDeckard et. al. 1996). Further supporting these findings, African American parents were found to use spanking more than White American parents, however Latino and White Americans did not differ in their rate of spanking in this same study (Wissow, 2001). Several studies have found that Latino Americans use similar rates of spanking as White Americans (Barkin et al., 2007; Berlin et. al., 2009; Weller, Romney, & Orr, 1987). Therefore, it may be incorrect to believe that ethnic minorities, in general, use more physical forms of punishment compared to White Americans. Instead, examining specific groups is important. Ethnic differences may also exist in the different types of corporal punishment used to discipline children. Specific corporal punishment behaviors may be important to study because certain behaviors may be more closely related to physical abuse than others (Zolotor et al., 2008). One study (Lansford et. al., 2004) found that African American parents used a wider range of corporal punishment behaviors to discipline their children than White American parents at kindergarten and sixth and eighth grades. Studies on attitudes about corporal punishment are fairly similar to the behavioral studies concerning ethnic differences in the use of corporal punishment. In these studies, African 32 Americans tend to use more corporal punishment techniques or behaviors than other ethnic groups and Latino and White American parents tend to use similar amounts of corporal punishment behaviors (Barkin et al., 2007; Berlin et. al., 2009; Deater-Deckard et. al. 1996; Straus & Stewart, 1999; Wissow, 2001). However, a limitation of these past studies on corporal punishment attitudes is that Asian Americans are often overlooked and not included in these samples. Future studies in the field of corporal punishment should attempt to include Asian Americans since corporal punishment behaviors among this group have not been frequently studied in the past and this group is underrepresented in national child abuse studies (U.S. DHHS, 2007; Sedlak et al., 2010a). If experiences of corporal punishment and physical abuse are related, this may have implications for groups that use corporal punishment and are over-represented in physical abuse cases. As previously described, certain ethnic groups were found to be over-represented in physical abuse cases reported to authorities (Hughes, 2006; Klevens & Leeb, 2010; Lee et al., 2008; U.S. DHHS, 2007; Sedlak & Broadhurst, 1996; Sedlak et al., 2010a). However, it is not clear if there may be similar ethnic differences in unsubstantiated cases or unreported cases of physical abuse. There may or may not be similar ethnic differences for unsubstantiated cases, or unreported cases of physical abuse. Unfortunately, less information is known about these other types of incidents that may involve physically abusive behaviors by parents. It would be beneficial if future research examined a general population which could be more representative of all of these types of physical abuse incidents. Information about possible physical abuse in the 33 general population may reveal different findings from the representation of ethnicity among children with documented experiences of physical abuse. In summary, past studies have shown there are ethnic differences in corporal punishment attitudes and behaviors. Some believe that a protective factor against negative psychological outcomes may exist for certain ethnic groups when using corporal punishment with their children, but it is unknown if this protective factor also occurs against the risk for negative outcomes in the use of physically abusive behaviors by parents. Ethnic differences may exist in the incidence of physical abuse cases as well, but little information is known about the representation of ethnic groups in general populations experiencing corporal punishment and physical abuse. There may be a possible relationship between corporal punishment experiences and physical abuse experiences. Future studies should examine the prevalence and types of corporal punishment used by different ethnic groups in the general population and investigate how these factors relate to the use of physically abusive behaviors by parents. This information may clarify why ethnic minorities may be over-represented in physical abuse cases. Other contributing factors. When examining possible relationships between ethnic group membership, parenting styles, parent disciplining practices, and physically abusive behaviors, several other variables were often included in previous studies and suggest a close association to these variables. These closely related variables may need to be considered in any research design that attempts to study the relationship between 34 ethnicity, parent disciplining practices, and physical abuse. It may be these variables are more closely related to physically abusive behaviors by parents than other variables such as parenting practices. Separate examination of these variables will help explain the relationship among ethnic group membership, parent disciplining practices, and physically abusive behaviors in a more precise way. Parent place of birth or immigrant status. Immigrant status or the parents’ place of birth may play a critical role in determining preferred parenting styles and parent disciplining practices. Immigrants who move to the United States may be following culturally accepted standards or parenting styles from their native countries or countries of origin even if their parenting behaviors are culturally different from what is the accepted norm in the U.S. For example, in a study of six countries, parents in Kenya and Italy reported the highest levels of corporal punishment, followed by parents in the Philippines and India who reported moderate levels of corporal punishment, and parents in China and Thailand reported the lowest levels of corporal punishment (Lansford et. al., 2005). About 75% of parents in Egypt use corporal punishment (Abolfotouh, El-Bourgy, Seif El Din, & Mehanna, 2009) in comparison with 94% of U.S. parents that use corporal punishment. In contrast, only 2% of Egyptian parents have used time-out compared with 69% of U.S. parents. In a study of over two hundred Jewish and Arab mothers in Israel, Arab mothers endorsed all forms of discipline (i.e., nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment) more than Jewish mothers (Khoury-Kassabri, 2009). This may suggest that newly immigrated people, or children of immigrants, may 35 report with a greater likelihood to use or experience certain disciplining practices more than others if this is the culturally accepted custom in their native countries. Another aspect of this factor is that often certain ethnic groups in the U.S. are more likely to be comprised of immigrants or children of immigrants. For example, Asian and Latino Americans are more likely to be immigrants or children of immigrants than African or White Americans. Unfortunately, general information about the frequency and prevalence of nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment is not readily available for many countries. The lack of such information makes it difficult to state any specific conclusions about known differences concerning parent discipline between different cultures. However, considering there may be possible crosscultural differences in parenting styles and disciplining techniques, this is a specific variable that should be considered when studying the relationship of ethnicity, corporal punishment, and physical abuse. Socioeconomic status. Socioeconomic status (SES) may also be associated with group differences in parenting styles, parent disciplining practices, and physical abuse. For example, it was found that people with higher SES were engaging in more authoritative parenting practices and people with lower SES engaging in more authoritarian parenting practices (Bluestone & Tamis-LeMonda, 1999; Kaufman et al., 2000). Lower socioeconomic status was also related to higher rates of permissive parenting (Bluestone, et al., 1999). Another study confirmed these results that high and middle SES families engaged in higher rates of authoritative parenting and low SES 36 families exhibited higher rates of authoritarian and permissive parenting than high/middle SES families (Dornbusch et al., 1987). Considering this variable in a study parenting styles would be important given the consistent differences in parenting styles across different SES levels. In a review of the literature, less is known about the relationship of SES to different forms of parent disciplining including corporal punishment. Several researchers do not believe there is a significant relationship between SES and nonviolent discipline (Barkin et al., 2007; Jackson et al. 1999). Specifically, both high and low SES parents were found to use similar amounts of nonviolent discipline. With regard to the use of psychological aggression by different SES groups, mixed results have been found. Most of the studies show no significant relationship between the use of psychological aggression and SES (Barkin et al., 2007; Jackson et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2008; Straus, 2003). However, one study found that lower SES may be related to using more psychological aggression (Berlin et al., 2009). In contrast, the use of corporal punishment has been found to be more consistently related to low SES (Berlin et al., 2009; Deater-Deckard et. al., 1996; Dietz, 2000; Lansford, et. al., 2004; Pinderhughes, Dodge, Bates, Pettit, & Zelli, 2000; Straus & Stewart, 1999; Wager, 2009). If this relationship is true, then it is possible that low socioeconomic status may explain higher corporal punishment use by parents. It is also possible that SES may show the closest association to use of corporal punishment, even when compared to ethnicity. There is some evidence to suggest that physically abused children often come from the lowest 37 SES families (Klimes-Dougan, & Kistner, 1990; Lee et al., 2008; Mulvaney & Mebert, 2007). Although SES may not be related to nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression, given the possible relationship between SES, corporal punishment use, and physical abuse, this is a variable that should be considered as a possible contributing variable in any study of corporal punishment and physical abuse. Inclusion of this variable would clarify any possible relationships between SES, ethnic group membership, parent disciplining practices, and physical abuse. Family structure. Family structure components, defined by family size and the marital status of the parents, are factors that are closely related to socioeconomic status and physical abuse of children in past literature. First, family size must be considered when examining SES since the financial resources of the family can become increasingly limited as the size of the family grows larger. Second, SES may be confounded by the marital status of the parents since a two-parent family is more likely to report with a higher SES than a single-parent family. This factor must be considered when studying socioeconomic status since a measure of SES based solely on income alone may not be reflective of the resources available for each family. Family size and parent marital status should be considered in a study of physical abuse since they may be closely related. Specifically, as a family grows larger, parents are more likely to feel the stressors of parenting and may be at greater risk to engage in physically abusive behavior (Baldwin & Oliver, 1975; Klerman, 1993). Finally, parents who are unmarried or separated may also experience extra stress due to separation and 38 the demands of single parenting and this could make such single parents more likely than married parents to engage in physically abusive behavior (Afifi, Boman, Fleisher, & Sareen, 2009; Irwin, 2009; Wu et al., 2004 ) Age. It may be important to consider the participant’s current age when studying different dimensions of parenting, such as behavior and attitudes, in an adult population. The participant’s age may better indicate the specific generation in which s/he grew up and the general experiences of parenting and disciplining associated with a specific time period may vary from generation to generation. One example of these differences is seen cross culturally in a study of mothers and grandmothers from three different cultures. While the mothers reported some similar beliefs about children and childcare to the grandmothers, many of the beliefs had changed over time and this may be due to differing child rearing environments (Lamm, Keller, Yovsi, & Chaudhary, 2008). Another reason for these changes may be that recommendations by parenting experts may have changed over time and this may lead to differences in parental attitudes and behaviors across generations. One example of possible differences in parenting styles was found among two different generations of parents in the United States. Specifically, the first-generation parents showed higher levels of authoritarian and permissive parenting and lower levels of authoritative parenting than the second-generation parents (Pritchard-Boone, 2007). Differences were also found in a comparative generational study of parent behaviors where the parenting for the first generation was observed in 1985, while the second generation was observed in 2002 (Bailey, Hill, Oesterle, & 39 Hawkins, 2009). Among these parents, some discontinuity in harsh parenting (i.e., yelling, spanking) was shown across generations, with the second generation reporting lesser endorsement of such behaviors. A similar finding was reported among immigrant families in the United States in which second generation parents adopted some of disciplining attitudes and methods of their parents’ generation, but they had moved away from the use of corporal punishment as a disciplining technique (Waters & Sykes, 2009). It may be that newer generations of parents may be less likely to engage in certain parenting styles and behaviors, particularly psychological aggression and corporal punishment as compared with previous generations. This may lead to increased use of alternative techniques to control child behavior, such as nonviolent discipline among these younger parents. When measuring responses about parenting styles and discipline among adult children, it may be important to examine the participant’s age to reduce some of the generational differences in parenting. Summary This literature review of the factors that may be related to physical abuse suggests several themes in the current literature: 1. Previous studies have shown an over-representation of ethnic minorities in reported physical abuse cases. However, the reasons for these findings are not clear. Previous studies have also found other factors that are highly correlated with physical abuse, such as low socioeconomic status, which may be over-represented in ethnic minority groups. However, recent studies 40 have shown that the gaps in SES have decreased between different ethnic groups. Other factors may be responsible for explaining the overrepresentation of certain groups in physical abuse cases. 2. Certain parent disciplining practices, particularly corporal punishment, may be associated with physical abuse of children. Parents typically have a range of disciplining practices that they can use with their children. In the past, there has been a lack of research attention regarding nonphysical parenting practices such as nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression. Parenting practices do not occur in isolation and future studies must examine these three types of discipline together when investigating a possible link to physical abuse. It is possible that access or use of nonphysical disciplining methods may reduce the likelihood of using more physical forms of punishment. 3. Previous studies have had differing and sometimes overlapping definitions of corporal punishment and physical abuse. This has made it difficult to determine if corporal punishment is a negative experience and also if it is associated with the use of physical abuse. Future studies should be more specific in the way these factors are described and operationally defined. For example, specificity in describing the behaviors associated with these factors would make the experiences of corporal punishment and the relationship between these corporal punishment and physical abuse clearer. 41 4. Parent disciplining techniques such as nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment may differ by ethnicity. Factors such as immigrant status and parents’ place of birth or the sex of the child being disciplined may contribute to the cultural group’s attitudes about discipline. It may be that different attitudes about parenting are culturally appropriate for each ethnic group and these may result in different disciplining behaviors. Future studies should report information about rates of specific disciplining behaviors in order to further support this relationship. 5. It is important to understand not only how rates of parent discipline is related to physically abusive behavior, but the context that would make it more likely for parents of certain ethnic groups to use particular parent disciplining techniques. One factor, preferred parenting styles, may be related to the decision to use some disciplining practices over others. Parenting styles are characterized by two dimensions, parental warmth and demandingness. For parents, the reported levels of these two factors may be related to using more coercive disciplining techniques such as corporal punishment and psychological aggression as compared with more acceptable forms of discipline such as nonviolent discipline. Present Study The present study will examine if there are gender and ethnic group differences in reported parent disciplining practices such as nonviolent discipline, psychological 42 aggression, and corporal punishment among Asian, African, Latino and White American students. This study will also examine if there are gender and ethnic differences in reported physically abusive behaviors in childhood. The significant relationship between the different types of corporal punishment and reported types of physical abuse will also be investigated. Finally, this study will examine each individual ethnic group to find possible differences in the relationship between specific disciplining techniques and reported physical abuse. The first set of hypotheses (1-5) will determine whether any sex and ethnic differences exist in parenting styles (authoritarian, authoritative, permissive), the three disciplining methods (nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, corporal punishment) and physically abusive behavior controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin (Figure 1). Hypotheses 1-5: It is hypothesized that males will report higher levels of authoritarian parenting, and lower levels of authoritative and permissive parenting in childhood than females. It is also hypothesized that males will report with lower nonviolent discipline, and higher psychological aggression, corporal punishment, and physically abusive behavior in childhood than females. It is hypothesized that Asian and African Americans will report with higher levels of authoritarian parenting style and lower rates of authoritative and permissive parenting styles in childhood than White or Latino Americans. Asian and African Americans will also report with fewer experiences of nonviolent discipline and more experiences of psychological aggression, corporal 43 punishment, and physically abusive behavior in childhood, than Latino and White Americans, controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin. The second set of hypotheses (6-11) will control for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin and will determine whether parenting styles are related to the three parent disciplining techniques and physically abusive behavior. In addition, the relationships between the three parent disciplining methods (nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment) will be tested. Finally, this set of hypotheses will explore if each of the disciplining methods are significantly related to physically abusive behavior by parents, controlling for the significant contributions of these covariates. Hypotheses 6: It is hypothesized that higher levels of authoritarian parenting style will be related to decreased experiences of nonviolent discipline and increased experiences of psychological aggression, corporal punishment, and physically abusive behavior in childhood controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin. Hypothesis 7: It is hypothesized that higher levels authoritative parenting style will be related to increased experiences of nonviolent discipline and decreased experiences of psychological aggression, corporal punishment, and physically abusive behavior in childhood controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin. 44 Hypothesis 8: It is hypothesized that higher levels of permissive parenting style will be related to decreased experiences of all three disciplining types in childhood controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin. It is also hypothesized that higher levels permissive parenting styles will be related to increased experiences of physically abusive behavior in childhood controlling for the significant contributions of covariates. Hypothesis 9: It is hypothesized that higher rates of reported nonviolent discipline will be associated with lower rates of psychological aggression, corporal punishment, and physically abusive behavior in childhood, controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin Hypothesis 10: It is hypothesized that higher rates of reported psychological aggression will be associated with experiencing greater corporal punishment and more physically abusive behaviors in childhood, controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin, and nonviolent discipline. Hypothesis 11: It is hypothesized that greater experiences of corporal punishment will be associated with higher rates of physical abusive behavior in childhood controlling for the significant contribution of socioeconomic status of family of origin, nonviolent discipline, and psychological aggression. The following hypothesis will test whether corporal punishment best explains physically abusive behaviors by parents when compared with the significant contributions of three parenting styles (i.e., authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive), 45 nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, ethnicity, sex, and socioeconomic status of family of origin. Hypothesis 12: It is hypothesized that controlling for the socioeconomic status of family of origin, higher rates of corporal punishment will the strongest predictor of physically abusive behaviors in childhood than sex, ethnicity, experiences of authoritarian parenting, authoritative parenting, permissive parenting, nonviolent discipline, and psychological aggression. 46 Chapter 2 METHOD Participants The participants were 945 undergraduate college students from a large northern California university that were recruited for a larger survey study. They were recruited for the study through the campus psychology research website and received course credit for participation. To be included in the final analysis, participants had to meet all of the following selection criteria: 1) only participants who self-identified themselves as being Asian, African, Latino, or White American were included in the final sample, 2) only participants within the age range of 17-25 years were included in the final sample to ensure a more age-compatible sample, 3) only participants who provide valid information about socioeconomic status were included in the final sample to control for this important variable, and 4) only participants who provided valid information for all other variables of interest in the study were included in the final sample. The final sample consisted 706 participants and the ethnic distribution was 242 Asian American (32.64% male, 67.36% female), 101 African American (29.70 % male, 70.30% female), 203 Latino American (23.65% male, 76.35% female), and 160 White American (25.63% male, 74.37% female) college students. The mean age of the final sample was 20.16 years and participants’ family of origin socioeconomic index scores ranged from 3 to 66, with a mean SES index of 37.11. The number of participants in the final sample exceeded the minimum number 47 of participants (N = 107) needed for a multiple regression analysis with more than eight variables with a power level of .80 and .05 alpha level for the effect size as outlined by Cohen (1992). Measures Independent variables Demographics. Participants provided demographic information including ethnicity, sex, age, parent place of birth, parent marital status, family size, parent occupation, and parent education. Ethnicity was self-identified by the participants (Asian American, African American, Latino American, White American, American Indian/Native American, or Other/Multiracial). Ethnicity was then dummy-coded for each participant, with each participant being coded as one for the ethnic group with which they self-identified and coded as zero for all other ethnic group variables. Participants also identified whether they were male or female their age in years, and their place of birth. In addition, participants indicated if their parents were U.S.-born or foreign-born, the country of mother and father’s birth, and their parents’ marital status. Finally, the students indicated how many people were dependent on their family’s income. Hollingshead’s Four Factor Index of Social Status (1975) was used to determine socioeconomic status of the participants’ families. This index has been commonly used to calculate socioeconomic status in past research (Deater-Deckard et al., 1996; Lansford et al., 2004; Lynam et al., 2008; Pinderhughes et al., 2000; Polaha et al., 2004) and uses a 48 formula based on the marital status of the parents, each parent’s occupation, and each parent’s level of education. In the present study, a deviation from Hollingshead’s original formula was used to include participants whose parents were reported as unemployed. Hollingshead’s original four factor calculation requires information on the parent’s occupation and education level to compute a final SES index score. The formula used to calculate the socioeconomic status for a family, result in a SES range from eight to 66. Socioeconomic index scores for a particular family are divided by two if parents are married and both employed and not divided if the parent is single or if one of the married parents does not have gainful employment. To include participants with both parents without gainful employment, only educational information was used to calculate the socioeconomic index for these participants and final scores on this modified SES index ranged from three to 66. For example, if two married parents had received an 7th grade education or lower but were both unemployed, they would be coded as one. This score would be weighted by multiplying by three, adding the two scores and then dividing by two, as suggested by Hollingshead. This deviated from Hollingshead’s formula because families with both parents being unemployed or single parents who were not employed were not considered to have a valid socioeconomic index in his four factor model. In the final sample for this study, only 33 out of 706 participants reported with two parents who were unemployed. Parenting styles. The Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) was used to measure authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting styles (Buri, 1991). This 49 30-item scale is often used in studies of parenting styles and child outcomes (Dominguez & Carton, 1997; Flett, Hewitt, & Singer, 1995; Furnham & Cheng, 2000; Gonzalez, Greenwood, & WenHsu, 2001; Klein, Bryant, & Hopkins, 1996; Pawlak & Klein, 1997). The PAQ requires informants, either children or parents, to rate their level of agreement on a five point scale ranging from 1-5 (strongly agree to strongly disagree) about the mothers’ and fathers’ general parenting attitudes. The authoritarian parenting subscale included items such as, “Whenever my parents told me to do something as I was growing up, they expected me to do it immediately without asking any questions.” The authoritative parenting subscale included items such as “As I was growing up, once family policy had been established, my parents discussed the reasoning behind to policy with the children in the family.” The permissive parenting subscale included items such as, “As I was growing up, my parents seldom gave me expectations and guidelines for my behavior.” These three ten-item subscales measured mother and father parenting style levels with scores ranging from 10-50 for each parent. The six subscales show high reliability ranging from .71-.92 across several studies (Buri, 1991; Dominguez & Carton, 1997; Furnham & Cheng, 2000). In the present study, the subscales show similar reliability with Chronbach’s alphas ranging from .74 to .85 for the six subscales. Parent discipline. The Parent-Child version of the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTSPC) was used to measure nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment (Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore & Runyan, 1998). The original Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) (Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996), was 50 designed to measure spousal abuse and this scale was later formed into a 22-item scale to measure parent disciplining methods and has been used in many studies (Afifi, et. al., 2006; Deater-Deckard, et. al., 1996; Dietz, 2000; Edwards, et. al., 2003; Feigelman, et. al., 2009; Fung & Lau, 2009; Turner & Muller, 2004). The CTSPC items for the three parent disciplining subscales were identified by Straus et al. Nonviolent discipline (4 items) is defined as the parents explaining why something is wrong, revoking privileges, using time-out, and giving a child something else to do. Psychological aggression (5 items) was defined as parents yelling/screaming, swearing/cursing, threatening to spank, threatening to kick out of the house, and namecalling. The CTSPC items or reported behaviors used to define corporal punishment required some modification for the present study. In past studies, the CTSPC has been used inconsistently to measure critical constructs such as “corporal punishment,” “physical punishment,” and “physical discipline.” To create clear operational definitions of corporal punishment for this study, a strategy was proposed to select specific items from the ordinary corporal punishment subscale of the physical assault subscale of the CTSPC to avoid overlap with definitions of physical abuse as done in past studies. In the present study, four individual items (spanking on the bottom with a bare hand, spanking on the bottom with a hard object, shaking, and pinching) from the ordinary corporal punishment subscale were used separately to measure corporal punishment. Although this definition of corporal punishment deviates from what the authors of the scale suggest as “corporal punishment,” it may result in a clearer definition 51 of corporal punishment as some researchers have used the CTSPC physical assault scale as a measure that represents corporal punishment, while others have used this same scale as a measure of physical abuse. Still others choose individual items from the subscales that may overlap across studies. Considering that the CTSPC physical assault scale contains both abusive and nonabusive items, this can pose a problem for comparing studies that use the CTSPC. To represent corporal punishment in the present study, only the 4 CTSPC items from the corporal punishment subscale were chosen that were not previously used in definitions of physical abuse in the psychology literature. The format of this measure requires the informant, usually the child’s parent, to indicate how many times the disciplining technique was used in a referent period (i.e., one week, twelve months). In the present study, an adult child population was used and the referent period was the adult’s childhood years. The authors of the measure propose two ways of scoring the occurrence of a particular parent disciplining technique: prevalence and chronicity. The prevalence method of scoring reports whether a parent disciplining technique ever occurred within a referent period or over a lifetime. The chronicity method of scoring requires the informant to report the number of times that a parent disciplining technique has occurred in a given referent period. The present study adopted the prevalence scoring method to assess for nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment. The authors of this scale have reported moderate to high reliability. The nonviolent subscale had an alpha coefficient of .70, the psychological aggression 52 subscale had an alpha coefficient of .60, and the overall physical assault scale, which includes both corporal punishment and physically abusive items, had an alpha coefficient of .55 (Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore & Runyan, 1998). Few studies have reported alpha coefficient values on the subscales as originally written. Some studies show that three of the subscales report a reliability range from .50-.77 for psychological aggression (Fung et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2008; Straus & Field, 2003; Tajima, 2002), .57-.80 for corporal punishment (Dietz, 2000; Fung et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2008; Turner & Muller 2004), and .62-.92 for the whole physical assault scale (Maikovich et al., 2008; Tajima, 2002). However, the reliability of the nonviolent disciplining subscale is unknown for other studies. The reliability for the corporal punishment items alone has not been explored by the authors of the CTSPC or by other researchers. In the present study, the Chronbach’s alpha scores were .43 for nonviolent discipline and .63 for psychological aggression. These wide ranges of reliability scores in previous and the present study may be due to different researchers using different scoring methods. The chronicity method of scoring is more commonly used among these studies than the prevalence method. It may be possible that the prevalence method of scoring may yield a more reliable scale. Preliminary analyses on covariates and independent variables. In the original design of this study, several variables were initially proposed as possible covariates for the final analysis based on a review of the literature on this subject matter. A frequency analysis showed a number of outliers on the upper end of participant age so a decision was made to focus the final analyses on participant within the ages of 17-25 years. The 53 ANOVA results from above clearly showed there was no significant ethnic difference on participant age (ages 17-25) and a subsequent decision was made to not include this variable as a covariate as initially proposed in the study. Finally, tests for multicollinearity were conducted to determine if some of the independent variables may be highly correlated. Based on this information, changes in covariates were made for the study hypotheses. One covariate that was removed from the analyses was mother and father place of birth. Since more ethnic minority students reported that their parents were foreign-born, this variable conflicted with one of the main study variables, ethnicity. Family size was removed from the final analyses because it was partially captured by Hollingshead’s socioeconomic status formula, which accounts for the number of parents in the participant’s household of origin. Finally, the Hollingshead socioeconomic status formula used parent marital status to calculate the final SES score for each family. Therefore, only one of these variables could be used in the final analyses. To address this issue and to determine the most relevant variable for the present study, all hypotheses analyses were conducted with parent marital status or socioeconomic status as a covariate. Socioeconomic status was chosen as a covariate because parent marital status was not consistently related to the study dependent variables. Dependent Variables Physically abusive behavior. The Parent-Child version of the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTSPC) was used to measure physically abusive behavior (Straus, Hamby, 54 Finkelhor, Moore & Runyan, 1998). Similar to the nonabusive discipline, the participant were asked to indicate if the physically abusive behavior has occurred during the referent period. In the present study, the prevalence scoring method was also used for this variable to better indicate whether different parent disciplining techniques are associated with the use or nonuse of these more extreme tactics. The physical assault scale, which includes corporal punishment and physically abusive behavior, has moderate to high reliability (Maikovich et al., 2008; Straus et al., 1998; Tajima, 2002). However, less information is known about the reliability of the severe physical assault subscale, which provides information about use of physically abusive items (hit another body part besides bottom with an object, threw/knocked down, kicked/hit with a fist, beat, choked, burned/scald, and threatened with an knife/gun). The authors of the measure reported close to 0 reliability for this measure (-.02). They suggested several of these behaviors are very rare and, it may be difficult to obtain high reliability for this scale due to this low prevalence rate. In the present study, two individual items from the severe physical assault subscale, beating up and the combined item of kicking/hitting with a fist, were combined into a subscale to measure physically abusive behavior used by parents. This modified and newly created subscale showed a Chronbach’s alpha of .61. As with the corporal punishment items, this subscale deviates from what the authors had defined as potentially physically abusive behavior. These items were chosen for two reasons. First, items from the severe physical assault subscale were chosen that were not previously used in definitions of corporal punishment. 55 Second, based on prevalence rates of past studies, the more commonly used items from the severe physical assault scale were chosen as the more extreme CTSPC items from the physically abusive subscale (e.g., burned/scald, threatened with knife/gun) were rarely reported. Studying more commonly used and reported physically abusive behaviors may show a more meaningful relationship between the different disciplining methods and physically abusive behavior. Procedure Prospective participants signed up for the study through the campus psychology research website. Upon arrival at the laboratory, students were informed of the study’s purposes and risks and then asked to provide written and verbal consent to participate in the study. If the student agreed to participate in the study, s/he received a copy of his/her written consent for his/her personal records. The survey took approximately an hour and a half to complete. After completing the survey, students received a debriefing form which explained the purpose of the study in greater detail and provided contact information of the primary investigator should they have any further questions about the study. Research assistants then submitted course credit for their participation directly through the psychology research website. 56 Chapter 3 RESULTS Demographic Information and Study Variables Descriptive characteristics about the overall sample are presented in this section and in the following tables. Concerning parenting styles (Table 1), the participants reported both mother and father authoritarian and authoritative parenting styles were present in their family of origin, but permissive parenting styles were largely absent. With regard to certain disciplining practices, the participants reported a higher use of nonviolent techniques (89%) such as time-out compared to psychological aggression (65%) such as verbal threats As for the use of some type of spanking for corporal punishment, the sample reported a high rate of both spanking with hand (78%) and spanking with an object (77%) . In contrast, pinching (45%) and shaking (38%) as a form of corporal punishment was less used. As for physically abusive behaviors, about 20% of the participants reported being violently beaten or thrown/knocked down by a parent in their family of origin Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine if significant ethnic differences were present for participant age, socioeconomic status (using the Hollingshead SES index), and participant gender (Table 2). The results of an analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed there was a significant, F(3,702) = 3.06, p = .028, ethnic difference for participant age. Specifically, Latino Americans were found to be slightly younger in 57 age than White Americans, p = .051. In addition, an ANOVA revealed a significant, F(3,702) = 28.35, p < .001, ethnic difference for SES. Specifically, White Americans reporting a higher family of origin SES than Asian and Latino Americans, F(3,702) = 28.35, p < .001, and F(3,702) = 28.35, p < .001, respectively. Also, African Americans reported a higher family of origin SES than Asian and Latino Americans, F(3,702) = 28.35, p < .001, and F(3,702) = 28.35, p < .001, respectively. A chi-square test did not show a significant ethnic difference in the distribution of participant sex. In general, women made up the majority (about 73-75%) of the participants for each ethnic group. Hypothesis 1 A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was conducted to determine if there were any significant sex (Table 3) and ethnic differences (Table 4) on scores of both mother and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting after controlling for the significant contribution of family of origin SES as a covariate (Table 6). Two separate MANCOVAs were completed, one for mother parenting styles and the other for father parenting styles. For the ANCOVAs of mother and father parenting styles, a Bonferroni correction was calculated and used to establish a more stringent cutoff point (p < .01) to identify significant main and interaction effects for these parenting styles. This decision to establish a more stringent cut-off point would also provide correction for the findings of unequal variances that were identified by the Levene’s test on both mother and father authoritative and permissive parenting. For the pairwise comparisons, the Bonferroni correction option was also selected in the SPSS program to 58 minimize the chances of reporting a false positive or negative finding in these multiple pairwise comparisons. The results for the MANCOVA on mother parenting styles showed a significant effect for the covariate family of origin SES, F(3,700) = 4.65, p = .003, and significant main effects for sex, F(3,700) = 3.49, p < .001, and ethnicity, F(3,700) = 5.02, p < .001. However, there was not a significant interaction effect of sex and ethnicity. The ANCOVA for mother authoritarian parenting did not show significant findings for the effect of the covariate family of origin SES, the main effect for participant sex or the interaction effects of participant sex and ethnicity. However, a significant main effect for ethnicity was found for mother authoritarian parenting, F(3,700) = 9.10, p < .001, η2 = .04. Specifically, Asian, African, and Latino Americans, reported higher rates of mother authoritarian parenting as children than White Americans. The ANCOVA for mother authoritative parenting did not show a significant effect for the covariate of family of origin SES, the interaction between participant sex and ethnicity, or sex and ethnicity. The ANCOVA for mother permissive parenting showed that family of origin SES had a marginally significant, F(1,702) = 6.51, p = .011, η2 = .01, effect on this parenting style. Specifically, lower family of origin SES was related to higher rates of permissive parenting as children. In contrast, neither the interaction effect of sex and ethnicity, nor the main effect for participant sex was found to be significant. A significant main effect for ethnicity was found for mother permissive parenting, F(3,700) = 4.47, p = .004, η2 = 59 .02. Specifically, Asian Americans reported higher rates of mother permissive parenting as children than African Americans. For father parenting styles, a MANCOVA was completed to test for sex and ethnic differences on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting after controlling for the significant contribution of family of origin SES as a covariate. The results for the MANCOVA on father parenting styles showed a significant effect for the covariate of family of origin SES, F(3,700) = 6.84, p < .001, and significant main effects for ethnicity, F(3,700) = 4.45, p < .001, and sex, F(3,700) = 4.16, p < .001. However, there was not a significant interaction effect for sex and ethnicity. The ANCOVA for father authoritarian parenting did not show significant findings related to family of origin SES, the interaction of participant sex and ethnicity, or participant sex. However, a significant, F(3,676) = 7.10, p < .001, η2 = .03, main effect for ethnicity was found for father authoritarian parenting. Specifically, Asian, African, and Latino Americans reported higher rates of father authoritarian parenting as children than White Americans. The ANCOVA for father authoritative parenting did not show a significant effect for the covariate family of origin SES, the interaction of sex and ethnicity, or a main effect for sex or ethnicity. The ANCOVA for father permissive parenting did not show a significant effect for the interaction of sex and ethnicity or the main effect of sex. A significant main effect for the covariate family of origin SES was found, F(1,678) = 8.21, p = .004, η2 = .01. Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations of Mother and Father Authoritarian, Authoritative, and Permissive Parenting Styles Variables M SD Mother authoritarian 35.81 6.81 Father authoritarian 36.50 7.12 Mother authoritative 34.91 6.82 Father authoritative 33.53 7.23 Mother permissive 24.77 6.19 Father permissive 24.46 6.40 Note: Mother/father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive scores range 10-50. 60 Table 2 Descriptive Statistics per Ethnic Group for Total Sample, Participant Sex, Family of Origin Socioeconomic Status, and Age Total Sample Asian Americans African Americans Latino Americans White Americans (N = 706) (N = 242) (N = 101) (N = 203) (N = 160) Variables Family of origin M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD 37.11 15.56 34.40b 16.77 42.62a 12.95 31.95b 15.00 44.29a 11.90 20.16 1.86 20.05 1.71 20.25 1.85 19.97 1.87 20.51 2.03 SES*** Participant age Participant sex: 72.00% 67.36% 70.30% 76.35% 74.37% Female Note: Family of origin SES scores range from 3-66, participant age ranges from 17-25, and participant sex indicates the percentage of females. Means marked with superscript, a > b. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 61 Table 3 Estimated Marginal Means for Males and Females in Mother/Father Parenting Styles Males Females (N = 198) (N = 508) Variables M SE M SE Mother authoritarian parenting 36.51 0.50 35.50 0.31 Mother authoritative parenting 34.85 0.51 35.23 0.32 Mother permissive parenting 25.23 0.46 24.40 0.29 Father authoritarian parenting 36.80 0.55 36.22 0.34 Father authoritative parenting 34.22 0.56 33.30 0.35 Father permissive parenting 25.29 0.49 23.85 0.30 Note: Parenting styles scores for mother and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting range from 10-50. 62 Table 4 Estimated Marginal Means for Ethnic Groups in Mother/Father Parenting Styles Asian American African American Latino American (N = 242) (N = 101) (N = 203) White American (N = 160) Variables Mother authoritarian M SE M SE M SE M SE 36.48a 0.43 37.05a 0.67 36.35a 0.47 32.97b 0.54 34.15 0.44 36.00 0.68 34.61 0.49 35.76 0.55 25.69a 0.40 23.48b 0.61 23.60b 0.44 25.69a 0.49 parenting*** Mother authoritative parenting Mother permissive parenting*** 63 Father authoritarian 36.79a 0.46 36.79a 0.75 38.12a 0.51 33.85b 0.58 33.31 0.47 33.21 0.77 32.95 0.52 34.81 0.59 25.60a 0.41 22.87b 0.67 23.07b 0.45 25.42a 0.51 parenting*** Father authoritative parenting Father permissive parenting*** Note: Parenting styles scores for mother and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting range from 10-50. Means marked with superscript, a > b. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 64 65 Specifically, lower family of origin SES was related to higher rates of permissive parenting as children. There was a marginally significant main effect for participant sex, F(1,678) = 6.28, p = .012, η2 = .01, with males reporting marginally higher rates of father permissive parenting as children than females. A significant main effect for ethnicity was found in father permissive parenting, F(3,676) = 6.13, p < .001, η2 = .03. Specifically, Asian Americans reported marginally higher rates of father permissive parenting as children than African, or Latino Americans. In addition, White Americans reported marginally higher rates of father permissive parenting as children than African Americans. Hypotheses 2-3 A multivariate analysis of covariance was conducted to determine if there were any significant sex (Table 5) and ethnic differences (Table 6) on scores of nonphysical parent discipline (i.e., nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression) in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. For the ANCOVAs of nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression, a Bonferroni correction was calculated and used to establish a more stringent cut-off point (p < .01) to identify significant main and interaction effects for these disciplining styles. This decision to establish a more stringent cut-off point would also provide correction for the findings of unequal variances that was identified by the Levene’s test on nonviolent discipline. For the pairwise comparisons, the Bonferroni correction option was also selected in the SPSS program to minimize the chances of reporting a false positive or 66 negative finding in these multiple pairwise comparisons. The results for the MANCOVA on nonviolent discipline showed a significant effect for the covariate family of origin SES, F(3,700) = 22.12, p = .003, and significant main effects for sex, F(3,700) = 5.89, p = .003, and ethnicity, F(3,700) = 4.68, p < .001. However, there was not a significant interaction effect of sex and ethnicity. The ANCOVA results showed a significant effect for the covariate of family of origin SES, F(1,704) = 41.94, p < .001, η2 = .05, with higher family of origin SES related to higher rates of nonviolent discipline experienced as children. There was a marginally significant main effect for participant sex, F(1,704) = 6.61, p = .010, η2 = .01, with males reporting marginally higher rates of nonviolent discipline experienced as children than females. A marginally significant main effect was also found for ethnicity, F(3,702) = 3.72, p = .011, η2 = .01. Specifically, Latino Americans reported higher rates of nonviolent discipline experienced as children than Asian Americans. There was no significant finding for the interaction effect of sex and ethnicity. The ANCOVA for psychological aggression did not show a significant effect for family of origin SES, or the interaction of sex and ethnicity. There was a significant main effect for participant sex, F(1,704) = 7.24, p = .007, η2 = .01, with males reporting higher rates of psychological aggression experienced as children than females. A significant main effect was also found for ethnicity, F(3,702) = 4.50, p = .004, η2 = .02. Specifically, Asian Americans reported higher rates of psychological aggression experienced as children than White Americans. In addition, African and Latino 67 Americans reported marginally higher rates of psychological aggression experienced as children than White Americans. Latino Americans also reported marginally higher rates of psychological aggression as children than Asian Americans, F(3,702) = 3.72, p = .021. Hypothesis 4 The percentages of sex and ethnic group who experienced each corporal punishment behavior are shown in Table 9. A hierarchical logistic regression analysis was conducted to determine if there were significant sex and ethnic differences in spanking with a bare hand in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES (Table 7). In this logistic regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES were entered in the first block, participant sex was entered into the second block, and the dummy coded ethnicity variables were entered in the third block. The overall regression model was found to be significant, χ²(5, N = 706) = 17.03, p = .004, and accounted for about 2% of the variance. The covariate of family of origin SES entered in the first block was found to be marginally significant, χ²(1, N = 706) = 3.13, p = .077, and accounted for less than 1% of the variance. Specifically, participants with higher family of origin SES reported a marginally higher likelihood (1%) of spanking with a bare hand as children than participants with lower family of origin SES. When adding participant sex, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(1, N = 706) = 11.19, p = .004, and this variable accounted for about 2% of the variance. Table 5 Estimated Marginal Means for Males and Females in Nonphysical Forms of Parent Discipline Males Females (N = 198) (N = 508) Variables M SE M SE Nonviolent discipline 3.68 0.05 3.55 0.03 Psychological aggression** 3.45a 0.10 3.12b 0.06 Note: Nonviolent discipline scores range from 0-4 and psychological aggression scores range from 0-5. Means marked with superscript, a > b. *p < .05, ** p < .01, ***p < .001. 68 Table 6 Estimated Marginal Means for Ethnic Groups in Nonphysical Forms of Parent Discipline Asian American African American Latino American White American (N = 242) (N = 101) (N = 203) (N = 160) Variables Nonviolent M SE M SE M SE M SE 3.42b 0.05 3.55a,b 0.07 3.65a 0.05 3.65a 0.06 3.45a 0.09 3.26a 0.14 3.30a 0.10 2.83b 0.11 discipline** Psychological aggression** Note: Nonviolent discipline scores range from 0-4 and psychological aggression scores range from 0-5. Means marked with superscript, a > b. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 69 70 Family of origin SES remained marginally significant, Wald(1, N = 706) = 2.74, p = .098, with participants with higher family of origin SES participants reporting a marginally higher likelihood (1%) of spanking with a bare hand as children than participants with lower family of origin SES. Participant sex was found to be significant, Wald(1, N = 706) = 7.48, p = .006, with males reporting a 46% higher likelihood of experiencing spanking with a bare hand as children than females. When adding the dummy coded ethnicity variables in the third block, the model was found to be significant, χ²(5, N = 706) = 17.03, p = .004, and this variable accounted for about 2% of the variance. Participant sex was still found to be significant, Wald(1, N = 706) = 8.53, p =.003, with males reporting a 48% higher likelihood of experiencing spanking with a bare hand as children than females. The dummy coded ethnicity variables were found to be significant in predicting spanking with a bare hand. Specifically, White Americans were 40% more likely to have been spanked with a bare hand as children than Asian Americans, Wald(1, N = 706) = 4.09, p = .043. Also, Latino Americans were found to be marginally more likely (50%) to have been spanked with a bare hand as children than Asian Americans, Wald(1, N = 706) = 3.05, p = .081. A hierarchical logistic regression analysis was conducted to determine if there were significant sex and ethnic differences in spanking with an object in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES (Table 7). In this logistic regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES were entered in the first block, participant sex was entered into the second block, and the dummy coded ethnicity 71 variables were entered in the third block. The overall regression model was found to be significant, χ²(6, N = 706) = 34.62, p < .001. The covariate of family of origin SES entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding participant sex, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 706) = 10.74, p = .001 and this variable accounted for about 2% of the variance. Participant sex was found to be significant, Wald(1, N = 706) = 9.80, p = .002, with males reporting a 51% higher likelihood of experiencing spanking with an object as children than females. When adding the dummy coded ethnicity variables in the third block, the model was found to be significant, χ²(5, N = 706) = 45.39, p < .001, and this variable accounted for about 6% of the variance. Participant sex, Wald(1, N = 706) = 10.09, p = .001, was still found to be significant with males reporting a 52% higher likelihood of experiencing spanking with an object as children than females. The dummy coded ethnicity variables were found to be significant in predicting spanking with an object. Specifically, Asian, Wald(1, N = 706) = 5.01, p =.025, African, Wald(1, N = 706) = 21.14, p < .001, and Latino American students, Wald(1, N = 706) = 16.16, p < .001, were 70%, 6.46 times and 2.89 times more likely to report experiencing spanking with an object than White American students, respectively. In addition, African, Wald(1, N = 706) = 11.06, p = .001, and Latino American students, Wald(1, 706) = 4.94, p = .026, were 3.82 times and 70% more likely to report experiencing spanking with an object than Asian American students, respectively. Finally, African Americans were marginally more likely (2.24 times) to 72 report spanking with an object than Latino Americans, Wald(1, N = 706) = 3.67, p = .056. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there were significant sex and ethnic differences in pinching in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES (Table 8). In this logistic regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES was entered in the first block, participant sex was entered into the second block, and the dummy coded ethnicity variables were entered in the third block. The overall model was found to be significant, χ²(6, N=706) = 63.23, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not significant. When adding participant sex, the second model was found to be significant and this variable accounted for 1% of the variance, χ²(2, N=706) = 9.60, p = .008. Participant sex was significant, with males reporting a 40% higher likelihood of experiencing pinching as children than females, Wald(1, N = 706) = 9.00, p =.003. When adding the dummy coded ethnicity variables in the third block, the model was found to be significant and this variable accounted for 8% of the variance, χ²(5, N = 706) = 63.06, p < .001. Participant sex was still significant with males reporting a 42% higher likelihood of experiencing pinching as children than females, Wald(1, N = 706) = 9.49, p = .002. The dummy coded ethnicity variables were found to be significant in predicting pinching. Specifically, Asian, Wald(1, N = 706) = 17.86, p < .001, African, Wald(1, N = 706) = 27.68, p < .001, and Latino Americans, Wald(1, N = 706) = 42.95, p < .001, were 2.73, 4.31, and 5.09 times more likely to report experiencing pinching than White American students. In addition, 73 Latino Americans, Wald(1, N = 706) = 10.23, p = .001, were 87% more likely to report experiencing pinching than Asian Americans. Finally, African Americans were marginally more likely (60%) to report pinching than Asian Americans, Wald(1, N = 706) = 3.51, p = .061. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there were significant sex and ethnic differences in shaking in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES (Table 8). In this logistic regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES was entered in the first block, participant sex was entered into the second block, and the dummy coded ethnicity variables were entered in the third block. The overall model was found to be significant, χ²(6, N = 706) = 63.23, p < .001, and accounted for about 1% of the variance. The covariate of family of origin SES entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding participant sex, the second model was found to be significant and this variable accounted for about 2% of the variance, χ²(2, N = 706) = 7.96, p = .019. Participant sex was significant, with males reporting a 36% higher likelihood of experiencing shaking as children than females, Wald(1, N = 706) = 6.65, p =.010. When adding the dummy coded ethnicity variables in the third block, the model was found to be marginally significant and this variable accounted for 2% of the variance , χ²(5, N=706) = 10.57, p = .061. Participant sex was still significant with males reporting a 35% higher likelihood of experiencing shaking as children than females, Wald(1, N = 706) = 6.15, p =.013. However, no significant ethnic differences in shaking were found. 74 Hypothesis 5 An analysis of covariance was conducted to determine if there were significant sex (Table 10) and ethnic differences (Table 11) on scores of physically abusive behavior in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. For the ANCOVA of physically abusive behavior, a Bonferroni correction was calculated and used to establish a more stringent cut-off point (p < .01) to identify significant main and interaction effects for this parenting behavior. This decision to establish a more stringent cut-off point would also provide correction for the findings of unequal variances that was identified by the Levene’s test on physically abusive behavior. For the pairwise comparisons, the Bonferroni correction option was also selected in the SPSS program to minimize the chances of reporting a false positive or negative finding in these multiple pairwise comparisons. The ANCOVA for physically abusive behavior did not show a significant effect for family of origin SES or the interaction of sex and ethnicity. There was a significant main effect for participant sex, F(1, 704) = 24.75, p < .001, η2 = .03, with males reporting higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children than females. A significant main effect was also found for ethnicity, F(3,702) = 10.72, p < .001, η2 = .04. Specifically, Asian and African Americans reported higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children than White Americans In addition, Asian and African Americans reported marginally higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children than Latino Americans. Table 7 Logistic Regression Results with Ethnicity Predicting Spanking with or without Object Spanking Hand Variables OR 95% CI Variables Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 Spanking object OR 95% CI Step 1 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 0.00 Step 2 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Participant sex** 0.54 [0.54, 0.54] Participant sex** 0.49 [0.49, 0.50] Cox & Snell R2 Cox & Snell R2 0.02 Step 3 0.02 Step 3 Family of origin SES 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Participant sex** 0.52 [0.52, 0.52] Participant sex 0.48 [0.48, 0.48] 75 Ethnicity Baseline: Asian Americans Ethnicity Baseline: Asian Americans African 1.10 [1.09, 1.10] African** 3.82 [3.81, 3.84] Latino 1.49 [1.49, 1.50] Latino* 1.71 [1.71, 1.72] White* 1.71 [1.71, 1.72] White* 0.59 [0.59, 0.59] Ethnicity Baseline: African Americans Ethnicity Baseline: African Americans Latino 1.36 [1.36, 1.37] Latino 0.45 [0.45, 0.45] White 1.56 [1.56, 1.57] White*** 0.15 [0.15, 0.16] Ethnicity Baseline: Latino Americans White Cox & Snell R2 1.15 [1.14, 1.15] 0.02 Ethnicity Baseline: Latino Americans White*** 0.35 Cox & Snell R2 0.06 [0.35, 0.35] Note: Family of origin SES scores ranged from 3-66. Sex was coded as 1 = male, 2 = female. Asian, African, Latino, and White American ethnic groups coded 0 = Not belonging to ethnic group, 1 = Belonging to ethnic group. Spanking with hand and spanking with object coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1= Occurred. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 76 Table 8 Logistic Regression Results with Ethnicity Predicting Pinching and Shaking Pinching Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 Variables Shaking OR 95% CI 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Step 1 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 0.00 Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Participant sex 0.60 [0.60, 0.60] Participant sex 0.64 [0.64, 0.64] Cox & Snell R2 Cox & Snell R2 0.02 Step 3 0.01 Step 3 Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Participant sex 0.58 [0.58, 0.58] Participant sex 0.65 [0.65, 0.66] 77 Ethnicity Baseline group: Asian Americans Ethnicity Baseline group: Asian Americans African 1.58 [1.58, 1.59] African 1.11 [1.11, 1.12] Latino** 1.87 [1.86, 1.87] Latino 0.90 [0.90, 0.91] White*** 0.37 [0.37, 0.37] White 0.76 [0.76, 0.76] Ethnicity Baseline group: African Americans Ethnicity Baseline group: African Americans Latino 1.18 [1.18, 1.19] Latino 0.81 [0.81, 0.81] White*** 0.23 [0.23, 0.23] White 0.68 [0.68, 0.68] Ethnicity Baseline group: Latino Americans White*** 0.20 Cox & Snell R2 0.09 [0.20, 0.20] Ethnicity White Cox & Snell R2 Baseline group: Latino Americans 0.84 [0.84, 0.84] 0.02 Note: Family of origin SES scores ranged from 3-66. Sex was coded as 1 = male, 2 = female. Asian, African, Latino, and White American ethnic groups coded 0 = Not belonging to ethnic group, 1 = Belonging to ethnic group. Pinching and shaking coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1= Occurred. For these models, the Cox & Snell estimation of effect size is reported. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 78 Table 9 Percentage of Each Sex and Ethnic Group Experiencing Spanking With Bare Hand, Spanking with Object, Pinching and Shaking Asian African Latino White Variables Spanking with American American American American (N = 242) (N = 101) (N = 203) (N = 160) 74%b 76%a,b 76%a,b 83%a bare hand* Spanking with Variables Spanking with Males Females (N = 198) (N = 508) 85%a 75%b 85%a 74%b bare hand** 75% b 92% a 83%a 64%b object*** Spanking with object*** Pinching*** 44%b 55%a,b 58%a 23%c Pinching** 54%a 41%b Shaking 40% 44% 37% 34% Shaking* 46%a 35%b Note: Means marked with superscript, a > b > c. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 79 Table 10 Estimated Marginal Means for Males and Females in Physically Abusive Behavior Males Variables Physically abusive behavior*** Females (N = 198) (N = 508) M SE M SE 0.60a 0.05 0.31b 0.03 Note: Physically abusive behavior ranges from 0-2. Means marked with superscript, a > b. * p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 80 Table 11 Estimated Marginal Means for Ethnic Groups in Physically Abusive Behavior Asian American African American Latino American White American (N = 242) (N = 101) (N = 203) (N = 160) Variables Physically abusive M SE M SE M SE M SE 0.57a 0.04 0.64a,b 0.07 0.39b 0.05 0.22b 0.06 behavior*** Note: Physically abusive behavior ranges from 0-2. Means marked with superscript, a > b. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 81 82 Hypotheses 6-8 A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between mother authoritarian, authoritative, or permissive parenting and nonviolent discipline in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this multiple regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and mother authoritarian authoritative, and permissive parenting were entered on the second block. For the regression model on mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 12), the overall model was found to be significant, F(2, 701) = 19.47, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was found to be significant, F(1, 702) = 52.09, p < .001, and accounted for 7% of the variance. Specifically, participants with higher family of origin SES reported experiencing more nonviolent discipline as children than participants with lower family of origin SES, t(702) = 7.22, p < .001. When adding mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting in the second block, the model was also found to be significant, F(4, 699) = 19.47, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 3% of the variance. Family of origin SES remained significant, t(702) = 6.57, p < .001, with participants of higher family of origin SES reporting higher rates of nonviolent discipline as children than participants of lower family of origin SES. Concerning mother parenting styles, authoritative parenting was found to be significant, t(702) = 4.49, p < .001, and was related to higher reported nonviolent discipline. Also, 83 mother permissive parenting was found to be significant, t(702) = 2.61, p = .009, and was related to lower reported nonviolent discipline. A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between father authoritarian, authoritative, or permissive parenting and nonviolent discipline in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this multiple regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the regression model on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 11), the overall model was found to be significant, F(2, 677) = 18.05, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was found to be significant, F(1, 678) = 53.97, p < .001, and accounted for 7% of the variance. Specifically, participants with higher family of origin SES reported higher rates of nonviolent discipline as children than participants with lower family of origin SES, t(678) = 7.35, p < .001. When adding father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting in the second block, the second model was found to be significant, F(4, 675) = 18.05, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 3% of the variance. Family of origin SES remained significant, t(678) = 6.49, p < .001, with participants of higher family of origin SES reporting higher rates of nonviolent discipline as children than participants with lower family of origin SES. Table 12 Multiple Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Nonviolent Discipline Mother parenting styles Father parenting styles Variables B SE β Step 1 Family of origin SES*** R2 B SE β Family of origin SES*** -0.00 0.00 0.27 Step 1 0.01 0.00 0.26 R2 0.07 Step 2 0.07 Step 2 Family of origin SES*** 0.01 0.00 0.24 Family of origin SES -0.00 0.00 0.24 Authoritarian parenting 0.00 0.01 0.01 Authoritarian parenting -0.00 0.00 -0.02 Authoritative parenting*** 0.02 0.00 0.17 Authoritative parenting*** 0.02 0.00 0.15 -0.01 -0.01 -0.11 -0.01 0.01 -0.11 Permissive parenting** R2 Variables 0.10 Permissive parenting R2 0.10 Note: Family of origin SES scores ranged from 3-66, nonviolent discipline ranged from 0-4, and psychological aggression ranged from 0-5. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 84 85 With regard to father parenting styles, authoritative parenting was found to be significant, t(678) = 3.79, p < .001, and was related to higher reported nonviolent discipline. Father permissive parenting was also found to be significant, t(678) = 2.54, p = .011, and was related to lower reported nonviolent discipline. A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between mother authoritarian, authoritative, or permissive parenting and psychological aggression in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this multiple regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and mother authoritarian authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the regression model on mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 13), the overall model was found to be significant, F(2,701) = 36.72, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting, the second model was found to be significant, F(4, 699) = 28.19, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 14% of the variance. With regard to mother parenting styles, authoritarian parenting, t(702) = 7.84, p < .001, and permissive parenting, t(702) = 3.06, p = .002, were both found to be significantly related to higher reported psychological aggression. Mother authoritative parenting was also found to be significantly related to lower reported psychological aggression, t(702) = 5.55, p < .001. 86 A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between father authoritarian, authoritative, or permissive parenting and psychological aggression in childhood when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this multiple regression model, the covariate of family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and father authoritarian authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the regression model on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 12), the overall model was found to be significant, F(2,677) = 27.80, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting, the second model was found to be significant, F(4, 675) = 27.80, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 14% of the variance. Concerning father parenting styles, authoritarian parenting, t(678) = 7.17, p < .001, and permissive parenting, t(678) = 2.53, p < .001, were both found to be significantly related to higher reported psychological aggression. Father authoritative parenting was also found to be significantly related to lower reported psychological aggression, t(678) = 6.04, p < .001. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between mother authoritarian, authoritative, or permissive parenting and the corporal punishment item of spanking with a bare hand when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. Table 13 Multiple Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Psychological Aggression Mother parenting styles Father parenting styles Variables B SE β Step 1 Family of origin SES R2 B SE β -0.00 0.00 -0.06 -0.00 0.00 -0.01 Step 1 -0.01 0.00 -0.06 Family of origin SES R2 0.00 Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 -0.00 Authoritarian parenting*** 0.06 0.01 0.32 Authoritarian parenting*** 0.06 0.01 0.29 Authoritative parenting*** -0.04 0.01 -0.20 Authoritative parenting*** -0.05 0.01 -0.23 0.03 0.01 0.12 0.02 0.01 0.11 Permissive parenting** R2 Variables 0.14 Family of origin SES Permissive parenting* R2 0.10 Note: Family of origin SES scores ranged from 3-66, nonviolent discipline ranged from 0-4, and psychological aggression ranged from 0-5. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 87 88 In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the logistic regression model on mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 14), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 704) = 19.09, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was found to be marginally significant, χ²(1, N = 704) = 3.29, p = .070, and accounted for 1% of the variance. Family of origin SES was found to be marginally significant, Wald(1, N = 704) = 3.30, p = .069, with participants of higher family of origin SES reporting a 1% higher likelihood of experiencing spanking with a bare hand as children than participants with lower family of origin SES. When mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(4, N = 704) = 19.08, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 3% of the variance. Family of origin SES became significant, Wald(1, N = 704) = 4.39, p = .036, with participants of higher family of origin SES reporting a 1% higher likelihood of experiencing spanking with a bare hand as children than participants of lower family of origin SES. With regard to mother parenting styles, authoritarian parenting was found to be marginally significant, Wald(1, N = 704) = 3.39, p = .065, and authoritative parenting was found to be significant, Wald(1, N = 704) = 5.25, p = .022. Specifically, mother authoritarian parenting was related to a marginally higher likelihood (3%) of spanking with a bare 89 hand in childhood, while mother authoritative parenting was related to a lower likelihood (3%) of reported experiences of spanking with a bare hand as children A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between father authoritarian, authoritative, or permissive parenting and the corporal punishment item of spanking with a bare hand when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the logistic regression model on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 14), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 20.01, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was found to be significant, χ²(1, N = 680) = 4.09, p = .043, and accounted for 1% of the variance (Table 16). Family of origin SES was significant, Wald(1, N = 680) = 4.11, p = .04, with participant of higher family of origin SES reporting a marginally higher likelihood (1%) of spanking with a bare hand as children than participants of lower family of origin SES3. When father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 20.01, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 2% of the variance. Family of origin SES remained significant, Wald(1, N = 680) = 5.51, p = .019, with participant of higher family of origin SES reporting a 2% higher likelihood of spanking with a bare hand as children than participants of lower family of origin SES. Table 14 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Spanking with Bare Hand Mother parenting styles Father parenting styles Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 Variables OR 95% CI 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Step 1 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 0.00 Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES* 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Family of origin SES 1.02 [1.01, 1.02] Authoritarian parenting 1.03 [1.03, 1.03] Authoritarian parenting 1.04 [1.04, 1.04] Authoritative parenting* 0.97 [0.97, 0.97] Authoritative parenting 0.97 [0.97, 0.97] Permissive parenting 0.98 [0.98, 0.98] Permissive parenting 0.99 [0.99, 0.99] Cox & Snell R2 0.03 Cox & Snell R2 0.03 Note: Spanking with hand and spanking with object were coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1 = Occurred. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 90 91 Father authoritarian parenting was significantly related to a 4% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 680) = 6.02, p = .014, and father authoritative parenting was significant in relation to a marginally lower likelihood, Wald(1, N = 680) = 3.68, p = .055, (3%) of reported experiences of spanking with a bare hand as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting and the corporal punishment item of spanking with an object in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES, (Table 15). In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the logistic regression model on mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 15), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 704) = 38.22, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 704) = 38.22, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 5% of the variance. Mother authoritarian parenting was related to an 8% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 704) = 22.37, p < .001, of reported experiences of spanking with an object as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive 92 parenting and the corporal punishment item of spanking with an object in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the logistic regression model on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 15), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 31.38, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 31.38, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 5% of the variance. Father authoritarian parenting was related to a 6% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 680) = 17.69, p < .001, of reported experiences of spanking with an object as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting and the corporal punishment item of pinching in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. Table 15 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Spanking with an Object Mother parenting styles Father parenting styles Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 Variables OR 95% CI 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Step 1 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 0.00 Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Authoritarian parenting*** 1.08 [1.08, 1.08] Authoritarian parenting*** 1.06 [1.06, 1.06] Authoritative parenting 0.98 [0.98, 0.98] Authoritative parenting 0.98 [0.98, 0.98] Permissive parenting 0.99 [0.99, 0.99] Permissive parenting 0.99 [0.99, 0.99] Cox & Snell R2 0.05 Cox & Snell R2 0.05 Note: Spanking with hand and spanking with object were coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1 = Occurred. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 93 Table 16 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Pinching Mother parenting styles Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 Father parenting styles Variables OR 95% CI 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Step 1 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 0.00 Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Authoritarian parenting*** 1.09 [1.09, 1.09] Authoritarian parenting*** 1.05 [1.05, 1.05] Authoritative parenting 0.99 [0.99, 0.99] Authoritative parenting 0.98 [0.98, 0.98] Permissive parenting 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Permissive parenting 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Cox & Snell R2 0.07 Cox & Snell R2 0.04 Note: Spanking with hand and spanking with object were coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1 = Occurred. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 94 95 For the logistic regression model on mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 16), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 704) = 50.25, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant and these variables accounted for 7% of the variance, χ²(2, N = 704) = 50.25, p < .001. Mother authoritarian parenting was significantly related to a 9% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 704) = 33.73, p < .001, of reported experiences of pinching as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting and the corporal punishment item of pinching in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the logistic regression model on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 16), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 26.16, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When father authoritarian parenting was added, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 26.16, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 4% of the variance. Father authoritarian parenting was significantly related to a 5% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 680) = 13.22, p < .001, of reported experiences of pinching as 96 children. Father authoritative parenting was related to marginally lower likelihood, Wald(1, N = 680) = 3.03, p = .082, (2%) of reported experiences of pinching as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a relationship between mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting and the corporal punishment item of shaking in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the logistic regression model on mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 17), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 704) = 32.99, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 704) = 32.99, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 5% of the variance. Mother authoritarian and permissive parenting were significantly related to a 6% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 704) = 14.79, p < .001, and a 5% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 704) = 8.65, p = .003, of reported experiences of shaking as children. Mother authoritative parenting was significantly related to a 4% lower likelihood, Wald(1, N = 704) = 12.38, p < .001, of reported experiences of shaking as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a relationship between father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting Table 17 Logistic Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Shaking Mother parenting styles Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 Variables Father parenting styles OR 95% CI 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Step 1 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R2 0.00 Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES* 1.04 [1.04, 1.04] Family of origin SES 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Authoritarian parenting*** 1.06 [1.05, 1.06] Authoritarian parenting** 1.05 [1.05, 1.05] Authoritative parenting*** 0.96 [0.96, 0.96] Authoritative parenting** 0.97 [0.97, 0.97] Permissive parenting** 1.05 [1.05, 1.05] Permissive parenting** 1.04 [1.04, 1.04] Cox & Snell R2 0.05 Cox & Snell R2 0.03 Note: Spanking with hand and spanking with object were coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1 = Occurred. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 97 98 and the corporal punishment item of shaking in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the logistic regression model on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 17), the overall model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 23.55, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 680) = 23.55, p = .002, and these variables accounted for 3% of the variance. Father authoritarian and permissive parenting were significantly related to a 4% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 680) = 6.95, p = .008, and a 5% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 704) = 8.65, p = .003 of reported experiences of shaking as children. Father authoritative parenting was significantly related to a 3% lower likelihood, Wald(1, N = 704) = 6.92, p = .008, of reported experiences of shaking as children. A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted determine if there was a significant relationship between mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting and physically abusive behavior in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this multiple regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. 99 For the multiple regression model on mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 18), the overall model was found to be significant, F(4, 699) = 27.76, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When mother authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant, F(4, 699) = 27.76, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 14% of the variance. Mother authoritarian, t(702) = 8.71, p < .001 and permissive parenting, t(702) = 6.11, p < .001, were significantly related to higher reported rates of physically abusive behavior as children. Mother authoritative parenting, t(702) = 4.56, p < .001, was significantly related to lower reported rates of physically abusive behavior as children. A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted determine if there was a significant relationship between father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting and physically abusive behavior in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In this multiple regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting entered in the second block. For the multiple regression model on father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting (Table 18), the overall model was found to be significant, F(4, 675) = 21.90, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. Table 18 Multiple Regression Results with Mother and Father Parenting Styles Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior Mother parenting styles Father parenting styles Variables B SE β Step 1 Family of origin SES R² Variables B SE β -0.00 0.00 -0.02 Step 1 -0.00 0.00 -0.01 0.00 Family of origin SES R² Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.05 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.04 Authoritarian parenting*** 0.03 0.00 0.35 Authoritarian parenting*** 0.03 0.00 0.33 Authoritative parenting*** -0.02 0.00 -0.17 Authoritative parenting** -0.01 0.00 -0.13 0.03 0.00 0.25 Permissive parenting*** 0.03 0.00 0.28 Permissive parenting*** R² 0.14 R² 0.05 Note: Physically abusive behavior ranged from 0-2. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 100 101 When father authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting were added, the second model was found to be significant, F(2, 677) = 19.46, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 12% of the variance. Father authoritarian, t(678) = 7.93, p < .001, and permissive parenting, t(678) = 6.53, p < .001, were significantly related to higher reported physically abusive behavior. Father authoritative parenting, t(678) = 3.44, p = .001, was significantly related to lower reported physically abusive behavior. Hypothesis 9 A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES (Table 19). In this multiple regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and nonviolent discipline was entered into the second block. The overall model was found to be significant, F(2, 703) = 10.54, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding nonviolent discipline, the second model was found to be significant, F(2, 703) = 10.54, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 3% of the variance. Family of origin SES became significant, t(704) = 2.63, p <.009, with participants of lower family of origin SES reporting higher rates of psychological aggression as children than participants of higher family of origin SES. Nonviolent discipline, t(704) = 4.23, p < .001, was significantly related to higher reported psychological aggression as children. 102 Hypothesis 10 In the previous hypothesis nonviolent discipline was found to be positively related to psychological aggression. Therefore, in the following analyses, the variable nonviolent discipline was included to control for its significance. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between psychological aggression, and spanking with a bare hand in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES, and nonviolent discipline (Table 20). In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, nonviolent discipline was entered into the second block, and psychological aggression were entered into the third block. The overall model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 111.87, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was found to be marginally significant, χ²(1, N = 706) = 3.13, p = .077, and accounted for less than 1% of the variance. Specifically, participants of higher family of origin SES reported a 1% marginally higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 3.14, p = .077, of reported experiences of spanking with a bare hand as children than participants of lower family of origin SES. When adding nonviolent discipline, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 706) = 33.52, p < .001, and these variables accounted for about 5% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline was significantly related to a 89% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 30.34, p < .001, of reported experiences of 103 spanking with a bare hand as children. When adding psychological aggression, the third model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 111.87, p < .001, and accounted for 15% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline remained significant. In addition, psychological aggression was related to a 87% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 68.16, p < .001, of reported experiences of spanking with a bare hand as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between psychological aggression, and spanking with an object in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES, and nonviolent discipline (Table 20). In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, nonviolent discipline entered into the second block, and psychological aggression was entered into the third block. The overall model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 135.48, p < .001. The covariate entered into the first block was not found to be significant. When adding nonviolent discipline, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 706) = 27.29, p < .001, and these variables accounted for about 4% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline was significantly related to a 83% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 27.40, p < .001, of reported experiences of and these variables accounted for 18% of the variance. When adding psychological aggression, the third model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 135.48, p < .001, and accounted for 18% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline remained significant. In addition, psychological aggression was 104 related to a higher likelihood (2.11 times), Wald(1, N = 706) = 88.37, p < .001, of reported experiences of spanking with an object as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between psychological aggression, and pinching in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES, and nonviolent discipline (Table 21). In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, nonviolent discipline was entered into the second block, and psychological aggression entered into the third block. The overall model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 109.53, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding nonviolent discipline, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 30.28, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 4% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline was significantly related to an 90% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 26.24, p < .001, of reported experiences of pinching as children. When adding psychological aggression, the third model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 109.53, p < .001, and accounted for 14% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline remained significant. In addition, psychological aggression was significantly related to a 71% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 69.69, p < .001, of reported experiences of pinching as children. A hierarchical logistic regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between psychological aggression, and shaking in childhood, 105 when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES , and nonviolent discipline (Table 21). In this logistic regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, nonviolent discipline was entered into the second block, and psychological aggression were entered into the third block. The overall model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 53.69, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding nonviolent discipline, the second model was found to be significant, χ²(2, N = 706) = 13.06, p = .001, and these variables accounted for 2% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline was significantly related to a 48% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 10.83, p = .001, of reported experiences of shaking as children. When adding psychological aggression, the third model was found to be significant, χ²(3, N = 706) = 53.69, p < .001, and accounted for 7% of the variance. Nonviolent discipline remained significant. In addition, psychological aggression was significantly related to a 46% higher likelihood, Wald(1, N = 706) = 37.62, p = .001, of reported experiences of shaking as children. A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine if there was a significant relationship between nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression, and physically abusive behavior in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES (Table 22). In this multiple regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, and variables nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression were entered into the second block. Table 19 Multiple Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline Predicting Psychological Aggression Psychological aggression Variables B SE β -0.01 0.00 -0.06 Step 1 Family of origin SES R² 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES** -0.01 0.00 -0.10 Nonviolent discipline*** 0.31 0.07 0.17 R² 0.03 Note: Psychological aggression scores ranged from 0-5. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 106 Table 20 Logistic Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline and Psychological Aggression Predicting Spanking with or without Object Spanking hand Spanking object Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R² Variables OR 95% CI 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Step 1 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] 0.00 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R² Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Family of origin SES 0.99 [0.99, 0.99] Nonviolent discipline*** 1.89 [1.89, 1.89] Nonviolent discipline*** 1.83 [1.83, 1.83] Cox & Snell R² 0.05 Cox & Snell R² 0.04 107 Step 3 Step 3 Family of origin SES 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Nonviolent discipline*** 1.71 [1.70, 1.71] Nonviolent discipline*** 1.64 [1.64, 1.64] Psychological aggression*** 1.87 [1.87, 1.88] Psychological aggression*** 2.11 [2.11, 2.11] Cox & Snell R² 0.15 Cox & Snell R² 0.18 Note: Spanking with hand and spanking with object were coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1 = Occurred. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 108 Table 21 Logistic Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline and Psychological Aggression Predicting Pinching and Shaking Pinching Shaking Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R² Variables OR 95% CI Step 1 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] 0.00 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R² Step 2 1.01 [1.01, 1.01] 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES* 0.99 [0.99, 0.99] Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Nonviolent discipline*** 1.70 [1.69, 1.70] Nonviolent discipline** 1.48 [1.47, 1.48] Cox & Snell R² 0.04 Cox & Snell R² 0.02 109 Step 3 Step 3 Family of origin SES* 0.99 [0.99, 0.99] Family of origin SES 1.00 [1.00, 1.00] Nonviolent discipline*** 1.85 [1.85, 1.85] Nonviolent discipline* 1.34 [1.34, 1.35] Psychological aggression*** 1.71 [1.71, 1.72] Psychological aggression*** 1.46 [1.46, 1.46] Cox & Snell R² 0.14 Cox & Snell R² 0.07 Note: Pinching and shaking were coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1 = Occurred. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 110 Table 22 Multiple Regression Results with Nonviolent Discipline and Psychological Aggression Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior Physically abusive behavior Variables B SE β -0.00 0.00 -0.02 Step 1 Family of origin SES R² 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES -0.00 0.00 0.01 Nonviolent discipline -0.02 0.03 -0.02 Psychological aggression*** 0.20 0.02 0.40 R² 0.16 Note: Physically abusive behavior scores ranged from 0-2. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 111 112 The overall model was found to be significant, F(3,702) = 43.98, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression, the second model was found to be significant and these variables accounted for 16% of the variance, F(3,702) = 43.98, p < .001. Psychological aggression was significantly, t(701) = 11.40, p <.001, related to a higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. Hypothesis 11 A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine a relationship between the four corporal punishment behaviors and physically abusive behavior in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES, nonviolent discipline, and psychological aggression, (Table 23). In this multiple regression model, the covariate family of origin SES was entered in the first block, nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression were entered in the second block, and spanking with a bare hand, spanking with an object, pinching, and shaking were entered into the third block. The overall model was found to be significant, F(7, 698) = 32.58, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression, the second model was found to be significant, F(3, 702) = 43.98, p < .001, and accounted for 16% of the variance. Psychological aggression was significantly, t(704) = 11.40, p < .001, related to higher reported experiences of physically abusive behavior as children. When adding the four 113 corporal punishment behaviors, the third model was found to be significant and these variables accounted for 25% of the variance, F(7, 698) = 32.19, p < .001. Psychological aggression remained significantly related, t(704) = 7.20, p < .001, to higher reported experiences of physically abusive behavior as children. Spanking with an object t(704) = 2.44, p = .015, pinching t(704) = 3.08, p = .002, shaking t(704) = 7.43, p < .001, but not spanking with a bare hand were significantly related to higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. Hypothesis 12 Considering the previous eleven hypotheses and their results, a change was made in hypotheses twelve and thirteen. Specifically, mother/father permissive parenting was removed from these hypotheses because this variable was not consistently significant in relation to the disciplining methods and this parenting style was not significantly related to the dependent variable, physically abusive behavior. The corporal punishment item of spanking with a bare hand was also removed from the analyses because when controlling for the other corporal punishment items, spanking with a bare hand was not significantly related to physically abusive behavior. Finally, some of the following analyses should be interpreted with caution because several of the independent variables were highly correlated. For hypothesis twelve, a hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to determine a significant relationship between the three disciplining types (nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, corporal punishment) and physically abusive 114 behavior in childhood, when controlling for the significant contributions of family of origin SES. In these multiple regression models, the covariate family of origin SES was entered into the first block, mother authoritarian, father authoritarian, mother authoritative or father authoritative parenting styles entered in the second block, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and the three corporal punishment items entered into the third block, participant sex entered into the fourth block, and ethnicity entered into the fifth block. For the first analysis, mother authoritarian parenting was included as the parenting style (Table 24). The overall model was found to be significant, F(11, 692) = 25.71, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding mother authoritarian parenting, the second model was found to be significant and these variables accounted for 7% of the variance, F(2, 701) = 28.20, p < .001. Mother authoritarian parenting, t(702) = 7.50, p < .001, was significantly related to higher reported rates of physically abusive behavior. When nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, spanking with object, pinching, and shaking were added, the third model was found to be significant, F(7, 696) = 34.93, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 19% of the variance. Mother authoritarian parenting remained significant, t(704) = 3.63, p < .001. Psychological aggression t(702) = 6.39, p < .001, pinching t(702) = 2.31, p = .021, and shaking t(702) = 7.44, p < .001, were found to be significant and spanking with object t(702) = 1.93, p = .054 was found to be marginally significant in relation to higher reported rates of physically abusive behavior. When adding participant sex, the fourth 115 model was found to be significant, F(8,695) = 33.15, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 2% of the variance. Mother authoritarian parenting t(702) = 6.63, p < .001, psychological aggression t(702) = 6.39, p < .001, pinching t(702) = 2.10, p = .036, and shaking t(702) = 7.29, p < .001, remained significant and spanking with an object remained marginally significant, t(702) = 1.70, p = .090. Nonviolent discipline became significant, t(702) = 1.70, p = .090. Significant sex differences remained after removing the impact of mother authoritarian parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and the three corporal punishment items, with males reporting higher reported rates of physically abusive behavior as children than females, t(702) = 3.94, p < .001. When adding ethnicity, the fifth model was found to be significant, F(11, 692) = 25.71, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 1% of the variance. Mother authoritarian parenting t(702) = 3.38, p = .001, psychological aggression t(704) = 6.11, p < .001, pinching t(702) = 2.15, p = .032, and shaking t(702) = 7.19, p < .001, remained significant. Significant sex differences remained, t(702) = 3.66, p < .001. Finally, some significant ethnic differences remained after removing the impact of mother authoritarian parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, the three corporal punishment items, and sex. Specifically, Asian Americans reported higher rates of physically abusive behavior of children than Latino, t(700) = 3.10, p = .002, and White Americans, t(700) = 2.24, p = .025. In addition, African Americans reported higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children than Latino Americans, t(700) = 2.49, p = .013 and 116 marginally higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children than White Americans, t(700) = 1.91, p = .056. For the second analysis, father authoritarian parenting was included as the parenting style (Table 24). The overall model was found to be significant, F(11, 668) = 22.97, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding father authoritarian parenting, the second model was found to be significant, F(2,701) = 19.45, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 5% of the variance. Father authoritarian parenting, t(678) = 6.22, p < .001, was significantly related to higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children. When nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, spanking with object, pinching, and shaking were added, the third model was found to be significant, F(7, 672) = 31.11, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 20% of the variance. Father authoritarian parenting remained significant, F(1, 678) = 2.69, p = .007. Psychological aggression t(678) = 6.15, p < .001, spanking with object t(678) = 1.98, p = .048, pinching t(678) = 2.56, p = .011, and shaking t(678) = 7.14, p < .001 were significantly related to higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. When adding participant sex, the fourth block was found to be significant, F(8, 671) = 29.34, p < .001, and accounted for 1% of the variance. Father authoritarian parenting remained significant, F(1, 678) = 2.72, p = .007. Psychological aggression t(678) = 6.17, p < .001, pinching t(678) = 2.39, p = .017, and shaking t(678) = 6.98, p < .001 remained significant. Table 23 Multiple Regression Results with Corporal Punishment Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior Variables Physically abusive behavior B SE β -0.00 0.00 -0.02 Step 1 Family of origin SES Cox & Snell R² 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.01 Nonviolent discipline -0.02 0.03 -0.02 0.20 0.02 .40 Psychological aggression*** Cox & Snell R² 0.16 117 Step 3 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.01 Nonviolent discipline -0.06 0.03 -0.07 0.13 0.02 0.28 -0.05 0.06 -0.03 Spanking object* 0.16 0.06 0.10 Pinching** 0.15 0.05 0.11 Shaking*** 0.35 0.05 0.26 Cox & Snell R² 0.25 Psychological aggression*** Spanking hand Note: Physically abusive behavior scores ranged from 0-2. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 118 119 Spanking with object t(678) = 1.75, p = .081, and nonviolent discipline, t(678) = 1.73, p = .085, became marginally significant, with nonviolent discipline related to marginally lower rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. Significant sex differences, t(678) = 3.61, p < .001, remained after removing the impact of father authoritarian parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and the three corporal punishment items, with males reporting higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children than females. When adding ethnicity, the fifth model was found to be significant, F(11, 668) = 22.97, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 1% of the variance. Father authoritarian parenting remained significant, F(1, 678) = 2.81, p = .005. Psychological aggression t(678) = 6.17, p < .001, pinching t(678) = 2.39, p = .017, and shaking t(678) = 6.98, p < .001 remained significant and spanking with an object t(678) = 1.75, p = .081, remained marginally significant. Significant sex differences remained, t(678) = 3.61, p < .001. Finally, some ethnic differences remained after removing the impact of father authoritarian parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, the three corporal punishment items, and participant sex. Specifically, Asian Americans reported higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children than Latino t(676) = 3.32, p = .001 or White Americans, t(678) = 2.59, p = .010. In addition, African Americans, t(676) = 1.91, p = .057, reported marginally higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children than Latino Americans. 120 For the third analysis, mother authoritative parenting was included as the parenting style (Table 25). The overall model was found to be significant, F(11, 692) = 24.82, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding mother authoritative parenting, the second model was found to be significant, F(2, 701) = 12.83, p < .001, and this variable accounted for 4% of the variance. Mother authoritative parenting was significantly related to lower rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children, t(702) = 5.05, p < .001. When nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, spanking with object, pinching, and shaking were added, the third model was found to be significant, F(7, 696) = 33.09, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 21% of the variance. Mother authoritative parenting became marginally significant, t(702) = 1.85, p = .064. Psychological aggression t(702) = 6.60, p < .001, spanking with object t(702) = 2.24, p = .025 pinching t(702) = 2.86, p = .004, and shaking t(702) = 7.33, p < .001, were significantly related to higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. When adding participant sex, the fourth model was found to be significant, F(8, 696) = 31.55, p < .001, and this variable accounted for 2% of the variance. Mother authoritative parenting remained marginally significant, t(702) = 1.93, p = .054. Psychological aggression t(702) = 6.57, p < .001, spanking with object t(702) = 2.00, p = .046, pinching t(702) = 2.64, p = .008, and shaking t(702) = 7.16, p < .001 remained significant. Table 24 Multiple Regression Results with Mother/Father Authoritarian Parenting and Parent Discipline Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior Mother authoritarian Father authoritarian Variables parenting B SE Variables β Step 1 Family of origin SES R² B SE β -0.00 0.00 -0.02 Step 1 -0.00 0.00 -0.01 0.00 Family of origin SES R² Step 2 Step 2 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.01 Family of origin SES -0.00 0.00 -0.01 Mother authoritarian 0.01 0.00 0.01 Father authoritarian 0.02 0.00 0.23 parenting*** R² parenting parenting*** 0.07 R² 0.03 121 Step 3 Step 3 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.02 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 Mother authoritarian 0.01 0.00 0.13 Father authoritarian 0.01 0.00 -0.05 0.03 parenting*** Nonviolent discipline R² parenting** -0.05 0.03 -0.05 Psychological aggression*** 0.12 0.02 0.25 Psychological aggression*** 0.12 0.02 Spanking object 0.11 0.06 0.07 Spanking object* 0.10 0.06 Pinching* 0.11 0.05 0.08 Pinching* 0.12 0.05 Shaking*** 0.35 0.05 0.25 Shaking*** 0.34 0.05 0.26 Nonviolent discipline R² 0.26 122 Step 4 Step 4 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.01 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.00 Mother authoritarian 0.01 0.00 0.13 Father authoritarian 0.01 0.00 0.10 -0.05 0.03 -0.06 parenting*** Nonviolent discipline parenting** -0.05 0.03 -0.06 Psychological aggression*** 0.12 0.02 0.24 Psychological aggression*** 0.11 0.02 0.24 Spanking object 0.10 0.06 0.60 Spanking object 0.10 0.06 0.07 Pinching* 0.10 0.05 0.08 Pinching** 0.12 0.05 0.09 Shaking*** 0.34 0.05 0.25 Shaking*** 0.33 0.05 0.24 -0.19 0.05 -0.13 -0.18 0.05 -0.12 Participant sex*** R² 0.28 Nonviolent discipline Participant sex*** R² 123 Step 5 Step 5 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.00 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.00 Mother authoritarian 0.01 0.00 0.12 Father authoritarian 0.01 0.00 0.10 -0.04 0.03 -0.04 parenting** parenting** Nonviolent discipline -0.04 0.03 -0.04 Psychological aggression*** 0.11 0.02 0.23 Psychological aggression*** 0.11 0.02 0.23 Spanking object 0.10 0.06 0.06 Spanking object 0.10 0.06 0.07 Pinching* 0.11 0.05 0.08 Pinching** 0.12 0.05 0.09 Shaking*** 0.33 0.05 0.24 Shaking*** 0.32 0.05 0.24 -0.18 0.05 -0.12 -0.16 0.05 -0.11 0.07 -0.02 Participant sex*** Ethnicity Nonviolent discipline Participant sex*** Ethnicity Baseline: Asian Americans African 0.01 Baseline: Asian Americans 0.07 0.00 African -0.04 124 Latino** -0.17 0.06 -0.12 Latino** -0.19 0.06 -0.13 White* -0.14 0.06 -0.09 White* -0.16 0.06 -0.10 Baseline: African Americans Baseline: African Americans Latino* -0.18 0.07 -0.12 Latino -0.14 0.07 -0.10 White -0.14 0.08 -0.09 White -0.12 0.08 -0.08 0.07 0.07 Baseline: Latino Americans White R² 0.03 0.29 Baseline: Latino Americans 0.07 0.02 White R² 0.02 0.27 Note: Family of origin SES scores ranged from 3-66. Mother/father parenting styles = 10-50, nonviolent discipline = 0-4, psychological aggression = 0-5, corporal punishment coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1= Occurred. Asian, African, Latino, and White American ethnic groups coded 0 = Not belonging to ethnic group, 1 = Belonging to ethnic group. Physically abusive behavior scores ranged from 0-2. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 125 126 Significant sex differences, t(702) = 3.98, p < .001, remained after removing the impact of mother authoritative parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and the three corporal punishment items, with males reporting higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children than females. When adding ethnicity, the fifth model was significant, F(11, 692) = 24.82, p < .001, and this variable accounted for 1% of the variance. Mother authoritative parenting became significant, t(702) = 2.09, p = .037. Psychological aggression t(702) = 6.24, p < .001, pinching t(702) = 2.53, p = .012, and shaking t(702) = 7.06, p < .001 remained significant and spanking with object t(702) = 1.83, p = .067 became marginally significant. Significant sex differences remained, t(702) = 3.70, p < .001. Finally, some ethnic differences remained after removing the impact of mother authoritative parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, the three corporal punishment items, and participant sex. Specifically, Asian Americans reported higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children than Latino t(700) = 3.09, p = .002, or White Americans, t(700) = 2.63, p = .009. In addition, African Americans reported higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children than Latino t(700) = 2.69, p = .007, or White Americans, t(700) = 2.43, p = .009. For the fourth analysis, father authoritative parenting was included as the parenting style (Table 25). The overall model was found to be significant, F(11, 668) = 22.01, p < .001. The covariate entered in the first block was not found to be significant. When adding father authoritative parenting, the second model was found to be significant, F(2, 677) = 5.43, p < .001, and this variable accounted for 2% of the 127 variance. Father authoritative parenting, t(678) = 3.27, p < .001, was significantly related to lower rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. When nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, spanking with object, pinching, and shaking were added, the third model was found to be significant, F(7, 672) = 29.76, p < .001, and these variables accounted for 22% of the variance. Psychological aggression t(678) = 6.68, p < .001, spanking with object t(680) = 2.23, p = .028, pinching t(678) = 2.76, p = .006, and shaking t(678) = 7.22, p < .001, were significantly related to higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. In addition, nonviolent discipline was marginally significant, t(678) = 1.76, p = .079, in relation to lower rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. When adding participant sex, the fourth model was found to be significant, F(8, 671) = 28.12, p < .001, and this variable accounted for 1% of the variance. Psychological aggression t(678) = 6.63, p < .001, spanking with object t(678) = 1.97, p = .049, pinching t(678) = 2.58, p = .010, and shaking t(678) = 7.04, p < .001, remained significant, and nonviolent discipline remained marginally significant, t(678) = 1.88, p = .061. Significant sex differences, t(678) = 3.60, p < .001, remained after removing the impact of mother authoritative parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and the three corporal punishment items, with males reporting higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children than females. When adding ethnicity, the fifth model was significant, F(11, 668) = 22.01, p < .001, and this variable accounted for 2% of the variance. Psychological aggression t(678) = 6.17, p < .001, pinching t(678) = 2.47, p = .014, and shaking t(678) = 6.96, p < .001 remained 128 significantly related to higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children. Spanking with an object remained marginally significant in relation to higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children, t(678) = 1.92, p = .055. Significant sex differences remained, t(678) = 3.31, p = .001. Finally, some ethnic differences remained after removing the impact of father authoritative parenting, nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, the three corporal punishment items, and sex. Specifically, Asian Americans reported higher rates of reported physically abusive behavior as children than Latino t(676) = 3.09, p = .002 or White Americans, t(676) = 2.82, p = .005. African Americans reported marginally higher rates of physically abusive behavior as children than Latino t(676) = 1.73, p = .085 or White Americans, t(676) = 1.69, p = .091. Table 25 Multiple Regression Results with Mother/Father Authoritative Parenting and Parent Discipline Predicting Physically Abusive Behavior Mother authoritative Father authoritative Variables parenting B SE Variables β Step 1 Family of origin SES R² B SE β -0.00 0.00 -0.02 -0.00 0.00 -0.01 0.02 0.00 0.23 Step 1 -0.00 0.00 -0.02 0.00 Family of origin SES R² Step 2 0.00 Step 2 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.00 Mother authoritative 0.02 0.00 -0.19 Family of origin SES Father authoritative parenting*** R² parenting parenting*** 0.04 R² 0.02 129 Step 3 Step 3 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.01 Family of origin SES Mother authoritative -0.01 0.00 0.07 Father authoritative parenting Nonviolent discipline R² 0.00 0.00 0.01 -0.01 0.00 -0.10 -0.06 0.03 -0.07 parenting -0.04 0.03 -0.05 Psychological aggression*** 0.12 0.02 0.26 Psychological aggression*** 0.13 0.02 0.27 Spanking object* 0.13 0.06 0.08 Spanking object* 0.13 0.06 0.08 Pinching** 0.14 0.05 0.10 Pinching** 0.14 0.05 0.10 Shaking*** 0.34 0.05 0.25 Shaking*** 0.35 0.05 0.26 0.25 Nonviolent discipline R² 0.23 130 Step 4 Step 4 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.01 Family of origin SES Mother authoritative -0.01 0.00 0.13 Father authoritative parenting* Nonviolent discipline 0.00 0.00 0.00 -0.00 0.00 -0.10 -0.06 0.03 -0.07 parenting -0.03 0.03 -0.03 Psychological aggression*** 0.11 0.02 0.24 Psychological aggression*** 0.12 0.02 0.26 Spanking object 0.11 0.06 0.07 Spanking object* 0.12 0.06 0.07 Pinching* 0.12 0.05 0.09 Pinching** 0.13 0.05 0.10 Shaking*** 0.33 0.05 0.24 Shaking*** 0.34 0.05 0.25 -0.18 0.05 -0.12 -0.18 0.05 -0.12 Participant sex*** R² 0.27 Nonviolent discipline Participant sex*** R² 0.25 131 Step 5 Step 5 Family of origin SES 0.00 0.00 0.00 Family of origin SES Mother authoritative 0.01 0.00 0.12 Father authoritative parenting** 0.00 0.00 0.01 -0.01 0.00 -0.01 -0.04 0.03 -0.05 parenting Nonviolent discipline -0.04 0.03 -0.04 Psychological aggression*** 0.11 0.02 0.23 Psychological aggression*** 0.12 0.02 0.25 Spanking object 0.10 0.06 0.60 Spanking object 0.11 0.06 0.07 Pinching* 0.11 0.05 0.08 Pinching* 0.12 0.05 0.09 Shaking*** 0.33 0.05 0.24 Shaking*** 0.33 0.05 0.24 -0.18 0.05 -0.12 -0.17 0.05 -0.11 0.07 -0.02 Participant sex*** Ethnicity Nonviolent discipline Participant sex*** Ethnicity Baseline: Asian Americans African 0.02 Baseline: Asian Americans 0.07 0.01 African -0.04 132 Latino** -0.17 0.06 -0.12 Latino** -0.17 0.06 -0.12 White** -0.16 0.06 -0.10 White** -0.18 0.06 -0.11 Baseline: African Americans Baseline: African Americans Latino** -0.19 0.07 -0.13 Latino -0.13 0.08 -0.09 White* -0.18 0.08 -0.12 White -0.13 0.08 -0.09 0.01 -0.06 Baseline: Latino Americans White R² 0.01 0.28 Baseline: Latino Americans 0.07 0.01 White R² -0.00 0.27 Note: Family of origin SES scores ranged from 3-66. Mother/father parenting styles = 10-50, nonviolent discipline = 0-4, psychological aggression = 0-5, corporal punishment coded as 0 = Never occurred, 1= Occurred. Asian, African, Latino, and White American ethnic groups coded 0 = Not belonging to ethnic group, 1 = Belonging to ethnic group. Physically abusive behavior scores ranged from 0-2. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. 133 134 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION Summary and Interpretation of Findings in the Context of Previous Literature The results of the present study partially supported the proposed hypotheses. In the first hypothesis, significant sex and ethnic differences were found in some parenting styles. However, these differences did not completely reflect what was proposed in the hypotheses. First, sex differences were only found in reported father permissive parenting, with males reporting higher father permissive parenting. In addition, while Asian and African Americans reported higher mother and father authoritarian parenting than White Americans, Latino Americans also reported higher authoritarian parenting than White Americans. Asian and Latino Americans unexpectedly reported significantly higher permissive parenting as compared with African and Latino Americans and no significant ethnic differences were found in mother and father authoritative parenting. Considering the range of scores for parenting styles, all of the participants, regardless of ethnicity, tended to agree that their parents were more authoritarian and authoritative, but disagreed that their parents were permissive. While the results of this first analysis did not fully support the first hypotheses, these results do support previous literature that states that boys may report higher permissive parenting than girls. However, both male and female participants reported that their parents did not use permissive parenting. In addition, these results support previous literature that has shown that ethnic minority groups report more authoritarian parenting 135 than White Americans, but that they also prefer an authoritative parenting approach. Finally, little information was found about permissive parenting style in the literature. However, the results of this study show that it is a parenting style that is not often used across all ethnic groups. When exploring the second and third hypotheses, significant sex and ethnic differences in nonphysical forms of discipline were found. First, males reported more nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression experienced as children than females. The expectation that Latino and White Americans would report the highest levels of nonviolent disciple experienced as children was supported. However, African Americans reported a similar level of nonviolent discipline to these two ethnic groups and Asian Americans reported less nonviolent discipline experienced as children than Latino and White Americans. However, it is important to note that all four ethnic groups reported that they had experienced almost all of the four nonviolent discipline items as children. Finally, the three ethnic minority groups reported more psychological aggression experienced as children than White Americans. Specifically, ethnic minority groups reported experiencing an average of about three out of six types of psychological aggression as children, while White Americans reported less than three types of psychological aggression as children. These findings were slightly different from what was expected as described in the hypotheses. However, these results were supportive of previous literature showing that boys report with higher levels of disciplining of all kinds when compared with girls. In 136 addition, some previous literature has shown that African, Latino, and White Americans parents use similar and high levels of nonviolent discipline. Research on Asian American parental attitudes had suggested a preference for more traditional Asian disciplining practices than Western disciplining techniques, which was supported by the results of this study. Finally, previous research on ethnic differences in psychological aggression has often been contrary to one another. The current study supports literature that suggests that ethnic minority parents may use more psychological aggression with their children than White American parents. The analyses conducted to explore the fourth hypothesis revealed significant ethnic differences on the four corporal punishment items. Interestingly, White Americans were more likely to report spanking with a bare hand as children than Asian Americans. This was directly contrary to the proposed hypothesis because it was expected that Asian Americans would be more likely to report experiencing all four of corporal punishment items than White Americans. In contrast, White Americans reported more spanking with an object and pinching as a child than the three ethnic minority groups. Significant ethnic differences for the ethnic minority groups were also reported on these two corporal punishment items. Specifically, Latino Americans were the most likely to report spanking with an object as children, followed by African Americans, and Asian Americans were the least likely to report spanking with an object for these three ethnic minority groups. Latino Americans were also the most likely to report pinching as children, while Asian Americans were the least likely to report 137 pinching as children. Finally, no significant ethnic differences were found in the final corporal punishment item of shaking for the four ethnic groups. While previous literature has not examined ethnic differences in individual corporal punishment items, the results of this study mostly supported previous literature examining ethnic differences in corporal punishment. The only discrepant finding regarding corporal punishment was between Asian and White Americans on spanking with hand. This finding shows that not all corporal punishment items may be equally acceptable in different ethnic groups or cultures. It may also be that certain corporal punishment items, such as shaking, are equally but infrequently used across these different ethnic groups. The results lend supportive evidence that shaking was not commonly used as a disciplining technique for these four ethnic groups. These results show some support for examining corporal punishment items separately in the future. These sex and ethnic differences in parenting styles and parent discipline experienced may be the result of cultural beliefs about how boys and girls should be treated and disciplined. There may also be inter-group cultural differences regarding the frequency or types of parenting styles and parent discipline used. Some trends show that boys may experience more of all three parenting styles and parent disciplining techniques than girls. This may be due to cultural beliefs that boys need more demandingness and discipline to control their behaviors than girls do. Similarly, the ethnic differences in parenting styles and parent discipline may reveal the trend that some groups may believe 138 that they must be comparatively more demanding and use stricter discipline to raise successful and well-behaved children. In the analyses conducted for the fifth hypothesis, significant sex and ethnic differences in physically abusive behavior were found. Male participants reported experiencing higher physically abusive behavior, defined as being beat up or being hit with a fist/kicked, than females, which supported the findings in previous literature. Significant ethnic differences in physically abusive behaviors experienced as children were somewhat different from what was expected. While Asian and African Americans reported experiencing more physically abusive behaviors as children than White Americans, the reported ethnic differences between African, Asian, and Latino Americans were less prominent for these physically abusive behaviors. Specifically, both African and Asian Americans reported more experiences of physically abusive behavior as children, a higher than the entire sample average rate (20%). In contrast, Latino Americans reported a below average rate of only 17% who had experienced some type of physically abusive behavior as children. Finally, White Americans reported the lowest rate of physical abuse experienced as children (10%), which was far lower than the three ethnic minority groups. These results support previous literature that has shown that ethnic minority families tend to be overrepresented in suspected child physical abuse cases and community epidemiological studies. In addition, as described in the previous section, sex and ethnic differences in parenting styles and parent discipline may indirectly 139 lead to situations in which strict parenting styles and discipline may escalate into more severe behaviors such as physical abuse. In the next set of hypotheses, the possible relationships between parenting style, parent discipline, and physically abusive behavior were explored. As shown in Table 26, a clear pattern is emerging regarding the relationship between parenting styles and parent discipline. As noted earlier, authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive parenting styles are based on the dimensions of parental warmth and demandingness. These results suggest that the high levels of demandingness in authoritarian parenting may lead to stricter parenting practices such as psychological aggression, corporal punishment, and physically abusive behavior. Conversely, these results also suggest that lack of demandingness, as in permissive parenting, may also eventually lead to stricter parenting practices such as psychological aggression, shaking, and physically abusive behavior. As suggested in the previous literature, these more impulsive parenting practices may result from initial lack of boundaries and demands from parents. Parents then may impulsively control child behavior through one of the more intrusive disciplining methods. Finally, authoritative parenting, which is characterized by both high warmth and demandingness is related to more use of nonviolent discipline and less use of the intrusive disciplining methods including, psychological aggression, spanking with a bare hand, shaking, and physically abusive behavior. These results suggest that both dimensions of parenting, warmth and demandingness, may be important for choosing less intrusive disciplining methods. It 140 may be possible that excessively strict or lenient parenting may lead to more intrusive disciplining and even physically abusive parenting. In the next set of hypotheses, relationships between the parent disciplining techniques and physically abusive behavior were explored. The results of these analyses partially supported the hypotheses. For hypothesis nine and ten, nonviolent discipline showed the opposite relationship to the other disciplining techniques than what was expected. Table 26 Summary of the Relationship between Mother/Father Authoritarian, Authoritative, and Permissive Parenting Styles, Parent Discipline, and Physically Abusive Behavior Mother parenting styles Father parenting styles Variables Nonviolent Discipline Psychological Authoritarian Authoritative Permissive Authoritarian Authoritative Permissive N.S. + - N.S. + - + - + + - + + - N.S + - N.S. + N.S. N.S. + N.S. N.S. Aggression Spanking with bare hand Spanking with object 141 Pinching + N.S. N.S. + N.S. N.S. Shaking + - + + - + Physical abusive + - + + - + behavior Note: A positive relationship is indicated by a +, a negative relationship is indicated by a -, no relationship is indicated by N.S. 142 143 First, nonviolent discipline was related to higher use of psychological aggression. In addition, nonviolent discipline, like psychological aggression, was positively and not negatively related to spanking with a hand, spanking with an object, pinching, and shaking (Table 27). Finally, nonviolent discipline was not related to reported physically abusive behavior. Previous literature had not explored the relationship between nonviolent discipline and other disciplining methods. While it was expected that nonviolent discipline would be negatively correlated with other disciplining methods, it makes logical sense that these variables are positively related because they are all forms of discipline or punishment. If the definition of nonviolent discipline focused on rewarding or praising type techniques, it is possible that the results may have been as expected. Although nonviolent discipline was not related to physically abusive behavior, this finding supported previous research that has shown no relationship between the two variables. It is also possible that, since nonviolent discipline is very commonly used, both abusive and nonabusive parents may be likely to use high levels of this disciplining technique. As expected, psychological aggression was positively related to corporal punishment and physically abusive behavior. Although this disciplining technique has not been frequently explored in previous literature, the more intrusive nature of psychological aggression, may be similar to the intrusiveness of corporal punishment and physically abusive behavior. Table 27 Summary of the Relationships between Nonviolent Discipline, Psychological Aggression, Corporal Punishment, and Physically Abusive Behavior Variables Nonviolent discipline Psychological Aggression Spanking with hand + + Spanking with object + + Pinching + + Shaking + + N.S. + Physically abusive behavior Note: A positive relationship is indicated by a +, a negative relationship is indicated by a -, no relationship is indicated by N.S. 144 145 For hypothesis eleven, the expected relationships between the four corporal punishment items, when controlling for nonphysical discipline, and physically abusive behavior were partially supported. Specifically, spanking with an object, pinching, and shaking but not spanking with a bare hand were related to physically abusive behavior. These findings supported previous literature that found that corporal punishment was positively related to physically abusive behavior. However, these findings were contrary to studies focusing on spanking, which also showed a positive relationship with physically abusive behavior. It is possible that since spanking with a bare hand is a common disciplining technique, rates of use do not differ between abusive and nonabusive parents. It is also possible that when the three other corporal punishment items are considered, spanking with a bare hand is no longer a relevant factor in relationship to physically abusive behavior. It is possible that these other corporal punishment items are comparatively more predictive of physically abusive behavior. However, these findings supported previous studies that have suggested that corporal punishment may be important to consider when studying physical abuse. Since corporal punishment is a physical form of discipline, it is possible that it can more easily escalate into physically abusive attempts to control behavior. It is important to note that these findings also suggest that nonphysical discipline, specifically psychological aggression, may be more important in predicting the occurrence of physically abusive behavior than the four corporal punishment items. The last hypothesis explored the relationships between SES, participant sex, 146 authoritarian/authoritative parenting styles, parent disciplining techniques, and physically abusive behavior. In hypothesis twelve, it was expected that the three corporal punishment items (i.e., spanking with object, pinching, and shaking) would fully mediate the relationship between ethnicity and reports of physically abusive behavior even when controlling for covariate SES, participant sex, mother/father parenting styles, and psychological aggression. The results of these analyses partially supported the hypotheses. First, some significant sex and ethnic differences were still present after mother/father parenting styles and parent discipline were added. Male participants continued to report higher physically abusive behavior than females. This finding continued to support previous literature, despite controlling for SES, parenting styles, and parent discipline. In regards to ethnic differences, the analyses controlling for mother/father authoritarian parenting styles resulted in no significant ethnic differences between African and White Americans. However, Asian Americans continued to report higher physically abusive behavior than Latino and White Americans. In addition, African Americans reported higher physically abusive behavior than Latino Americans, which was a change from the marginal differences in the original analyses exploring ethnic differences. In the analyses controlling for mother/father authoritative parenting, the significant ethnic differences remained similar to the original analyses exploring ethnic differences. The differences between African and Latino Americans became significant again in these final analyses exploring authoritative parenting. Another result that was 147 counter to expectations was that the three corporal punishment items were not consistently the factors that predicted occurrence of physically abusive behavior. While pinching and shaking consistently predicted the occurrence of physically abusive behavior, spanking with an object did not. Finally, psychological aggression also consistently predicted the occurrence of physically abusive behavior. Another discrepancy found in these final analyses, is that the corporal punishment items were not all predictive of physically abusive behavior and they were not the only significant parent disciplining items. Specifically, a nonphysical disciplining method, psychological aggression, continued to be predictive of physically abusive behavior. This finding was unexpected because previous literature has suggested that physical forms of punishment may be most important in determining risk factors for physical abuse. However, as described previously, one limitation of past studies is that nonphysical forms of discipline are often not explored. An explanation for the current findings is that more common and planned forms of parent discipline, such as spanking, are unlikely to be related to a higher risk of physically abusive behavior. One commonality between the items that were found to be highly related to physically abusive behavior (i.e., pinching, shaking, and psychological aggression) is that they are parental behaviors that may be unplanned, much like the nature of physically abusive behavior. Limitations in the Current Study The current study included some limitations that should be addressed in future studies. First, the adult child sample used to determine parenting styles, parent discipline, 148 and parental physically abusive behavior may have resulted in some errors. Participants reported on childhood memories of these factors and it is possible that they may have been faulty. Conversely, it may have been difficult to measure some of the constructs in the current study (i.e. physically abusive behavior) in a child sample with either parents or children reporting on parent behavior. However, the results of the current study seem to report similar results to studies with children and adult children samples. Another limitation may lie in the way the Parent-Child version of the Conflict Tactics Scale was used. The corporal punishment subscale was divided and was used in a way not suggested by the original authors. However, past studies showed that looking at individual corporal punishment behaviors may show stronger predictions to physically abusive behavior, than measures with multiple items. It is also important to note that this study has supported previous research that has emphasized corporal punishment to be an important variable to consider for studies of physical abuse. Another limitation includes generalizability from the current sample to larger populations. First, the sample was from a college student population. The college population may differ from the larger community in a number of ways including family of origin socioeconomic status, participant sex, and age which may have impacted the results. However, the result of this study was similar to other studies of parent discipline utilizing a college sample and the current sample had a wide variability among the students in terms of family of origin socioeconomic status, and age. For example, on about half of the current sample experienced all four corporal punishment behaviors. 149 This finding supports previous studies that have also found a similar proportion of students experiencing various corporal punishment behaviors (Miller-Perrin et al., 2009; Turner & Muller, 2004). Next, this study only included very specific corporal punishment behaviors. It is possible that these results will only generalize to families that use the specific behaviors described in this study. However, it is important to note that the behaviors used to describe corporal punishment in this study were reflective of the behaviors used in previous studies and included the most common corporal punishment behaviors. Finally, one limitation of the current sample was the disproportionately low number of male participants. Since sex differences in reported parenting styles and discipline were highly significant, it may be important to collect a more evenly distributed sample in regards to participant sex in future studies. However, in regards to distributions of sexes in college samples, it may be more reflective of the college population to have more female participants. This distribution is particularly evident among the psychology undergraduate student population. Due to these issues, precautions were taken to avoid finding significant sex differences if none existed. Finally, despite the disproportionately low number of males, the results regarding parent discipline and physically abusive behavior seem to support previous research. Clinical Implications The current study gives a basis for a number of methodological and practical implications. One aspect of the study revealed that socioeconomic status was not found to 150 be a consistent predictor of parent discipline or physically abusive behavior. This finding supported a trend that is beginning to emerge that different ethnic groups are becoming more similar in socioeconomic status. It may be beneficial to continue to examine this variable, however, researchers may focus on how this variable is changing as a possible predictor. However, one negative aspect of including this variable in future studies is that it may reduce the impact or importance of other variables such as parenting styles and parent discipline in studies of physical abuse. Another thing that may be important to consider is the context of parent discipline. Some of the findings in this study suggest that general parent attitudes may be related to different parent disciplining methods and physically abusive behavior, at least for some ethnic groups. Since the parenting attitudes of an ethnic group precede parent behavior, this factor may be important to include in future studies to better understand attitudinal risk factors for physical abuse. This study also showed that there may be possible ethnic or cultural differences in the use of different corporal punishment items and these are differentially related to physical abuse. It may be beneficial to separate corporal punishment behaviors in future studies to better determine their relationships to physically abusive behavior. Finally, the inclusion of nonphysical disciplining methods, specifically psychological aggression, may be important for future research. Including these other methods of discipline in studies of corporal punishment and physical abuse may give a better understanding of how using different disciplining methods together may heighten the risk for physical abuse. 151 Some practical implications for social service agencies and mental health clinicians may be derived from the results of the current study. While previous research has focused on less severe physical punishment (i.e., corporal punishment) as a precursor to physical abuse, the current study suggests that other parental behaviors, such as psychological aggression, may be a risk factor to consider as well. This information may be important in the assessment of families referred to social service agencies for child physical abuse. It may be beneficial to incorporate assessment of nonphysical forms of discipline in families rather than only assessing parents for corporal punishment use or children for physical injuries. The results of this study showed that spanking was not a strong predictor of physically abusive behavior. However, since nonviolent discipline may be indirectly related and psychological aggression is directly related to physically abusive behavior and these behaviors would be beneficial to assess in parents who are referred. This information may also be helpful to incorporate into parent training programs for parents mandated to attend counseling after a referral for physical abuse. In the past, parent training programs have focused on the teaching of alternative nonviolent disciplining techniques. However, in the current study, there is some suggestion that the two nonphysical forms of discipline, nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression, may both occur at high rates for some parents. It may be possible that only teaching nonviolent disciplining methods may not lead to a reduction in psychological aggression. Therefore, it may be beneficial to address parental psychological aggression in parent training programs as well. Finally, the results of this study have suggested teaching the 152 use of high demandingness but also high warmth, as described by authoritative parenting styles, may be important in indirectly assisting parents in choosing less intrusive or severe disciplining methods that may lead to physically abusive behavior. Future Directions in Research The current study is a basis for future studies in this field. One such study could utilize a younger study sample for more current reports of parenting styles and disciplining practices. When using a younger or child sample, it may also be beneficial to include parent reports of behavior as well to minimize biases in child reports of parent behavior. Also, it may be important to control for child behavior in future studies. It is possible that rate of child misbehavior, such as noncompliance, may be a factor to control for when studying parent discipline. In addition, the examination of individual parenting behaviors has been shown to be important in studies of physical abuse and in the current study. It may be useful to examine parenting behaviors, such as nonviolent discipline, psychological aggression, and corporal punishment behaviors this way in the future. Future studies may attempt to explore other relevant factors, such as acculturation and parenting differences for boys and girls, to better explain the overrepresentation of boys and ethnic minorities in physical abuse cases. When the current and past studies have focused on nonphysical discipline, these behaviors have included punishment behaviors such as nonviolent discipline and psychological aggression. Since these behaviors are indirectly and directly related to 153 physical abusive behavior, it may be beneficial to explore other parenting practices that aim to control child behavior but are not punishing in nature. These future studies could include parenting behaviors such as using praise or a reward system to control child behavior. It would be beneficial to know if these other behaviors are related to nonphysical discipline, corporal punishment and physically abusive behaviors. These types of studies may lead to more knowledge about safer parenting practices. Finally, it is suggested by this study that parent attitudes, nonphysical discipline, corporal punishment, and physically abusive behavior may be related to one another in a progressively aggressive sequence. It may be valuable to examine parent attitudes in the form of parenting styles in a general population of parents and use this information for a basis of a longitudinal study to better examine this possible sequence. A longitudinal study may facilitate understanding the relationship between parenting styles, nonphysical discipline, corporal punishment, and physically abusive behavior. Specifically, it would be beneficial to study parenting attitudes and styles in new parents and examine these parents’ use of parent discipline and physically abusive behavior as the child grows into an adult. This information could also be useful for studying the same child sample as adults to gain better understanding of how parenting is perceived in adult children. A longitudinal study can also attempt to link parent discipline to actual referrals of parents for physical abuse or founded cases of physical abuse in the same sample. The current study supported the importance of parenting styles and parent disciplining practices when examining physically abusive behavior. It has been shown 154 that these variables may explain some of the differences in rates of abuse among different demographic groups, such as males and ethnic minority groups. Future studies should continue to focus on these variables. Ultimately, uncovering the predictive variables for physically abusive behavior and designing studies with generalizable results will pave the way to the creation of effective child protection measures and proliferation of safe parenting practices. 155 REFERENCES Abada, T., Hou, F., & Ram, B. (2008). The effects of harassment and victimization on self-rated health and mental health among Canadian adolescents. Social Science & Medicine, 67, 557-567. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.04.006 Abolfotouh, M. A., El-Bourgy, M. D., Seif El Din, A. G., & Mehanna, A. A. (2009). Corporal punishment: Mother’s disciplinary behavior and child’s psychological profile in Alexandria, Egypt. Journal of Forensic Nursing, 5, 5-17. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-3938.2009.01025.x Ackerman, P. T., Newton, J., McPherson, W. B., Jones, J.G., & Dykman, R. A. (1998). Prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder and other psychiatric diagnoses in three groups of abused children (sexual, physical, and both). Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 759–774. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(98)00062-3 Afifi, T.O., Brownridge, D. A., Cox, B. J., & Sareen, J. (2006). Physical punishment, childhood abuse and psychiatric disorders. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30, 1093– 1103. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.04.006 Allen, D. M., & Tarnowski, K. J. (1989). Depressive characteristics of physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 1-11. doi:10.1007/BF00910766 Amato, P. R., & Fowler, F. (2002). Parenting practices, child adjustment, and family diversity. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 703–716. doi:10.1111/j.17413737.2002.00703.x 156 Amuwo, S., Fabian, R., Tolley, G., Spence, A., & Hill, J. (2004). Child discipline and family decision-making. The Journal of Socioeconomics, 33, 153-173. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2003.12.016 Bailey, J. A., Hill, K. G., Oesterle, S., & Hawkins, J. D. (2009). Parenting practices and problem behavior across three generations: Monitoring, harsh discipline, and drug use in the intergenerational transmission of externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1214-1226. doi: 10.1037/a0016129 Barbo, E. (2004). The effects of child sexual abuse, child physical abuse, and combined child sexual and physical abuse on adult sexual victimization and adult posttraumatic stress disorder. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 64 (12-B), 6321. (AAI3114433). Barkin, S., Scheindlin, B., Ip, E. H., Richardson, I., & Finch, S. (2007). Determinants of parental discipline practices: A national sample from primary care practices. Clinical Pediatrics, 46, 64-69. doi:10.1177/0009922806292644 Baumrind, D. (1996). A blanket injunction against disciplinary use of spanking is not warranted by the data. Pediatrics, 98, 828-831. Baumrind, D. (1984). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75, 42-89. Bean, A. W., & Roberts, M. W. (1981). The effect of time-out release contingencies on changes in child noncompliance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 9, 95-105. doi:10.1007/BF00917860 157 Berlin, L. J., Ispa, J. M., Fine, M. A., Malone, P. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Brady-Smith, C., Ayoub, C., & Bai, Y. (2009). Correlates and consequences of spanking and verbal punishment for low-income white, African American, and Mexican American toddlers. Child Development, 80, 1403–1420. doi:10.1111/j.14678624.2009.01341.x Bluestone, C., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1999). Correlates of parenting styles in predominantly working and middle-class African American mothers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 61, 881-893. Boyle, C. L., Sanders, M. R., Lutzker, J. R., Prinz, R. J., Shapiro, C., & Whitaker, D. J. (2010). An analysis of training, generalization, and maintenance effects of primary care Triple P for parents of preschool-aged children with disruptive behavior. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 41, 114-131. doi:10.1007/s10578-009-0156-7 Bradley, C.R. (1998). Child rearing in African-American families: A study of the disciplinary practices of African-American parents. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 26, 273-281. Bryan, J. W., & Freed, F. W. (1982). Corporal punishment: Normative data and sociological and psychological correlates in a community college population. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 11, 77-87. doi:10.1007/BF01834705 Burchinal, M., Skinner, D., & Reznick, J. S. (2010). European American and African American mothers' beliefs about parenting and disciplining infants: A mixed- 158 method analysis. Parenting: Science and Practice, 10, 79-96. doi:10.1080/15295190903212604 Buschgens, C. J. M., van Aken, M. A. G., Swinkels, S. H. N., Ormel, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010). Externalizing behaviors in preadolescents: Familial risk to externalizing behaviors and perceived parenting styles. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 19, 567-575. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0086-8 Buri, J. R. (1991). Parental authority questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 110-119. Calder, J., McVean, A., & Wang, W. (2010). History of abuse and current suicidal ideation: Results from a population based survey. Journal of Family Violence, 25, 205-214. doi:10.1007/s10896-009-9284-x Caughy, M. O., & Franzini, L. (2005). Neighborhood correlates of cultural differences in perceived effectiveness of parental disciplinary tactics. Parenting: Science and Practice, 5, 119-151. doi: 10.1207/s15327922par0502_1 Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159. Day, D. E., & Roberts, M. W. (1983). An analysis of the physical punishment component of a parent training program. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 141152. doi:10.1007/BF00912184 Dearing, E. (2004). The developmental implications of restrictive and supportive parenting across neighborhoods and ethnicities: Exceptions are the rule. Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 555–575. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2004.08.007 159 Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 161-175. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0803_1 Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S. (1996). Physical discipline among African American and European American mothers: Links to children's externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1065- 1072. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.6.1065 Dietz, T. L. (2000). Disciplining children characteristics associated with the use of corporal punishment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 1529–1542. doi:10.1016/S01452134(00)00213-1 Dodge, K. A., Bates, J.E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science, 250, 1678-1683. doi:10.1126/science.2270481 Eamon, M. K. (2001). Antecedents and socioemotional consequences of physical punishment on children in two-parent families. Child Abuse & Neglect, 6, 787– 802. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(01)00239-3 Edwards, V. J., Holden, G. W., Felitti, V. J., & Anda, R. F. (2003). Relationship between multiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health in community respondents: Results from the adverse childhood experiences study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1453-1460. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1453 Feldman, R. S., Salzinger, S., Rosario, M., Alvarado, L., Caraballo, L., & Hammer, M. (1995). Parent, teacher, and peer ratings of physically abused and nonmaltreated 160 children's behavior. Joumal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 317-334. doi:10.1007/BF01447560 Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2008). Exposure to childhood sexual and physical abuse and adjustment in early adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 607-619. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.12.018 Fite, P. J., Stoppelbein, L., & Greening, L. (2009). Predicting readmission to a child psychiatric inpatient unit: The impact of parenting styles. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18, 621-629. doi: 10.1007/s10826-009-9284-8 Fletcher, A. C., Walls, J. K., Cook, E. C., Madison, K. J., & Bridges, T. H. (2008). Parenting style as a moderator of associations between maternal disciplinary strategies and child well-being. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 1724-1744. doi: 10.1177/0192513X08322933 Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Childhood mistreatment and adolescent and young adult depression. Social Science & Medicine, 68, 799-806. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.12.005 Fluke, J. D., Yuan, Y. T., Hedderson, J., & Curtis, P. A. (2003). Disproportionate representation of race and ethnicity in child maltreatment: Investigation and victimization. Children and Youth Services Review, 25, 359–373. doi:10.1016/S0190-7409(03)00026-4 161 Flynn, C. P., (1998). To spank or not to spank: The effect of situation and age of child on support for corporal punishment. Journal of Family Violence, 13, 21-37. doi:10.1023/A:1022808716048 Frias-Armenta, M., & McCloskey, L. A. (1998). Determinants of harsh parenting in Mexico. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 129-139. Fung, J. J., & Lau, A. S. (2009). Punitive discipline and child behavior problems in Chinese-American immigrant families: The moderating effects of indigenous child-rearing ideologies. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 520–530. doi:10.1177/0165025409343749 Gamez-Guadix, M., Straus, M. A., Carrobles, J. A., Munoz-Rivas, M. J., & Almendros, C., (2010). Corporal punishment and long-term behavior problems: The moderating role of positive parenting and psychological aggression. Psicothema, 22, 529-536. Garbarino, J. (1977). The human ecology of child maltreatment: A conceptual model for research. Journal of Marriage and Family, 39, 721-735. doi:10.2307/350477 Gest, S. D., Freeman, N. R., Domitrovich, C. E., & Welsh, J. A. (2004). Shared book reading and children’s language comprehension skills: The moderating role of parental discipline practices. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 319–336. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.04.007 162 Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 539-579. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.539 Gil, D. G. (1970). Violence against children: Physical child abuse in the United States. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Giles-Sims, J., Straus, M. A., & Sugarman, D. B. (1995). Child, maternal, and family characteristics associated with spanking. Family Relations, 44, 170-176. doi:10.2307/584804 Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children. (2010, August). Countdown to universal prohibition. Retrieved September 3, 2010 from http://www.endcorporalpunishment.org/pages/frame.html Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children. (2009). Human rights and corporal punishment of children. Retrieved October 1, 2009 from http://www.endcorporalpunishment.org/pages/frame.html Hakman, M., Chaffin, M., Funderburk, B., & Silovsky, J. F. (2009). Change trajectories for parent-child interaction sequences during parent-child interaction therapy for child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 461-470. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.08.003 Haskett, M. E., Allaire, J. C., Kreig, S., & Hart, K. C. (2008). Protective and vulnerability factors for physically abused children: Effects of ethnicity and parenting context. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 567–576. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.06.009 163 Hollingshead, W. (1979). The Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Socioeconomic Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University, New Haven, CT. Horn, I. B., (2004). Nonabusive physical punishment and child behavior among AfricanAmerican children: A systematic review. Journal of the National Medical Association, 96, 1162-1168. Hughes, T. (2006). The neglect of children and culture: Responding to child maltreatment with cultural competence and a review of Child Abuse and Culture: Working with Diverse Families. Family Court Review, 44, 501-510. doi:10.1111/j.17441617.2006.00103.x Ibanez, E. S., Borrego, J., Pemberton, J. R., & Terao, S. (2006). Cultural factors in decision-making about child physical abuse: Identifying reporter characteristics influencing reporting tendencies. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30, 1365–1379. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.06.007 Jackson, S., Thompson, R. A., Christiansen, E. H., Colman, R. A., Wyatt, J., Buckendahl, C. W.,…Peterson, R. (1999). Predicting abuse-prone parental attitudes and discipline practices in a nationally representative sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23, 15-29. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(98)00108-2 Jackson-Newsom, J., Buchanan, C. M., & McDonald, R. M. (2008). Parenting and perceived maternal warmth in European American and African American adolescents. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70, 62–75. 164 Jaffe, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Taylor, A. (2004). Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 44-55. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.44 Jambunathan, S., Burts, D. C., & Pierce, S. (2000). Comparisons of parenting attitudes among five ethnic groups in the United States. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 31, 395-406. Johnsona, R. M., Kotch, J. B., Catellier, D. J., Winsor, J. R., Dufort, V., Hunter, W., & Amaya-Jackson, L. (2002). Adverse behavioral and emotional outcomes from child abuse and witnessed violence. Child Maltreatment, 7, 179-186. doi:10.1177/1077559502007003001 Joiner, T. E., Sachs-Ericsson, N. J., Wingate, L. R., Brown, J. S., Anestis, M. D., & Selby, E. A. (2007). Childhood physical and sexual abuse and lifetime number of suicide attempts: A persistent and theoretically important relationship. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 539–547. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.04.007 Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. A. (1981). Child abuse: An interactional event. New York, NY: Columbia University Press. Kaufman, D., Geston, E., Santa Lucia, R. C., Salcedo, O., Rendina-Gobioff, G., & Gadd, R. (2000). The relationship between parenting style and children’s adjustment: The parents’ perspective. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 9, 231–245. 165 Kazdin, A. E., Moser, J., Colbus, D., & Bell, R. (1985). Depressive symptoms among physically abused and psychiatrically disturbed children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 298-307. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.94.3.298 Kennedy, M. A., (2003). Identifying child abuse: A structural equation modeling analysis of history of abuse and westernization on perceptions of abuse among Asiandescent and European-descent students. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 65 (3-A), 829. (AAINQ90205) Khoury-Kassabri, M. (2009). Attitudes of Arab and Jewish mothers towards punitive and non-punitive discipline methods. Child and Family Social Work, 15, 135-144. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2206.2009.00667.x Kim, E., & Hong, S. (2006). First-generation Korean-American parents’ perceptions of discipline. Journal of Professional Nursing, 23, 60-68. doi:10.1016/j.profnurs.2006.12.002 Kiser, L., Heston, J., Millsap, P., & Pruitt, D. (1991). Physical and sexual abuse in childhood: relationship with post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 776-783. doi:10.1097/00004583-199109000-00013 Klevens, J., & Leeb, R. T. (2010). Child maltreatment fatalities in children under 5: Findings from the National Violence Death Reporting System. Child Abuse & Neglect, 34, 262-266. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.07.005 166 Klimes-Dougan, B., & Kistner, J. (1990). Physically abused preschoolers' responses to peers' distress. Developmental Psychology, 26, 599-602. doi:10.1037/00121649.26.4.599 Lamm, B., Keller, H., Yovsi, R. D., & Chaudhary, N. (2008). Grandmaternal and maternal ethnotheoriesa about early child care. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 80-88. Lansford, J. E., Chang, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Palmerus, K.,… Quinn, N. (2005). Physical discipline and children’s adjustment: Cultural normativeness as a moderator. Child Development, 76, 1234-246. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00847.x Lansford, J. E., Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2004). Ethnic differences in the link between physical discipline and later adolescent externalizing behaviors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 801– 812. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00273.x Larzelere, R. E., Klein, M., Shumm, W. R., & Alibrando, S. A. (1989). Relations of spanking and other parenting characteristics to self-esteem and perceived fairness of parental discipline. Psychological Reports, 64, 1140-1142. Larzelere, R. E., & Kuhn, B. R. (2005). Enhancing behavioral parent training with an extended discipline ladder. The Behavior Therapist, 28, 105-108. Leary, C. E., Kelley, M. L., Morrow, J., & Mikulka, P. J. (2008). Parental use of physical punishment as related to family environment, psychological well-being, and 167 personality in undergraduates. Journal of Family Violence, 23, 1-7. doi:10.1007/s10896-007-9124-9 Lee, S. J., Guterman, N. B., & Lee, Y. (2008). Risk factors for paternal physical child abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 846–858. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.11.006 Lee, J., Pomeroy, E. C., & Bohman, T. M. (2007). Intimate partner violence and psychological health in a sample of Asian and Caucasian women: The roles of social support and coping. Journal of Family Violence, 22, 709-720. doi:10.1007/s10896-007-9119-6 Litrownik, A. J., Newton, R. R., & Landsverk, J. A. (2005). Assessment of depressive symptomatology in young maltreated children. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 11, 135-156. doi: 10.1300/J137v11n03_07 Lynam, D. R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2008). The stability of psychopathy from adolescence into adulthood: The search for moderators. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 35, 228-243. doi:10.1177/0093854807310153 Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents' differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267-296. doi:10.1037/00332909.109.2.267 Maccoby, E. E. & Martin, J.A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent– child interaction. In P. Mussen and E. M. Hetherington, (Eds), Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed.) (pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley. 168 Maikovich, A. K., Jaffe, S. R., Odgers, C. L., & Gallop, R. (2008). Effects of family violence on psychopathology symptoms in children previously exposed to maltreatment. Child Development, 79, 1498-1512. doi:10.1111/j.14678624.2008.01201.x Maker, A. H., Shah, P. V., & Agha, Z. (2009). South Asian, Middle Eastern, East Asian, and Latina women in the United States child physical abuse: Prevalence, characteristics, predictors, and beliefs about parent-child violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 1406-1428. doi:10.1177/0886260505278713 Mathurin, M. N., Gielen, U. P., & Lancaster, J. (2006). Corporal punishment and personality traits in the children of St. Croix, U.S. Virgin Islands. Cross-Cultural Research, 40, 306-324. doi:10.1177/1069397105284678 Matta, M. A. (2002). Parental corporal punishment as a predictor of child maladjustment: Race and parental responsiveness as potential moderators. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 63 (4-B), 2091. (AAI3049173) McCabe, K. M., Clark, R., & Barnett, D. (1999). Family protective factors among urban African American youth. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 137-150. McLoyd, V. C., & Smith, J. (2002). Physical discipline and behavior problems in African American, European American, and Hispanic children: Emotional support as a moderator. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 40-53. doi:10.1111/j.1741- 3737.2002.00040.x 169 McKinney, C., & Renk, K. (2008). Differential parenting between mothers and fathers: Implications for late adolescents. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 806-827. doi: 10.1177/0192513X07311222 Medora, N. P., Wilson, S., & Larson, J. H. (2001). Attitudes toward parenting strategies, potential for child abuse, and parental satisfaction of ethnically diverse lowincome U.S. mothers. The Journal of Social Psychology, 141, 335-348. doi:10.1080/00224540109600555 Mersky, J. P., Berger, L. M., Reynolds, A. J., & Gromoske, A.N. (2009). Risk factors for child and adolescent maltreatment: A longitudinal investigation of a cohort of inner-city youth. Child Maltreatment, 14, 73-88. doi:10.1177/1077559508318399 Miller-Perrin, C. L., Perrin, R. D., & Kocur, J. L. (2009). Parental physical and psychological aggression: Psychological symptoms in young adults. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.12.001 Moore, T. E., & Pepler, D. J. (2006). Wounding words: Maternal verbal aggression and children’s adjustment. Journal of Family Violence, 21, 89-93. doi:10.1007/s10896-005-9007-x Mulvaney, M. K., & Mebert, C. J. (2007). Parental corporal punishment predicts behavior problems in early childhood. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 389–397. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.389 Mupinga, E. E., Garrison, M. E. B., & Pierce, S. H. (2002). An exploratory study of the relationships between family functioning and parenting styles: The perceptions of 170 mothers of young grade school children. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 31, 112-129. Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Carlo, G. (2004). “It’s not fair!” Adolescents’ constructions of appropriateness of parental reactions. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 389401. doi:10.1023/B:JOYO.0000037632.46633.bd Park, H., & Bauer, S. (2002). Parenting practices, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and academic achievement in adolescents. School International Psychology, 23, 386396. doi: 10.1177/0143034302234002 Paquette, D., Bolte, C. Turcotte, G., Dubeau, D., & Bouchard, C. (2000). A new typology of fathering: Defining and associated variables. Journal of Infant and Child Development, 9, 213-230. Pelcovitz, D. (1984). Adolescent abuse: Family structure and implications for treatment. Journal of American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 85-90. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198401000-00012 Pinderhughes, E. E., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Zelli, A. (2000). Discipline responses: Influences of parents' socioeconomic status, ethnicity, beliefs about parenting, stress, and cognitive-emotional processes. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 380-400. doi:10.1037//0893-3200.14.3.380 Polaha, J., Larzelere, R. E., Shapiro, S. K., & Pettit, G. S. (2004). Physical discipline and child behavior problems: A study of ethnic group differences. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 339-360. doi:10.1207/s15327922par0404_6 171 Radziszewska, B., Richardson, J. L., Dent, C. W., & Flay, B. R. (1996). Parenting style and adolescent depressive symptoms, smoking, and academic achievement: Ethnic, gender, and SES differences. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 289305. Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., & Hart, C. H. (2001). The parenting styles and dimensions questionnaire. In J. Touliatos & B. Perlmutter (Eds.), Handbook of family measurement techniques. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Sedlak, A. J., & Broadhurst, D. D. (1996). Third national incidence study of child abuse and neglect. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth, and Families. Sedlak, A. J., McPherson, K., & Das, B. (2010b). Fourth national incidence study of child abuse and neglect (NIS–4): Supplementary analyses of race differences in child maltreatment rates in the NIS–4. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families. Sedlak, A. J., Mettenburg, J., Basena, M., Petta, I., McPherson, K., Greene, A., & Li, S. (2010a). Fourth national incidence study of child abuse and neglect (NIS–4): Report to congress. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families. Simons, L. G., & Conger, R. D. (2007). Linking mother-father differences in parenting to a typology of family parenting styles and adolescent outcomes. Journal of Family Issues, 28, 212-241. doi: 10.1177/0192513X06294593 172 Smagner, J. P., & Sullivan, M. H., (2005). Investigating the effectiveness of behavioral parent training with involuntary clients in child welfare settings. Research on Social Work Practice, 15, 431-439. doi:10.1177/1049731505276994 Springer, K. W., Sheridan, J., Kuo, D., & Carnes, M. (2007). Long-term physical and mental health consequences of childhood physical abuse: Results from a large population-based sample of men and women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31, 517– 530. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.01.003 Steinberg, L., Blatt-Eisengart, I., & Cauffman, E. (2006). Patterns of competence and adjustment among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful homes: A replication in a sample of serious juvenile offenders. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 47–58. Steinberg, L., Mounts, N. S., Lamborn, S. D., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Authoritative parenting and adolescent adjustment across varied ecological niches. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 1, 19-36. Strassberg, Z., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Spanking in the home and children’s subsequent aggression toward peers. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 445-461. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006040 Straus, M. A. (2000). Corporal punishment and primary prevention of physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 1109-1114. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00180-0 Straus, M. A., & Field, C. J., (2003). Psychological aggression by American parents: 173 national data on prevalence, chronicity, and severity. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 795-808. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00795.x Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Moore, D. W., & Runyan, D. (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-child conflict tactics scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 249-270. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(97)00174-9 Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The revised conflict tactics scale (CTS2). The Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283-316. Straus, M. A., & Stewart, J. H. (1999). Corporal punishment by American parents: National data on prevalence, chronicity, severity, and duration, in relation to child and family characteristics. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2, 5570. doi: 10.1023/A:1021891529770 Tajima, E. A., (2002). Risk factors for violence against children : Comparing homes with and without wife abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17, 122-149. doi:10.1177/0886260502017002002 Thompson, M. P., Kingree, J. B., & Desai, S. (2004). Gender differences in long-term health consequences of physical abuse of children: Data from a nationally representative survey. American Journal of Public Health, 94(4), 599-604. doi:10.2105/AJPH.94.4.599 Thompson, R. A., Christiansen, E. H., Jackson, S., Wyatt, J. M., Colman, R. A., Peterson, R. L., Wilcox, B. L., & Buckendahl, C. W. (2009). Parent attitudes and discipline 174 practices: Profiles and correlates in a nationally representative sample. Child Maltreatment, 4, 316-330. doi:10.1177/1077559599004004005 Trickett, P. A., & Kuczynski, L. (1986). Children's misbehaviors and parental discipline strategies in abusive and nonabusive families. Developmental Psychology, 22, 115-123. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.1.115 Turner, H. A., & Muller, P. A. (2004). Long-term effects of child corporal punishment on depressive symptoms in young adults: Potential moderators and mediators. Journal of Family Issues, 25, 761-782. doi:10.1177/0192513X03258313 U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration on Children, Youth and Families. (2007). Child Maltreatment 2007. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Wainwright, N. W. J., & Surtees, P. G. (2002). Childhood adversity, gender and depression over the life-course. Journal of Affective Disorders, 72, 33-44. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00420-7 Waters, M. C., & Sykes, J. E. (2009). Spare the rod, ruin the child? First- and secondgeneration West Indian child-rearing practices. In N. Foner (Ed.), Across Generations: Immigrant Families in America (pp. 72-94). New York: New York University Press. Webster-Stratton, C., & Hancock, L. (1998). Training for parents of young children with conduct problems: Content, methods, and therapeutic processes. In C. E. Schaefer 175 & J. M. Briesmeister (Eds.), Handbook of parent training (pp. 98–152). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Weller, S. C., Romney, A. K., & Orr, D. P. (1987). The myth of a sub-culture of corporal punishment. Human Organization, 46, 39-47. Whipple, E. E., & Richey, C. A. (1997). Crossing the line from physical discipline to child abuse: How much is too much? Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 431-444. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(97)00004-5 Whiteside-Mansell, L., Bradley, R. H., & McKelvey, L. (2009). Parenting and preschool child development: Examination of three low-income U.S. cultural groups. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18, 48-60. doi:10.1007/s10826-008-9206-1 Wilson, S. R., Rack, J. J., Shi, X., & Norris, A. N. (2008). Comparing physically abusive, neglectful, and non-maltreating parents during interactions with their children: A meta-analysis of observational studies. Child Abuse and Neglect, 32, 897-911. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.01.003 Wissow, L. S. (2001). Ethnicity, income, and parenting contexts of physical punishment in a national sample of families with young children. Child Maltreatment, 6, 118129. doi:10.1177/1077559501006002004 Wolff, N. L., & Shi, S. (2010). Trauma and incarcerated persons. In Scott, Charles L. (Ed.), Handbook of correctional mental health (2nd ed.) (pp. 277-320). Arlington, VA, US: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. 176 Yates, T. (2006). A longitudinal study of self-injurious behavior in a community sample. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 66 (8-B), 4518. (AAI3184985) Zolotor, A. J., Theodore, A. D., Chang, J., Berkoff, M. C., & Runyan, D. (2008). Speak softly and forget the stick: Corporal punishment and child physical abuse. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35, 364-369. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2008.06.031