October 26 Charitable & Willing 2009 A big part of the world depends on the altruistic behavior of others. The charity organizations intermediating between beneficiaries and benefactors rely on selfless behavior for their funds and staffing. What makes people behave pro-social? What makes them sacrifice self for the common good? What motivates people to display the willingness to donate to charities? MSC Thesis Economics & Business, Marketing By Wouter Chömpff Supervisor Ron van Schie 1 The role of individual dispositions and charity perceptions on the willingness to donate Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Contents Summary ................................................................................................................................................. 4 Chapter 1 ................................................................................................................................................. 5 Altruism; the root of charitable behavior........................................................................................... 5 Problem statement.............................................................................................................................. 7 Definitions ........................................................................................................................................... 8 Purpose of the research ...................................................................................................................... 9 Chapter 2; Theoretical Framework ....................................................................................................... 10 Willingness to donate ........................................................................................................................ 10 Socio-cultural factors......................................................................................................................... 12 Age ................................................................................................................................................. 12 Gender ........................................................................................................................................... 13 Income ........................................................................................................................................... 13 Education ....................................................................................................................................... 14 Urban or non-urban ...................................................................................................................... 14 Individual disposition towards altruistic behaviour .......................................................................... 16 Faith in humanity........................................................................................................................... 16 Spirituality ..................................................................................................................................... 17 Selfishness ..................................................................................................................................... 18 Attitude towards charity organizations............................................................................................. 19 Efficiency ....................................................................................................................................... 19 Administrative tolerance ............................................................................................................... 20 Effectiveness.................................................................................................................................. 21 Interaction: selfishness & effectiveness ........................................................................................ 21 Conceptual model ............................................................................................................................. 22 Chapter 3; Empirical elements .............................................................................................................. 23 Questionnaire design ........................................................................................................................ 24 Willingness to donate .................................................................................................................... 24 Urban ............................................................................................................................................. 24 Faith in humanity........................................................................................................................... 25 Selfishness ..................................................................................................................................... 25 Spirituality ..................................................................................................................................... 25 Efficiency & Administrative Tolerance .......................................................................................... 25 Effectiveness.................................................................................................................................. 25 2 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Results ................................................................................................................................................... 26 Factor analysis ................................................................................................................................... 26 Reliability test .................................................................................................................................... 27 Descriptive statistics .......................................................................................................................... 28 Regression: outline ............................................................................................................................ 30 General model ............................................................................................................................... 31 Dispositions ................................................................................................................................... 32 Attitudes towards charity .............................................................................................................. 32 Interaction effect included; ........................................................................................................... 32 Willingness to donate time ............................................................................................................... 33 General model ............................................................................................................................... 33 Dispositions & attitudes towards charity ...................................................................................... 33 Interaction effect included ............................................................................................................ 34 Concluding ............................................................................................................................................. 35 Discussion .......................................................................................................................................... 36 In general ....................................................................................................................................... 36 On donating money ....................................................................................................................... 36 On donating time........................................................................................................................... 37 Limitations ......................................................................................................................................... 38 Future research ................................................................................................................................. 39 Thanksgiving .......................................................................................................................................... 40 References ............................................................................................................................................. 41 Appendix................................................................................................................................................ 47 3 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Summary When observing modern day charitable donations to organizations operating in a highly competitive market one cannot help but to be tickled by the question: what motivates those donating? 3 main categories of factors which should influence the propensity to donate time & money where constructed. Within those, several variables were inserted as to answer the following questions: The socio-cultural factors, depicting the background of the respondent; which sociodemographic variables account for the amounts donated to charity? Factors concerning the personal disposition; spiritual focus, selfish tendencies and the faith one has in his peers. Which innate characteristics produce altruistic behaviour? Does the individual tend to harbour a disposition towards altruistic behaviour which influences his propensity to give? Factors depicting the outlook on charities; focussing on the effectiveness (do charities reach their goals) as the efficiency (do charities manage to get a lot of mileage out of the donated euro)? Is there an attitude to charity which fertilizes this behaviour and so makes it more probable that the individual will make donations? We found that the outlooks on charity explained significantly more of the donated amounts than the personal dispositions. Those considering the charity to be efficient and effective were more willing to donate money. This effect was reinforced if the individual was both unselfish and deemed charities effective. Those with a spiritual disposition and a belief in the effectiveness of charities are more likely to donate their hours to a cause. Charities could draw the inference that they should advertise their effectiveness as their strongest trait, as it is the single strongest predictor of money and time donations. Currently, charity advertising focuses on the grave need of the beneficiary. Even though the realisation of acute need might be a motivation for people to join the cause, if the intermediary charity organisation is considered to be ineffective or inefficient the positive stimulus may fall into dry ground. Then, volunteer recruiting efforts should focus on the spiritual as they are more inclined to donate their hours. They also should be told their efforts will contribute greatly – if they deem their work effective they are more willing to sign up. The efficiency of spending during fundraising could be accentuated, as currently people only think that 56 cents of every euro ends up at the beneficiary. 4 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Chapter 1 Altruism; the root of charitable behavior He who wishes to secure the good of others, has already secured his own. Confucius Love thy neighbour. In giving we receive. Truly great have great charity, althus spoke Confucius. Throughout the ages, it was deemed a virtue to stretch one’s own resources for another man’s benefit; it was man’s natural duty. Apart from societal and religious obligations, it was believed that the nature of man was imbued with a certain benevolence. In his 1759 treaty on moral sentiments, economist Adam Smith exclaims; “How selfish so ever man be supposed, there are evidently some principles in his nature, which interest him in the fortune of others, and render their happiness necessary to him, though he derives nothing from it, except the pleasure of seeing it.” But then the further blossoming of modern science gave birth to the strict rationality which stated that every act appearing to be motivated by concern for someone else’s fate will, under close scrutiny, bear witness to selfish motives. (See for an oversight Piliavin & Hong-Wen, 1990) Within the frame of this egoistic model, biologists, psychiatrists, sociologists and economists agreed that pure selfless deeds do not exist, nor ever existed. More recently, these assumptions were relaxed and over the years discarded. Certain observations, namely by Hill (1984) shook the pillars of the egoistic model: many cases of extreme heroism where a total disregard for the actor’s own life signify a lack of conscious awareness of the deed. Consider the act of jumping in front of a train to pull a child from the tracks. Or running into a burning building to save a stranger. Crawford, Smith and Krebs (1987) reasoned that no egoistic motive can be present when a person spontaneously puts his/her life on the line. Concerning all the acts of spontaneous helping one could suggest that humans are genetically disposed to engage in impulsive altruistic behaviour. This would be supported by observations in nature; birds alarm their flock when predators approach, baboons help defend the troops, mother rats endure severe electric shocks to save their offspring. Masserman, Stanley and Terris (1964) found that Rhesus monkeys refrain from operating a device for securing food if this causes another monkey to suffer an electric shock. Lower species – not in possession of a rational mind – show altruistic behaviour as well. As humans, we most likely share tendencies towards similar behaviour. Bear in mind that we are very much like animals: according to Britten (1992), we share 98% of our DNA with the Chimpanzee. We can merge the necessity of scientific explanation with the observation that humans consistently respond to kin’s distress: they claim that selflessness has a neural base that may have been present early in human evolution. 5 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 However, it is not believed that humans are the products of instincts. Vine (1983) warns that genetic involvement does not mean fixed action patterns. Between an impulse and a response humans have a unique –though not always used – decision space. Thus besides ‘evolutionary altruism’, where neural networks seem to spur the actor into selfless behaviour, there must be an alternative. Sober (1988) identifies what he calls ‘vernacular altruism’, the pure motive of benefiting others. We rationally come to the decision to anonymously donate 10% of our income to charity. We decide to send a mourning card to a stranger. We decide to donate millions to fund our former school. To qualify as a vernacular altruistic act, the actor must consciously formulate the intention to benefit the other and the act must be motivated mainly out of consideration of another’s needs rather than one’s own. Giving money to charity could fit in this definition; often, without any immediate neural trigger, the benefactor proceeds towards parting with her resources to assist the beneficiary. When observing modern day charitable donations to organisations operating in a highly competitive market one cannot help but to be tickled by the question: how does this vernacular altruism function, and who is likely to portray it? As the charities have to battle for the favour of the altruist it is of even more interest to them to dissect the motivations and the psychological profile of their prospective contributors. With gaining insight into who is most likely to donate to your cause comes also the possibility of fine-tuning your marketing instruments to the relevant opinions and dispositions. Finally and most importantly, those who take the hardest blows in this world are all the more dependent on the consistent altruism of others. 6 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Problem statement The charity organizations intermediating between beneficiaries and benefactors rely on the selfless behaviour of others for their funds and staffing. Their proper financing is of the greatest importance, as they are the main counterforce to vast suffering. 1 So what makes their lifeblood - the people - behave pro-social? What makes them sacrifice self for the common good? Tscheulin and Lindenmeier (2005) captured several socio-demographic factors that influence the willingness to donate, but their work is far from comprehensive as it fails to discuss the views on charity organizations or personal dispositions. Sworonski (1997) came to several attitudinal and situational determinants relating to the willingness to donate organs, but his dependent variable is very limited whilst a broadly applicable one is useful considering the scope of the problem. Thus, many questions therefore arise concerning the concept of ‘giving’ and the altruistic behaviour it requires. Altruistic behaviour is what the actor could have done better for himself had he chosen to ignore the effect of his choice on others” (Margolis 1982). Who does this? Which sociodemographic variables account for the amounts donated to charity? Which innate characteristics produce altruistic behaviour? Does the individual tend to harbour a disposition towards altruistic behaviour which influences his propensity to give? And, is there an attitude to charity which fertilizes this behaviour and so makes it more probable that the individual will show donation behaviour. Which of those attitudes concerning charities impact the willingness to donate to them? This thesis will analyze previous research and Dutch respondents when posing the question: What determines the willingness to donate to charities? In order to answer the main question we will answer the following research questions: 1 Which socio-cultural factors affect the willingness to donate? Which individual dispositions towards charitable behavior affect the willingness to donate? Which attitudes toward charities affect the willingness to donate? A big part of the population depends on the altruistic behaviour of others. Half the world population lives on less than $2,50 a day. Every 4 minutes a child dies a preventable death. 1 billion people have inadequate access to water. Without the many donations, support and volunteered hours the situation could be worse. http://www.globalissues.org/article/26/poverty-facts-and-stats 7 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Definitions ‘What determines’; That which makes us give. The intrinsic disposition and environment which makes us liable to display altruistic behaviour. These driving forces behind altruistic behaviour where classified according to their nature: socio-demographic factors, the individual’s disposition towards altruistic behaviour and her attitude towards charity organizations. Socio-demographic factors include age, gender, income and education. The other antecedents are grouped within two categories of which the ‘attitudes towards charity organizations’ concern the individual's perception of the voluntary sector and organizational factors which might drive willingness to donate, whilst the ‘individual disposition’ focuses on those elements influencing any predisposition to support another person or group. ‘Willingness to donate’; DeVoe and Pfeffer (2007) defined the willingness to donate as the extent to which an individual shows readiness to volunteer or to donate money to a recipient who is collecting for a charitable organization. Willingness to donate thus measures to which extent the individual is willing to sacrifice time and money to the charity, two of the most fundamental resources available to man. They are related and to a certain extent exchangeable. ‘Charity’; Used in the broadest sense; an organization that collects money and other voluntary contributions of help for a worthy cause. (The extent to which a charity is worthy is often defined by an independent organisation, like www.cbf.nl) 8 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Purpose of the research According to Giving USA Foundation (2007), the charity market is huge: a $300 billion industry in the United States alone. Charity organizations argue that their single most prominent concern is to motivate people to donate money (West 2004). Then, the number of (American) volunteers steadily shrank over the past 4 years. (see for recent statistics the U.S. Labor Department statistics) The purpose of the research is to uncover the triggers and motives behind donation behaviour. The results will – if significant – be a foundation for fund-raising and staff recruitment. With this knowledge, charity organizations are provided with new arms in the battle for funds, staff and humanity. Academic literature previously focussed on the narrow boundaries of socio-cultural determinants (Tscheulin and Lindenmeier 2005) or more specific personal dispositions in the frame of a specific donations cause (Sworonski 1997). Current standards will be enriched with insights into the direct effect of the degree of spiritual focus, the amount of faith in humanity and other individual determinants of donation behaviour. Also, Sargeant’s (2007) research on the trust in charity will be inserted in the broader framework of charity donations. Then, socio-demographical predictions for other countries will be verified for the Netherlands, just as the Dutch respondent’s esteem of charities’ efficiency and effectiveness. . 9 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Chapter 2; Theoretical Framework The theoretical framework sketches the outline in which I wish to conduct the study. The independent indicators which are to indicate the willingness to donate are grouped by category, and from there the existing literature on the subjects is studied to distil the relevant expectations per predictor. First we will define what we are looking for with donation behaviour, then we decide on what is the most relevant to observe in measuring this. We conclude the chapter with a theoretical framework summarizing expected effects of the socio-cultural factors, the individual dispositions and the outlooks on charitable organisations on the willingness to volunteer and to donate money. Willingness to donate The distinction between vernacular altruism and the evolutionary altruism cannot always be clearly drawn. Holding the door open could be classified as altruism, just as refilling your neighbour’s teacup. As shown in the introduction, there is a distinct difference between altruistic behaviour in a state of emergency - which often equals unconscious decision-making (Hill 1984) – and the altruistic behaviour that is a deliberate, conscious choice (Sober 1988). As we investigate the driving force behind altruistic behaviour that manifests itself in aiding charitable organisations, we find that it is never done in a state of emergency, and shall therefore always be classified as vernacular altruism. Therefore, not all research on altruism is applicable, as not all portray this clear distinction between evolutionary or vernacular altruism. The search for the profile of the typical donator thus involves vernacular altruism only, and measuring the degree of this type of altruism is the challenge. The spectrum of previous research shows that this helping behaviour can be defined in many ways. We are interested in finding a general definition which is practical to use and aims at measuring the principal element of our research: the willingness to donate to charity organizations. 10 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Generally, the first method to come to mind is the measuring of actual helping behaviour, measurable through the formal channel of charity. Pandey (1979) defined it as “the extent to which an individual shows willingness and feelings about helping and donating money to a recipient who is collecting funds for beneficiaries.” Although this is a comprehensive description, the feelings about helping and donating money are a matter of scrutiny in this thesis: we measure the effect of feelings on willingness, and not the feelings themselves. DeVoe and Pfeffer (2007) observed that the resources time and money are related and to a certain extent exchangeable, such that it indicates the willingness to give trough a broader dimension. They defined it as “the extent to which an individual shows readiness to volunteer or to donate money to a recipient who is collecting for a charitable organization” (Van Diepen 2009) This is more specific and thus more useful. We will use it from here onwards, slightly amended as to fit our research better: The willingness to donate is the extent to which an individual shows readiness to donate time or money 11 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Socio-cultural factors Sketching the profile, the factors which should influence willingness to donate were divided into three main categories; The socio-cultural factors, depicting the background of the respondent, e.g. education & age. Earlier research has been done concerning these factors, and the only true novel addition was the living situation; whether someone lived in a village or a city. Factors concerning the personal dispositions that measure degrees of personal tendencies (or personality traits) like one’s inner life or selfish outlook on interpersonal relationships. Factors depicting the outlook on charities; whether someone has a positive or negative outlook on the effectiveness (do charities reach their goals) as the efficiency (do charities manage to get a lot of mileage out of the donated euro). Age Earlier American research tended to find that the amount of monetary donations increased with age but declined after the age of 65 (Edmundson, 1986; Danko and Stanley, 1986; Halfpenny, 1991). This is in line with research on altruism. Pro-social behavior emerges between the ages of 1 and 2 (ZahnWaxler e.a. 1992) and the quality and frequency of this behavior increases with age (Bar-Tal e.a. 1980). Why the willingness to donate diminishes after pension age has not been firmly established but can be explained by the fixed retirement age which comes with lower incomes and the fear of poverty. Edmunson (1986) found that those fearing their own financial security are less likely to contribute. We arrive at the following hypothesis, constituting the first part of our hypothesis on socio-demographics; H1a: Older respondents are more willing to support charities than younger donors 12 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Gender Females are slightly more altruistic. Concerning gender differences Austin (1979) found that men and women are equally likely to intervene at a high level of (potential) damage, but that women are quicker to intervene at lower levels, indicating a lower threshold for noticing. Eagly and Crowly (1986) discovered that females receive more help than males, perhaps because these are deemed to be less able to support themselves. This immediately applies to the support of charities and the willingness to donate time and money to them, as Jones and Posnet (1991) found that women tend to donate a larger amount in money. We thus suspect that women, as they tend to be more altruistic and often are more active in voluntary service are more willing to donate to charitable causes. Summarized in this hypothesis we shall verify that; H1b: Women are more willing to donate. Income Those given the most resources in a social game contribute the most to the shared cause; the rich donate more (Repoport 1988). If participants in an experiment see their contribution in the frame of being rich, they perceive their donation to be non-expensive, for the donation is a smaller part of their total wealth. Parting with resources would be less threatening to their own well-being than it would be to a poorer participant (Thaler 1999). Numerous authors have identified a positive correlation between an individual’s income and his level of donation (Kitchen and Dalton, 1990; Jencks, 1987). Jones and Posnett (1991) found that the probability of being a fixed donor was influenced by income and other antecedents, whilst the height of the donation was only affected by the income of the individual. Thus the following hypothesis should show; H1c: Respondents with a higher income are more willing to donate. 13 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Education Eisenberg and Miller (1987) proved that the more empathic people are, the more pro-social behaviour they display. If you try to see the world trough another man’s eyes it is more likely that you feel with that person. When that person is in need, you may feel like you want to relieve his need for his need is now yours. The correlation between empathy and pro-social behaviour does not imply that the pro-social behaviour always is altruistically motivated.2 One can argue that the level of education expands man’s worldview, and with that his empathy. Previous research by Kitchen and Dalton (1990) found the same connection. We verify; H1d: Respondents possessing higher education are more willing to donate. Urban or non-urban An often heard complaint is that the urban landscape lacks the outskirts’ warmth. The values and mood differs between small villages and inner cities: differences in attitude (and perhaps in altruistic behaviour) can be predicted by measuring the size of the community the respondent comes from. Glenn (1977) found that the community size of origin – or the size of the town one feels to have ‘come from’ – and the community size of residence is not as strong a predictor of behaviour as age or education (as examined above) but just a reliable predictor as family income or occupational prestige. This might not be entirely applicable to the Netherlands, as we hardly have any ‘true’ rural areas, but the message is just the same. Differences between education and income are obvious; according to Arcury & Christianson, (1993), rural citizens (defined as those living a certain distance from the urban area) are in general less education and enjoy a lower mean income. However, in the Netherlands many rich people make the switch from urban areas to rural areas, thus we do not expect this to influence the differences in willingness to donate. Examining rural-urban differences in the relationships of socio-demographic, social network, and lifestyle factors concerning mortality Motoki et al (2002) found that not only the educational level differs amongst the two groups, but that even marital status and the relationship to neighbors shows significant differences. Also, there exists what Bell (1992) calls the Urban-Rural continuum; that the ‘community’ idea is more 2 Remember; altruistic motivation being the vernacular altruism; rationally coming to the decision to part with resources and time for another’s benefit. Sober, E. (1988) “What is Evolutionary Altruism”, Canadian Journal on Philosophy, 14:75-99 14 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 characteristic of country places than cities. He analyses a countryside village and discovers that those living thither derive social-psychological benefits (like self-esteem and a sense of identity) as well as material benefits (multi-factor support from peers) from their living situation. The following table illustrates some differences; Factor Urban Area Rural Area Expected effect on charitable behavior by non-urban citizens Marital Status Relation with Less often More often Higher, as increased happiness should lead married married to more frequent charitable behavior. Less More Higher, for a good relationship with your neighbors direct peers can provide an incentive to donate to the general cause. Isolated individuals are less likely to contribute. Identity Other Rooted in living Higher, see relation with neighbors. situation Volunteering can be more rewarding when assisting closer peers. Summarizing, we hypothesize that there exists a significant difference in the charitable behavior of non-urban citizens versus those who live in urban areas. Those living in non-urban areas are more likely to donate as their individual disposition will prompt them earlier. In our study, we replace rural with non-urban and we define non-urban as those who live in a village with less than 25.000 inhabitants. Formulated in a sentence: H2: Non-urban respondents are more likely to donate than urban respondents 15 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Individual disposition towards altruistic behaviour The driving forces behind altruistic behaviour were classified according to their nature: sociodemographic factors, the individual’s disposition towards altruistic behaviour and her attitude towards charity organizations. The individual dispositions focuses on what the respondent esteems altruistic behaviour to be. It encompasses different layers of the personality and the individual environment as these all affect how one views the world and charitable behaviour. The individual disposition as a classification is rather efficient combined with the external outlook provided by ‘attitude towards charitable organisations’, as it sketches the totality of that which makes us liable to display altruistic behaviour. Faith in humanity Harrison et al (1998) described faith in humanity as the opinion that others are typically wellmeaning and reliable, the extent to which one believes that non-specific others are in general trustworthy. Previously, Klein and Simmons (1977) discovered that the amount of faith in humanity is an important predictor of whether the subject is willing to donate organs to a family-member. In their study, the only significant difference between kidney donors and non-donors was simply faith in humanity.3 74% of those willing to donate a kidney believed humanity was to be trusted, whilst 43% of the non-donors thought as such. It was concluded that the less faith in one’s peers someone has, the less willing he was to alleviate the suffering for the sufferer was to some extent deserving his or her demise. Those who had a high trust in humanity were thus inclined to donate a kidney. This tractate will observe if it is also of influence to the willingness to donate to a charity. This could mean that the moment trust in humanity is breached, donations suffer. One could say that only those who deem mankind trustworthy are inclined to contribute to the common good, because those who deem mankind wretched esteem their misery as a proper punishment, or the sufferer as somehow responsible. In this line, we propose; H3: Respondents with faith in humanity are more willing to donate. 3 One of the questions that measured this construct; “Are people inclined to help others or themselves?” See; Simmons, R.G. Klein, S.D. & Simmons, R.L. (1977) “The Gift of Life: The Social and Psychological Impact of Organ Donation”, New York: Wiley 16 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Spirituality Then again, the mere fact that some population members do not reciprocate – and could be labelled untrustworthy - does not seem to make ‘co-operators’ discard their endeavours to contribute to the collective (Kamer et al 1986). Thus, after witnessing the evidence that some members of the population are not willing to return the favour, these people still persist in their unselfishness. This indicates that a generosity is more of a disposition than the result of the behaviour and situation of peers. And yes, as religious and spiritual precepts often accentuate the obligation to donate money and ‘love thy neighbour’, Edmundson (1986) and Halfpenny (1990) discovered that respondents considering religion to be important to them were more likely to give to charity. Where religious dispositions have been found to increase the willingness to donate, current society is not as involved in church as were the 1970’s respondents of the research. This signals the need for a more contemporary construct. Nowadays, evaluating the degree of spiritual maturity has become a prominent subject in the media and various professions and disciplines. As Moberg, (2002) advises, we therefore broaden the research to the - perhaps more subjective term – ‘spirituality’, and see if those who coin themselves as spiritual donate more to charity than those who shun the concept. We use the broader term spirituality as it encompasses more than religion and will highlight any attitudinal dispositions more effectively. Those involved in practices involving higher planes are more likely to adjust their behavior towards the commandments imposed by the various disciplines. As the commandment of ‘love thy neighbor’ is universal and can be coined as a common element we expect the spiritual respondent to be more willing to donate. The commandment to practice altruistic behavior can be found in all spiritual walks of life. Therefore, practicing spirituality should result in more willingness to donate. We formulate our expectation as follows; H4: Respondents to which spirituality is important are more willing to donate. 17 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Selfishness The personal attitude towards others can be expected to have a significant impact on the willingness to donate. By studying the characters of those who rescued Jews in Nazi Europe Oliner and Oliner(1988) found that – compared to non-rescuers – they had higher ethical values, beliefs in equity, greater pity/empathy and often saw all people as equal. Staub (1978) found more character traits in observing the charitable: high in self-esteem, high in competence, high in internal locus of control, low in need for approval and high in moral development. According to these studies, when the individual is more advanced in moral development he is more likely to show both charitable and even heroic behaviour. These studies do not advance the depth of their constructs, as the measurement of ‘ethical judgments’ is often done arbitrarily. They merely arouse a suspicion that those with superior ethical judgment are more willing to help others. In measuring this ‘ethical judgment’, Reidenbach and Robin (1990) state that the breadth and complexity of the construct dictate a broad and complex construct. What is this ethical judgment? Individuals often use more than one rationale in making ethical judgments to which different weights are assigned according to the situation. To arrive at a measurable notion of the individual’s developed morality we follow the notion of selfishness developed by Reidenbach and Robin (1990); the extent to which the individual considers only the consequences of her actions to herself. Then, after observing this notion we test if the statement above is correct; those who are less selfish – those who do not consider solely the impact of their actions on themselves – can be expected to be more likely to donate time and money to others. H5: Selfish respondents are less willing to donate. 18 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Attitude towards charity organizations. Over the past two decades the functioning of charities has changed dramatically, with a radical shift towards professionalization in delivering services and fundraising (Saxton 2004). The websites are flashy and personnel are better trained. However, according to Ebeling (2005) problems in the voluntary sector have been accumulating for years. The amount of charities increased rapidly and many people enjoy the great personal benefits being adhered to them. Sargeant and Stephen (2002) state that public trust in the voluntary sector has collapsed after some severe incidents, exposing the lack of proper supervision. In a recent attempt by Precision Marketing to chart the percentage of the British public that had confidence in the charities, a study revealed that it decreased by 9 percentage points to 42%. This is an improvement; in 1997 only 33% of the British population had 'a great deal' or 'quite a lot of confidence in UK charities. (See the Henley Centre’s rapport) Many charity supporters believe that the organization supporting their cause is to some extent corrupt or not directing the resources provided to those for whom the donation was intended (Bruce and Baily 1992). Charity donors suspect that only 65 pence per £1 of any amount they donate will end up at the cause. For non-donors the score is 45p for every pound (Sargeant 1999). Efficiency Herzlinger (1996) sketched several scandals illustrating 3 major image problems the voluntary sector has been wrestling with, not entirely unjust; 1. Ineffectiveness; the charities do not accomplish their social missions. Hospitals that reject poor patients. The hoarding of money. 2. Inefficiency; the charities get little mileage out of your euro. Less than 50% ends up at the cause.4 (A charity is more efficient as the percentage of the donated money that ends up at the beneficiary grows.) 3. Private inurnment; the heads of the tax-exempt organizations attain excessive benefits for themselves. Managers and employees use the charity’s resources for their own benefit. Executives earning more than $1m in compensation. 4 Economic theory suggests that, given the nature of the "market" in which they operate, private charities are inherently inefficient. Marshall, G.P. 1978. “The Control of Private Charities.”, Public Administration 19 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 These handicaps are hardly visible because most charities’ performance is shrouded in a veil of secrecy. Besides, Bird (1983) observed that the majority of those interested in charity focuses on their service functions and not on analyzing their financial affairs. Interesting is the observation by Lu-Hsu and Liang (2005), who found that only 25% of charity donors concern themselves about the financial management of charities. They conclude that when charities communicate information, they should focus on images related to the reputation of the charity and the donation recipients for they produce more interest. (40% of the respondents indicate they matter) Then again, often used (as by Sargeant 1999) to measure charity sector support is the question: “how much of your euro ends up at the beneficiary?” This directly concerns financial management. We reason that those who deem charities efficient – expecting a lot of their euro ending up aiding the beneficiary – are more willing to donate, for they will consider their euro well spent. H6: Respondents deeming charities efficient are more willing to donate. Administrative tolerance Here, we measure on one hand the amount that the Dutch estimate to end up at the beneficiary, while on the opposing end their opinion on what is an acceptable amount for a charity to use in their organizational overhead. Administrative tolerance refers to the threshold people have concerning the amount the charity is allowed to spend on upkeep of the organization and acquiring funds. This personal judgment can reverberate because the willingness to donate is influenced also by the willingness to pay for the charity organizations’ staffing and fundraising activities. Harvey & McCrohan et al (1988) revealed not only a ‘meaningful relationships between perceived organization efficiency and the level of giving’, but also imply that – because of the value agents put on efficiency – there could exist a difference in the amount people find acceptable to spend on the mentioned organizational cost factors. In a way this variable resembles the previous, but it also enables the analysis of a new element’s influence: is there a significant difference in the amount that people deem acceptable in spending on administrative expenditures, and does this influence also the willingness to donate? 20 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 H7: Respondents upholding a low acceptance threshold for administrative expenditures are less willing to donate Effectiveness The construct of effectiveness measures the degree to which the respondent beliefs the average charities achieve the goals they aim to achieve. According to Sargeant (2002) it is the individual trusting the charitable organization to realize its mission. It follows logically that those who suspect charities to lose sight of their ultimate aims are less willing to support their ineffective striving. Also, evidence suggests that donors who perceive charity organizations as effective are more likely to donate to them. (Schlegelmilch, 1988; Yavas et al, 1980; Harvey, 1990; Harvey and McCrohan, 1988; Mindak and Bybee, 1971). H8: Individuals suspecting charities to be effective are more willing to donate. Interaction: selfishness & effectiveness In between factors it will be interesting to see which effects reinforce the others, and how personal dispositions interact with the outlooks on charity. I will check different interactions between the dispositions and outlooks, but am specifically interested in the moderating effect of the co-play between selfishness and effectiveness. For example, it is probable that those respondents who are selfish are expected to show less consideration for the effectiveness of charity organizations – their capacity to alleviate suffering. That is to say; it is possible that both independents will act strongly enough as to amplify resistance against the concept of giving, but just as likely that those not caring for others neither care about the fact that others do care. Or, caring about others (i.e. not being selfish) has a stronger effect on willingness to donate when we also view the organisation as achieving its goals. The organisation will be regarded as a suitable channel to promulgate the need to help others. H9: The effect for ‘effectiveness’ on Willingness to donate is stronger for respondents scoring low on ‘selfishness’ 21 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Conceptual model The conceptual model consists of the nine hypothesis formulated in the previous part. It aims to clarify the various relationships that were sketched previously, and provide a clear oversight into what is being researched. 22 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Chapter 3; Empirical elements We arrive with a total of nine hypotheses and a dependent which can be separated in 2 elements; the donation of time and the donation of money. This is best done with an ordinal regression, as the dependent variable is a categorical. First we furnish the general model from the hypotheses we constructed, then we decide how to measure the several different constructs in a survey. The hypotheses formulated in the previous chapter can be summarized in the following equation, with the different independents and interaction effect influencing the dependent willingness to donate; γ (willingness to donate) = β0 + β1*age + β2*gender + β3*income + β4*education + β5*urban + β6*faith + β7*spirit + β8*selfish + β9*efficient + β10*administrative + β11*effective + β12*interaction + Error Explanatory variable Willingness to donate Faith in humanity Spirituality Selfish Efficiency Administrative tolerance Effectiveness Age Gender Income Education Living Fashion Categories in which respondents indicate what they have donated or volunteered in the last year. Construct measuring the degree to which the respondent deems people in general to be selfish in opposition to altruistic Construct measuring the degree to which spirituality is important in the respondent’s life Construct measuring the degree to which the respondent considers only the effects of his actions on himself Which percentage of the donated euro is suspected to end up with the beneficiary Which percentage of the donated euro is allowed to be used for administrative purposes Construct measuring the degree to which the respondent beliefs the average charity achieves the goals they aim to achieve. Measured in years Binary: Female = 1 Male = 2 Monthly income in euro’s 5 dummy variables indicating the level of finished education: ‘Basisschool’/’Middelbare school’/’MBO’/’HBO’/’Universiteit’ Amount of inhabitants in the habitat 23 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Questionnaire design We collect data on the different constructs by forging a questionnaire. The elements therein measuring the constructs consist of several questions which were borrowed from previous research. Questions aiming to measure the constructs were translated in Dutch as to ensure the full comprehension by the Dutch respondents. The questions concerning statements are evaluated on a 5-point bipolar scale. Then, the data is collected during the noon hour of a busy shopping day in the centre of Rotterdam, addressing a heterogeneous public as to ensure representativeness. The questionnaire is attached as an appendix. Willingness to donate Lee and Chang (2007) found that an efficient way to measure the support of charities in general is to ask respondents whether or not they have donated in the past to a charity, and whether that is time or money. This circumvents the measuring of good intentions, which – as one well knows – can deviate significantly from actual helping behavior. We therefore ask whether respondents have donated time or money in the past to a charitable organisation, where we take Van Diepen (2009) as a guide. Lui and Aaker (2008) has been done about the form and order in which the questions are asked: “How much time are you willing to donate?” and “How much money are you willing to donate?” Research has indicated however, that the order in which you ask these questions is of importance. Asking the individual for time first primes him in an emotional state for he will imagine himself spending time with the beneficiaries; he will be more inclined to give money afterwards. The other way around primes him into a scarcity state; a state less fruitful for donation propensity! Urban Asked is how many citizens inhabit the city or village where the respondent lives. Other research concerning rural areas and non-rural areas often used the absolute distance from the nearest city centre as measure to determine whether the respondent lived in a remote area. In Holland, where nearly all towns are relatively proximate, it could be wiser to inquire to the amount of inhabitant in a city. This will not only be easier to estimate by the respondent, but also more accurate in determining whether the respondent lives in a non-urban area: a definition already exists. This question is therefore not to be found in previous literature, so I developed it myself; Do you live in a city or a village?; City (more than 25000 inhabitants Village (less than 25000 inhabitants) 24 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Faith in humanity Faith in humanity means that one believes that others are typically well-meaning and reliable. Thus, the ‘faith in humanity’ as a construct measures the degree to which the respondent deems people in general to be selfish in opposition to altruistic (Harrison, D., Cummings, L., Chervany, N., 1998). The 5-point scale questions aiming to measure this construct are borrowed from Glaeser et al 2000. Selfishness Reidenbach and Robin (1990) defined selfishness as “the extent to which the individual considers only the consequences of her actions to herself.” They developed several questions which measure if this is the case. They are included on a 5-point bipolar scale. Spirituality McGinn (1993) claims that spirituality is like obscenity; we may not know how to define it, yet we know it when we see it, and the “fickleness” of academics’ inability to provide precise definitions has never prevented people from practicing it. It is also possible to draw over 31 definitions of religiousness and 40 of spirituality from past social science publications (Zinnbauer et al 1999). Whatever scale you will pick, every spirituality scale will reflect “only limited aspects of a highly complex, multidimensional, and largely nonmaterial ontological reality.” The 5-point scale questions aiming to measure this construct were derived from the Fetzer Institute’s Workbook. Efficiency & Administrative Tolerance When measuring the perceived efficiency of charities we can use the method used most often in research on this subject; a percentage of the euro which is suspected to end up at the beneficiary (See Schlegelmilch, 1997 and Sargeant, 1999). Effectiveness The construct of effectiveness measures the degree to which the respondent beliefs the average charities achieve the goals they aim to achieve. The following 5-point scale questions aim to measure this construct, which were derived from Sargeant (2002). 25 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Results Of the 130 questionnaires taken I had to discard 13 due to severe incompleteness or obvious mockeries. The 117 remaining questionnaires were used to constitute the regression. In these were some missing values, a second round was conducted to supplement the original dataset towards 127 useable respondents. Factor analysis The first essential step in processing the results was to produce factor analysis for the different latent variables. As you cannot measure spirituality or selfishness directly, we identify groups or clusters of variables that point towards these constructs. Thus, the factor analysis describes how coherent the questions are – or how well they link to the construct, after we posed the questions. For example, the ‘selfishness’ construct, consisting of 4 questions is scrutinized to discover whether those 4 questions measure the same construct. The interpretability of factors was improved through varimax rotation, and resulted in the following results5 based on Eigenvalues greater than one and 64.3% of the variance explained: Component Q1 Q2 Q3 Q4 # Construct questions Spirituality 0,850 0,906 0,914 0,804 4/4 Effectiveness 0,780 0,709 0,806 0,867 4/4 Faith in Humanity 0,078 0,853 0,832 0,745 4/4 Selfishness 0,634 0,532 0,823 0,639 4/4 Of all the questions the singular (People are inclined to help themselves before others) failed to correlate with any of the factors. With hind knowledge –thus after posing the question - it was decided that this question was to be deleted due to its concern with ‘helping’ and not with ‘faith in one’s peers’. After removal of the faulty question we arrived at the following, based on Eigenvalue’s greater than 1 and 67.9% of the variance explained: Component Q1 Q2 Q3 Q4 # Construct questions Spirituality 0,852 0,905 0,916 0,803 4/4 Effectiveness 0,789 0,716 0,805 0,869 4/4 0,853 0,835 0,744 3/4 0,543 0,829 0,651 4/4 Faith in Humanity Selfishness 5 0,637 Only the summaries are portrayed here. See appendix for full analysis ‘Rotated Component Matrix’ 26 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Reliability test Then we continued with a reliability analysis with the found factors6. The Cronbach’s Alpha, hereunder depicted, delivers the internal consistency of the constructs. If a question turns out to diminish the statistic, it can be opted for removal, with a possible healthier Cronbach’s α as a result. Construct Initial Cronbach’s α Adjustments Final Cronbach’s α Spirituality 0,902 None 0,902 Effectiveness 0,765 None 0,765 Faith in Humanity 0,622 Minus Q1 0,750 Selfishness 0,611 Minus Q1 & Q2 0,643 Including Q1 & Q2 0,611 The factor faith in humanity included a faulty question (‘People are inclined to help themselves before others’), so that after deleting the culprit the Alpha rose to the more desirable level of 0,750. Regarding the ‘selfishness’ construct, I considered it wise to include the two questions of the ‘selfishness’ construct, as they were considered to contribute to the overall stability of the concept; 4 questions inherently indicate more than 2. After analyzing the reliability of scales we took the average such that one value per construct remains. 6 See appendix ‘Reliability Statistics 27 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Descriptive statistics Of the supplemented dataset of N = 143 (the initial dataset had been found to lack too many usable responses), some 127 turned out to be useful. The frequency tabs7 left to conclude that the sample could be regarded as representative for the population. Of the total valid dataset, females dominated with roughly 60% of the population. Gender Income Living situation Education The income groups selected seem to be adequately represented, considering also the location of the survey; down-town in the shopping quarter. Median income seems to emerge from the survey, which is consistent with population estimations. The level of education shown here-under reveals the same confirmation of representativeness, as the populace is smoothly distributed amongst the education classes. 7 Refer to ‘Frequency tabs’ in the appendix 28 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Donated amounts Donated amounts were equally spread amongst population, where it was interesting to observe that only 1/3 shunned the concept of donating. Then, of all the respondents, we found that the average expected amount to end up at the beneficiary equals €0,56, meaning that a the public opinion can be judged to be fairly distrusting. Sargeant (1999) concluded likewise when he found that British respondents believed that only £0,65 of every pound was actually devoted to the cause. We also found that the average administrative tolerance was €0,16, indicating a wide gap (€0,28)between the perceived efficiency and the demanded efficiency of charity organizations. Volunteered hours Volunteered hours were fairly distributed, with the remark that respondents who labored more than 10 hours exceeded respondents volunteering 10 hours. One could say that the categories should be wider spread in an extra study. 29 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Regression: outline Upon the initial estimation of representativeness and the judgment that the data was fit for regression (refer to the checklist below), we conducted an ordinal regression with the data, for we can rank the values of the amounts donated but neither a real nor stable distance between the categories of the variables of interest (see above). The following assumptions as required for the ordinal regression were checked8; One ordinal dependent variable (either donated money or donated time) The relationship of the predictor to the ordinal dependent is independent of the category of the dependent. Adequate sample size (workable sample of 127 respondents) The construct of which the faulty questions were filtered were summarized and inserted into the equation. Inserted as factors and covariates were; Factors (categorical independent variables) Covariates (continuous numeric independent variables) Gender Age Urban Income Education Faith in humanity construct average Spirituality construct average Selfishness construct average Effectiveness construct average Efficiency Administrative tolerance 8 For an overview of what an ordinal regression requires to be reliable: McCullagh, Peter. (1980). Regression models for ordinal data. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 42 30 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate money General Model Variable H1a Age H1b Gender H1c Income H1d Education H2 Living H3 Faith in humanity H4 Spirituality H5 Selfishness H6 Effectiveness H7 Efficiency H8 Administrative H9 Interaction Pseudo R-Square β 0.081 -0.227 0.135 0.246 -0.198 -0.216 0.095 0.065 0.700 0.041 -0.002 0,460 P .000 0.540 0.294 0.208 0.632 0.327 0.566 0.824 0.017 000.0 0.822 - Dispositions β .047 -.053 .267 .290 -.148 -.103 .150 -.240 0,279 P .001 .875 .025 .107 .703 .606 .327 .384 - Perceptions β .087 -.229 .100 .288 -.251 .640 .042 -.002 0,457 P .000 .530 .406 .128 .533 .021 .000 .846 - Interaction β .070 -.293 .177 .226 -.432 -.231 .132 3.274 2.860 .042 -.003 -1.016 0,492 P .000 .434 .175 .251 .311 .303 .432 .011 .001 .000 .733 .010 General model Commencing with willingness to donate money we discovered that –confirming earlier studies - the elderly were more prepared to donate money.9 The effect is not tremendously strong, however significant: the β of 0,081 means that the monetary donation category in which a person falls (1-7) increases with every year the person ages. Also, the larger the figure estimated to end up at the beneficiary, just as the more effective the charity was deemed to be, the more was donated. This also goes for efficiency; if the respondent thought that a lot of his euro ended up at the beneficiary he was more willing to donate. This matches the hypothesis: the donator will get more value for money, as his euro is well spend and makes an impact. Thus, taking the model as a whole (for now without the interaction), we observe that it is mainly the external perceptions towards efficiency and effectiveness of the charity sector which contribute to the likeliness of a regular respondent to donate to the industry. Therefore, improving upon these factors can increase contributions made, as it is mainly negative perceptions on the effectiveness of charities which stand between an individual and his donation. Advertising/fundraising campaigns should accentuate on the difference made – highlight their effectiveness – as to break down any negative attitude towards their capacity to actually aid those requiring assistance. 9 Refer to appendix; ‘Regression: Willingness to donate money’ 31 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Dispositions However, these effects alter slightly when separately analyzing the individual disposition factors or the attitudes towards charity as separate entities in conjunction with the two dependents. When shifting the focus towards the individual disposition, the dataset confirms previous analysis, even now the factors of the individual disposition are unhindered by their external perspective counterparts; there, by taking a purer model undistorted by any of the added independents on the external view on charity, the model still sheds the same light on those factors belonging to the personal disposition.10 They matter little when it concerns to donating money – that is to say, the personal factors hypothesized to contribute to the willingness to donate money turn out to be insignificant. Attitudes towards charity When concerning only the attitudes towards charitable behavior, the same effects surfaced as were observed in the general analysis. Thus, in the purer model only considering the external effects our initial suppositions were confirmed; efficiency and effectiveness are the key elements in persuading people to donate. Interaction effect included; Afterwards, the ordinal modeled was altered for to the main effects, the interaction effect of selfishness and efficiency was inserted, as to scan for any additional factors; And indeed, the interaction exposes that those tending to be unselfish [the reversed polarity in the questionnaire necessitates caution, as the measured construct highlights the selfish individuals by inquiring whether they agree with selfish paradigm statements] whilst considering the charities to be effective are even more likely to donate money to a charity. The two factors reinforce each other. As it elaborates on the previous model without violating its conclusions, we can assume these results are fairly stable. Other interactions between the personal dispositions and the outlooks on effectiveness turned out to be insignificant. 10 Refer to appendix; ‘Regression: Willingness to donate money’. Construct ‘Faith’ is colored yellow herein for the significance did not attain to 5%, but did not surpass 10%. 32 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate time General Model Variable H1a Age H1b Gender H1c Income H1d Education H2 Living H3 Faith in humanity H4 Spirituality H5 Selfishness H6 Effectiveness H7 Efficiency H8 Administrative H9 Interaction Pseudo R-Square β -0.016 -0.511 0.029 0.184 0.275 0.043 0.387 0.027 0.632 -0.003 -0.01 0.123 P 0.331 0.194 0.833 0.376 0.532 0.853 0.027 0.930 0.043 0.742 0.314 - Dispositions β -.014 -.265 .057 .037 .503 .070 .458 -.060 0.085 P .370 .462 .655 .843 .213 .742 .006 .837 - Perceptions β -.007 -.472 -.067 .316 .102 .723 -.002 -.013 0.077 P .643 .215 .596 .116 .809 .019 .789 .203 - Interaction β -.022 -.527 .045 .184 .196 .049 .398 1.556 1.604 -.003 -.011 -.467 0.132 P .216 .181 .745 .380 .661 .834 .025 .249 .081 .729 .301 .249 General model Continuing forward, when do people volunteer – donate their time? We inserted first the factors and asked for the main effects, before requesting the validation of our hypothesized interaction effect. The data shows that those involved in spiritual matter are more likely to volunteer. Better yet, for every point towards a spiritual outlook on life (on the 5-point bipolar scale as in the questionnaire) people are more willing to volunteer for a charitable cause. Where traditional research focused on donation behavior relating to money, this paves the way for the conclusion that marketing efforts aiming to recruit volunteers for charities have more success when targeting the spiritually focused. That those volunteering also deem their work effective is not the strangest of findings; those considering charities as not accomplishing their goals are not like to volunteer for its tasks – it would be ineffective. However, the same conclusion can be drawn as above; where volunteers are wanted, the organization (apart from the industry image, for now) has to retain the impression of being effective. It weighs heavy in the decision to participate in such work, and a negative outlook on the possible contribution to be made could serve as a barrier for signing up. Dispositions & attitudes towards charity A refocus on one type of predictor does not change the results dramatically. The same conclusions hold. 33 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Interaction effect included Here, the results are somewhat distorted by inserting the interaction effect. Interesting to observe is that the fit of the model teeters on the brink of insignificance, a downgrade in the light of the previous analysis. Spirituality stand valiantly upright, establishing the earlier finding that the spiritual are more like to volunteer even more firmly. Regardless of the new addition, we can safely conclude that the factors in the first analysis (free from the interaction effect) spirituality and effectiveness, both significantly contribute to the willingness to donate your time. Other interactions between the personal dispositions and the perceptions of charity were found to be insignificant.11 11 Interactions between dispositions and perceptions were checked; see appendix 34 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Concluding This research sprouted from the desire to discover what makes people donate. Having insight into this phenomenon is of great value to those who rely on their help, and those who wish to help but require the funds and time from donators. The research focused thus on the vernacular altruistic act, and what instigated this. Based on previous research 3 main categories of factors which should influence the propensity to donate time & money: The socio-cultural factors, depicting the background of the respondent, based on previous research, with the addition of the living situation. Factors concerning the personal disposition; spiritual focus, selfish tendencies and the faith one has in his peers. Factors depicting the outlook on charities; focussing on the effectiveness (do charities reach their goals) as the efficiency (do charities manage to get a lot of mileage out of the donated euro)? Also, how much is the charitable organisation allowed to spend on administrative expenditures? The findings were as follows, with the outlooks on charity explaining significantly more of the donated amounts than the personal dispositions; Category Socio-cultural factors Personal dispositions Charity perceptions Dispositions * perceptions Variable H1a Age H1b Gender H1c Income H1d Education H2 Living H3 Faith in humanity H4 Spirituality H5 Selfishness H6 Effectiveness H7 Efficiency H8 Administrative H9 Interaction Money donations Supported Volunteered hours Supported Supported Supported Supported 35 Supported Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Discussion In general Many factors turned out to be insignificant. Overall, we could say that the perceptions on charities weighed far more in the decision to make a donation. Education played with the edge of significance several times, but was not found a consistent predictor of donations. It was later found to be dominated by the other factors. Administrative tolerance was also found not to be a factor, even when removing those of whom I suspected not understanding the question. (>80 could be spend on such costs) This means that administrative tolerance – as a manifestation from concern for financial management – is not considered when choosing whether or not to make a donation. The living situation of the respondent also did not seem to matter. I presume there were conflicting forces at work – yes, the non-urban respondents enjoy in general a lower income and thus donate less but it was most likely offset by an opposing influx like a greater sense of community. Of course, many other factors could influence the willingness to donate in a more or less subtle way. Further research has to inquire into this question in a more detailed fashion. On donating money Those considering the charity to be efficient and effective were most willing to donate money. The interaction effect sketches the reinforcing effect of both the individual disposition of being unselfish as the external opinion that charities are effective. That is to say, someone with and an unselfish disposition and a positive outlook on charities’ efficiency is even more likely to donate. These results could be slightly skewered: the question inquiring about the degree of selfishness is less likely to be answered by a selfish person. Therefore, those being selfish suffer a high probability of being underrepresented in the survey. The conclusion can be drawn that perceived effectiveness (average effectiveness score: 3.2 out of 5) plays the strongest role in the decision to sacrifice money for the common good. It weighs heavier than age, education or all the other factors included in this study. Charities could draw the inference that they should advertise their effectiveness as their strongest trait, since their image with the donating populace has been battered trough several scandals. Currently, advertising focuses on the need of the beneficiaries, like their hunger or their lack of education or healthcare. If advertising would focus on the results (effectiveness) and the little which it took to bring about a positive change (efficiency) it could have a lot more effect and bring in far greater revenues than the current method. 36 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 On donating time Those with a spiritual disposition and a belief in the effectiveness of charities are more likely to donate their hours to a cause. This is interesting as the effect was not found in donating money. An explanation could be that those involved in charity in order to help their neighbour feel better when they are actively participating. Seeing the fruit of their labour directly could be more rewarding to them than the rather distant transfer of money. This could be used for several advertising approaches. As your pool of volunteers will consist of many spiritually focussed, you could focus recruitment on target groups with this trademark. Also, when recruiting, accentuating the effectiveness – how greatly you can contribute by volunteering little above all other traits can make put extra weight in the scale. Interesting in both cases was the observation that not all of the previous research was confirmed; not all the socio-demographic variables turned up significant. This could be because similar studies are rare in the Netherlands and represent different populations but it is more likely that the other factors dominated the dependent. We found just that when running a control regression. Concerning the previous research it was interesting to find that indeed for money donations old predictors were verified, but for time donations the known factors did not come into play: Money donations Variable H1a Age H1b Gender H1c Income H1d Education Volunteered hours β P β P .052 .000 -.003 .853 .056 .865 -.135 .696 .238 .027 -.051 .654 .375 .025 .133 .438 And thus we come to the fruition of the project with the invaluable conclusion that in the contemporary highly competitive charity market it is highly recommendable to single out the main reasons for donating time and money. We saw that perceptions on effectiveness and efficiency highly influence the propensity to donate, and charities would do well to focus their advertisements on removing the barriers which stand between the individual and his donation. Because after all, the more one is willing to sacrifice for the common good, the more this world will profit. 37 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Limitations The researched literature was from many different geographical areas, whereas our survey was conducted in the Netherlands. It should be representative as we do not examine any cross-cultural specific factors. Then, we asked whether the individual has donated time & money to charitable causes. Despite the research being anonymous & confidential, it could be that some respondents feel the urge to nudge up the actual amount of their altruistic behaviour. Also, when asked about prosocial behaviour the respondent could be tempted to list other behaviours than actually shown. Anonymity should have mitigated this effect, but it is to be taken into account. The location where the survey was conducted could also have posed a minor infringement on the striving towards representativeness. Not all charitable persons are to be found in the centre of Rotterdam, and perhaps slightly disinterested in shopping. The survey asking about selfishness deserves some scrutiny as the survey was a voluntary service to the team; most likely the most selfish people are unrepresented as they probably refused to participate in the survey. 38 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Future research This is an incredible opportunity for charities, as further research can inquire into the practical consequences of our discussion. One could confront respondents with different advertisements, aiming to portray same beneficiary. The first advert could sketch how bad the beneficiary’s situation is. The second advert could aim to describe how easily the boy was helped by a minor influx of money. (An accent on efficiency in expenditures – i.e. 90% of your money ends up with them – could harvest big support). Then, the respondents are asked what they deem to be a better destination for their money. If this research proves to be positive and we find that the effect of help weighs more than the intensity of the suffering in the decision to donate, we could see an entire shift in the focus of charity advertising campaigns. Follow-up research concerning volunteering can focus the success rate of recruitment efforts among those absorbed in a spiritual activity at the time of contact? Are those spoken to after a church mass more likely to contribute than those after another event? And, what if recruitment advertisements focus on the contribution to be made with little effort instead of the great necessity to contribute? In general more research has to be done about the motives for donating time. Volunteering may not be as easy quantifiable as donating money, but reasons behind this kind of altruistic behaviour deserve to be studied, for the very same reason that its monetary counterpart is: so many people depend on other people’s efforts for a decent existence. More sophisticated models with more variables should be constructed after some exploratory research into what drives people in striving for a better world. 39 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Thanksgiving For the inexhaustible patience and dedication of Ron van Schie, on whom I could always count during the project. Even after the abandonment of the original plan, the same encouragement and enthusiasm persisted throughout the many months. My grandmother Elisabeth Bosch for reviewing the thesis and its language. For my friends Rolf and Jill, who were kind enough to survey with me in the streets of Rotterdam. For my family for supporting me on the academic journey. 40 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 References Aronoff, J & Wilson, J.P. (1984) “Personality in the Social Process”, Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum Arucry, T.A. & Christianson, E.H. (1993) “Rural-Urban Differences in Environmental Knowledge and Actions”, Journal of Environmental Education, 25(1) 19-25 Austin, W. (1979) “Sex Differences in Bystander Intervention in a Theft”, Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, Volume 37(11) 2110-2120 Baily, A. & Bruce, M. (1992) "United Way: The Fallout After The Fall", Chronicle Of Philanthropy, Vol. 4(3) 30-32 Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., Leiser, T. (1980) “The Development of Altruistic Behaviour: Empirical Evidence”, Developmental Psychology, Vol. 16(5) 516-524 Bell, M. (1992) “The Fruit of Difference; the Rural-Urban Continuum as a System of Identity”, Rural Sociology, Vol. 57(1) 65-82 Bird, P. (1983) “What’s Wrong With Charity Accounts?” Accountancy, Vol. 94(1084) 55 (1p) Britten, R.J. (1992) “Rates of DNA Sequence Evolution Differ Between Taxonomic Groups”, Science, Vol. 231, Iss. 4744 Buckley, C. (2007) “Man is Rescued by Stranger on Subway Tracks”, NyTimes.Com, retrieved on 07/04/2009 Crawford, C. Smith, M & Krebs, D (1987) “Socio-biology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications” Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum Danko, W. D. & Stanley, T. J. (1986) “Identifying and Reaching the Donation Prone Individual: A Nationwide Assessment”, Journal of Professional Services Marketing, 2(1/2) 117-122 DeVoe, S. E. & Pfeffer, J. (2007), “When Time Is Money: The Effect of Hourly Payment on the valuation of Time,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 104(1) 1-13 Diamantopoulos, A., Schlegelmilch, B. & Love, A. (1993) “Giving to Charity: Determinants of Cash Donations trough Prompted Giving”, Maketing Theory and Application, Vol. 4 23-54 41 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Diepen, M. (2009) “Dynamics and Competition in Charitable Giving”, Erasmus University of Rotterdam, PhD Thesis Ebeling, A. (2005) “The Coming Charity Crackdown.” Forbes, August Edmundson, B. (1986), ‘Who Gives to Charity?’, American Demographics, Iss. 11 45-49 Halfpenny, P. (1990) “Charity Household Survey 1988/8”, Charities Aid Foundation: Tonbridge Eisenberg, N. & Miller, P.A. (1987) “The Relation of Empathy to Prosocial and Related Behaviours” Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 101(1) 91-119 Eagly, A.H. & Crowley, M. (1986) “Gender and Helping Behaviour: A Meta-Analysis Review of the Social Psychological Literature”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 100(3) 283-308 Glenn, N. (1977) “Rural-Urban differences in Attitudes and Behaviour in the United States”, The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol. 429(1) 36-50 Giving USA Foundation (2007), “Annual Report on Philanthropy 2006,” http://www.aafrc.org Retrieved on 07/05/2009 Glaeser, E., Liabson, D., Scheinkman, J. & Soutter, C. (2000) “Measuring Trust”, The Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol 115(3) 881-846 Halfpenny, P. (1991) “The 1989/90 Charity Household Survey”, In Charity Trends, Charities Aid Foundation, Tonbridge. Harvey, J. & McCrohan, K. (1988) “Fund-raising Costs - Societal Implications for Philanthropies and Their Supporters’, Business and Society, Vol. 3(1) 14-28 Harvey, J. W. (1990) “Benefit Segmentation for Fund-Raisers”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 18(1) 77-89 Harrison, D., Cummings, L. & Chervany, N. (1998) “Initial Trust Formation in New Organizational Relationships”, The Academy of Management Review, Vol. 28(3) Henley Centre, (1997) "Planning for Social Change", Henley Centre, London Herzlinger, R. (1996) "Can Public Trust in Nonprofits and Governments Be Restored?", Harvard Business Review, Vol 74(3) 97-107 42 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Hill, J. (1984) “Human Altruism and Sociocultural Fitness”, Journal of Social and Structural Biology, Vol 32(1) 1-22 Hoffman, M. L. (1981) “Is altruism part of human nature?”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 1(4) 232-339 Jones, A. & Posnett, J. (1991) “Charitable Donations by UK Households: Evidence from the Family Expenditure Survey”, Applied Economics, Vol. 23(2) 343-351 Kitchen, H. & Dalton R. (1990) “Determinants of Charitable Donation by Families in Canada: A Regional Analysis”, Applied Economics, Vol. 22(3) 285-299 Kramer, R. McClintock, C.G. & Messick, D.M. (1986) “Social Values and Cooperative Response to a Simulated Resource Conservation Crisis” Journal of Personality, Vol 54(3) 576-582 Lee, YK. & Chang, CT. (2007) “Who Gives What to Charity? Characteristics Affecting Donation Behaviour” Social Behaviour and Personality, Vol. 3(1) 14-28 Lui, W. & Aaker, J. (2008) “The Happiness of Giving: The Time-Ask Effect”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol 35(10) 543-557 Margolis, H. (1982) “Selfishness, Altruism and Rationality”, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Masserman, J.H., Stanley, W. & Terris, W. (1964) “Altruistic Behavior in Rhesus Monkeys”, The American Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 121 584-585 McGinn, B. (1993) “The letter and the spirit: Spirituality as an academic discipline.” Christian Spirituality Bulletin, Vol. 17(3) 17-18 Mindak, W. & Bybee, H. (1971) “Marketing’s Application to Fund-Raising”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 35(3) 13-18 Motoki, I. Otani, T. Ohta, A. Yosiaki, S. Kuroiwa & M. Suzuki, S. (2002) “Rural-urban differences in sociodemographic, social network and lifestyle factors related to mortality of middle-aged Japanese men from the Komo-Ise cohort study” Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 12(2) 93-104 National Institute on Aging Working Group. (2003) “Multidimensional Measurement of Religiousness/Spirituality for Use in Health Research” Fetzer Institute: Kalamazoo MI 43 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Oliner, S.P. & Oliner, P.M. (1988) “The Altruistic Personality: Rescuers of the Jews in Nazi Europe”, New York: Free Press. Pandey, J. (1979) “Effects on Benefactor and Recipient Status on Helping Behaviour” Journal of Social Psychology, Vol 18(1) 171-176 Piliavin, J.A. & Hong-Wen, C. (1990) “Altruism: A Review of Recent Theory and Research”, Annual Review of Sociology, Vol. 16 p27-65 Posnett, J. (1989) “Determinants of Household Giving to Charity in the UK”, Sources of Charity Finance, Charities Aid Foundation: Tonbridge Reidenbach & Robin, D. (1990) “Towards the Development of a Multidimensional Scale for Improving Evaluations of Business Ethics “ Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 9(8) 639-653 Repoport, A. (1988) “Provision of Step-level Public Goods; Effect of Inequality in Resources”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 54 432-440 Sargeant, A. (1999) "Charity Giving: Towards A Model of Donor Behaviour", Journal of Marketing Management, Vol. 15(4) 215-238 Sargeant, A. Stephen, L. (2002) “Individual and contextual antecedents of donor trust in the voluntary sector” Journal of Marketing Management, Vol. 18(7-8) 779-802 Saxton, J. 2004. “The Achilles’ Heel of Modern Non-Profits” International Journal of Nonprofit & Voluntary Sector Marketing, Vol. 9(3) 188-190 Schlegelmilch, B., Diamantopoulos, A. & Love, A. (1992) “Determinants of Charity Giving: An Interdisciplinary Review of the Literature and Suggestions for Future Research”, AMA Winter Educators Conference Proceedings, Vol. 3 507-516 Skowronski, J. (1997) “On the Psychology of Organ Donation: Attitudinal and Situational Factors Related to the Willingness to Be an Organ Donor”, Basic & Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 19(4) 427456 Smith, A. (1759) “The Theory of Moral Sentiments”, Indianapolis: Liberty Classics Sober, E. (1988) “What is Evolutionary Altruism”, Canadian Journal on Philosophy, Vol. 14 75-99 44 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Staub, E. (1978) “Positive Social Behaviour and Morality”, New York: Academic Press Steinberg, J. (1998) “Alumni to Give Brooklyn Tech Huge Donation”, NyTimes.com, retrieved on 07/05/2009 Thaler, H.T. (1999) “Mental Accounting Matters”, Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, Iss.12(3) 183-206 Tscheulin, D.K. & Lindenmeier, J. (2005) “The Willingness to Donate Blood: an Empirical Analysis of Socio-demopgrahic and Motivation-related Determinants, Health Services Management Research, Iss. 18 165-174 U.S. Labor Department, ‘Bureau of Labor Statistics’ (2007), http://www.bls.gov/, retrieved on 07/05/2007 Vine, J. (1983) “Sociobiology and Social Psychology – Rivalry or Symbiosis?”, Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 22(1) 1-11 West, L. A. (2004) “Non-profits Face Funding Pressures,” Journal of Accountancy, Vol. 198(11) 1-2 Yavas, U., Reicken, G. & Parameswaran, R. (1980) “Using Psychographics to Profile Potential Donors”, Business Atlanta, Vol. 30 41-45 Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., Chapman, M. (1992) “Development of Concern for Others”, Developmental Psychology, Iss. 28 126-136 Zinnbauer, B. J., Pargament, K. I. & Scott, A. B. (1999) “The emerging meanings of religiousness and spirituality: Problems and prospects”, Journal of Personality, Vol. 67(6) 889-919 45 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 46 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Appendix I Factor analysis Rotated Component Matrix I Component 1 People are more inclined to help themselves than they are to help others 2 3 4 -.112 .258 .078 .286 You can’t count on strangers anymore .072 .070 .853 .056 Most people would take advantage of you instead of being honest .129 .036 .832 .160 Spirituality plays a prominent role in my life .080 .277 .745 -.110 My spiritual beliefs help me to know right from wrong .850 .018 .042 -.095 My whole approach to life is based on my spiritual beliefs .906 .046 .204 -.042 My life’s mission is shaped by a higher power .914 .050 .034 -.050 Fristly, I consider my own welfare, even if others have a hard time because of it I’m not really concerned about others if they have problems I don’t bother if the things I do disturb others .804 -.037 .008 .119 -.085 -.052 -.055 .634 .167 -.330 -.356 .532 I strive towards my desires, even if I realize that my striving causes problems for others Charities usually achieve their goals .043 -.082 .035 .823 .012 .208 .185 .639 Charities often find the best way to releive suffering .045 .780 .066 -.041 Charities understand the needs of their benificiaries -.065 .709 .365 -.119 Of charities one can expect that they observe the changes in societies needs -.012 .806 .181 .008 .156 .867 -.089 .119 You can’t count on strangers anymore Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 5 iterations. 47 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Rotated Component Matrix II; after removal of faulty question Component 1 2 3 4 Most people can’t be trusted .074 .063 .853 .035 You can’t count on strangers anymore .121 .050 .835 .170 Most people would take advantage of you instead of being honest .086 .261 .744 -.143 Spirituality plays a prominent role in my life .852 .021 .040 -.087 My spiritual beliefs help me to know right from wrong .905 .055 .204 -.029 My whole approach to life is based on my spiritual beliefs .916 .054 .033 -.042 My life’s mission is shaped by a higher power .803 -.030 Fristly, I consider my own welfare, even if others have a hard time because of it I’m not really concerned about others if they have problems I don’t bother if the things I do disturb others I strive towards my desires, even if I realize that my striving causes problems for others Charities usually achieve their goals .010 .128 -.096 -.040 -.045 .637 .162 -.320 -.350 .543 .030 -.066 .047 .829 .651 -.004 .229 .195 .036 .789 .067 -.039 Charities often find the best way to releive suffering -.074 .716 .366 -.119 Charities understand the needs of their benificiaries -.018 .805 .183 -.009 Of charities one can expect that they observe the changes in societies needs Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 5 iterations. 48 .148 .869 -.085 .105 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 II Reliability Statistics Cronbach’s Alpha; Faith in Humanity Cronbach's Alpha Based on Cronbach's Alpha Standardized Items N of Items ,662 ,648 4 Item-Total Statistics; Faith in Humanity Scale Mean Scale Corrected Item- Squared Cronbach's if Item Variance if Total Multiple Alpha if Item Deleted Item Deleted Correlation Correlation Deleted Most people can’t be trusted You can’t count on strangers anymore Most people would take advantage of you instead of being honest People are more inclined to help themselves than they are to help others 9,64 5,144 ,579 ,397 ,492 9,46 5,090 ,561 ,434 ,505 8,98 5,821 ,497 ,265 ,558 8,45 7,696 ,160 ,046 ,750 Cronbach’s Alpha; Faith in Humanity after removing faulty question Cronbach's Alpha Based on Cronbach's Alpha Standardized Items ,750 N of Items ,749 3 Item-Total Statistics; Faith in Humanity after removing faulty question Scale Corrected Squared Cronbach's Scale Mean if Variance if Item-Total Multiple Alpha if Item Item Deleted Item Deleted Correlation Correlation Deleted De meeste mensen zijn niet te vertrouwen 5,91 3,724 ,597 ,386 ,645 Je kan niet meer op vreemden rekenen 5,73 3,447 ,648 ,430 ,582 De meeste mensen zullen eerder voordeel proberen te trekken dan eerlijk zijn in een open situatie 5,26 4,371 ,497 ,252 ,754 49 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Cronbach’s Alpha; Selfishness Cronbach's Alpha Based on Standardized Cronbach's Alpha Items N of Items ,611 ,612 4 Item-Total Statistics; Selfishness Scale Mean Scale Corrected Item- Squared Cronbach's if Item Variance if Total Multiple Alpha if Item Deleted Item Deleted Correlation Correlation Deleted Fristly, I consider my own welfare, even if others have a hard time because of it. I’m not really concerned about others if they have problems I don’t bother if the things I do disturb others I strive towards my desires, even if I realize that my striving causes problems for others. 5,66 4,427 ,305 ,094 ,603 6,04 4,162 ,344 ,172 ,578 6,03 3,627 ,556 ,346 ,409 5,95 4,298 ,375 ,235 ,553 Cronbach’s Alpha; Selfishness after removing 2 faulty questions Cronbach's Alpha Based on Cronbach's Alpha Standardized Items N of Items ,643 ,643 2 Item-Total Statistics; Selfishness after removing two questions Corrected Item- Squared Cronbach's Scale Mean if Scale Variance Total Multiple Alpha if Item Item Deleted if Item Deleted Correlation Correlation Deleted I don’t bother if the things I do disturb others I strive towards my desires, even if I realize that my striving causes problems for others. 1,95 ,808 ,474 ,225 .a 1,87 ,874 ,474 ,225 .a a. The value is negative due to a negative average covariance among items. This violates reliability model assumptions. You may want to check item codings. Cronbach’s Alpha; Effectiveness Cronbach's Alpha Based on Cronbach's Alpha Standardized Items 50 N of Items Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Cronbach’s Alpha; Faith in Humanity Cronbach's Alpha Based on Cronbach's Alpha Standardized Items ,765 N of Items ,766 51 4 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Item-Total Statistics of Effectiveness Charities are a very effective form of organization Charities often find the best way to relieve suffering Charities fully understand the needs of their beneficiaries Charities can be counted on to monitor changes in the needs of their beneficiaries. Questions opted for deletion Code FaithQ1 SelfishQ1 SelfishQ2 Scale Scale Mean if Variance if Corrected Squared Cronbach's Item Item Item-Total Multiple Alpha if Item Deleted Deleted Correlation Correlation Deleted 10,04 4,181 ,534 ,334 ,728 10,01 4,591 ,552 ,358 ,716 9,57 4,211 ,595 ,434 ,692 9,63 4,362 ,581 ,407 ,700 Question People are more inclined to help themselves than they are to help others. Fristly, I consider my own welfare, even if others have a hard time because of it I’m not really concerned about others if they have problems 52 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 III Frequency tables Gender Frequency Valid Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent Female 74 58.3 58.3 58.3 Male 53 41.7 41.7 100.0 Total 127 100.0 100.0 Household income Frequency Valid Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent Up to €1000 30 23.6 23.6 23.6 €1000 to €1500 15 11.8 11.8 35.4 €1500 to €2000 19 15.0 15.0 50.4 €2000 to €3000 36 28.3 28.3 78.7 €3000 to €5000 17 13.4 13.4 92.1 €5000 to €10000 4 3.1 3.1 95.3 More than €10000 6 4.7 4.7 100.0 127 100.0 100.0 Total Village or city Frequency Valid Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent Village 37 29.1 29.1 29.1 City 90 70.9 70.9 100.0 Total 127 100.0 100.0 Level of education Frequency Valid Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent Elementary 7 5.5 5.5 5.5 High school 28 22.0 22.0 27.6 MBO 30 23.6 23.6 51.2 HBO 49 38.6 38.6 89.8 University 13 10.2 10.2 100.0 127 100.0 100.0 Total 53 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 54 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 IV Regression statistics Willingness to donate money; general without interaction Model Fitting Information; willingness to donate money Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 402.469 Final 331.648 df 70.820 Sig. 11 .000 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; willingness to donate money Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 567.758 787 1.000 Deviance 331.648 787 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; willingness to donate money Cox and Snell .460 Nagelkerke .474 McFadden .176 Link function: Logit. 55 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; willingness to donate money 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Std. Error Threshold Location Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound [Money = 1] 6.102 1.569 15.120 1 .000 3.026 9.178 [Money = 2] 7.306 1.604 20.731 1 .000 4.161 10.450 [Money = 3] 8.442 1.652 26.120 1 .000 5.205 11.680 [Money = 4] 9.588 1.709 31.465 1 .000 6.238 12.938 [Money = 5] 10.576 1.766 35.849 1 .000 7.114 14.038 [Money = 6] 11.113 1.802 38.048 1 .000 7.582 14.644 [Money = 7] 13.796 2.115 42.530 1 .000 9.650 17.942 .041 .008 24.044 1 .000 .025 .058 -.002 .009 .051 1 .822 -.020 .016 Age .081 .017 23.276 1 .000 .048 .113 Income .135 .129 1.101 1 .294 -.117 .387 Education .246 .195 1.585 1 .208 -.137 .629 FaithAvg -.216 .221 .960 1 .327 -.650 .217 SpiritAvg .095 .166 .329 1 .566 -.230 .421 SelfAvg .065 .292 .049 1 .824 -.508 .637 EffectAvg .700 .292 5.730 1 .017 .127 1.273 [Gender=1] -.227 .371 .376 1 .540 -.954 .499 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] -.198 .415 .229 1 .632 -1.011 .614 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . Efficient Administrative Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. 56 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate money; individual dispositions Model Fitting Information; individual dispositions Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 452.168 Final 410.050 df 42.117 Sig. 8 .000 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; individual dispositions Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 783.462 790 .559 Deviance 410.050 790 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; individual dispositions Cox and Snell .279 Nagelkerke .287 McFadden .093 Link function: Logit. 57 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; individual dispositions 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Threshold Location Std. Error Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound [Money = 1] 1.838 1.155 2.533 1 .112 -.426 4.102 [Money = 2] 2.782 1.170 5.655 1 .017 .489 5.076 [Money = 3] 3.797 1.189 10.197 1 .001 1.466 6.127 [Money = 4] 4.658 1.208 14.881 1 .000 2.291 7.025 [Money = 5] 5.474 1.231 19.773 1 .000 3.061 7.887 [Money = 6] 5.994 1.253 22.901 1 .000 3.539 8.449 [Money = 7] 8.301 1.599 26.963 1 .000 5.167 11.434 Age .047 .014 10.736 1 .001 .019 .075 Income .267 .119 5.056 1 .025 .034 .500 Education .290 .180 2.599 1 .107 -.063 .642 FaithAvg -.103 .200 .266 1 .606 -.496 .289 SpiritAvg .150 .153 .960 1 .327 -.150 .451 SelfAvg -.240 .276 .758 1 .384 -.781 .300 [Gender=1] -.053 .340 .025 1 .875 -.720 .613 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] -.148 .387 .146 1 .703 -.906 .611 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. 58 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate money; attitudes towards charity Model Fitting Information; attitudes towards charity Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 404.666 Final 333.874 df 70.792 Sig. 8 .000 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; attitudes towards charity Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 583.555 790 1.000 Deviance 333.874 790 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; attitudes towards charity Cox and Snell .457 Nagelkerke .471 McFadden .175 Link function: Logit. 59 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; attitudes towards charity 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Std. Error Threshold Location Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound [Money = 1] 6.437 1.326 23.556 1 .000 3.838 9.037 [Money = 2] 7.627 1.369 31.054 1 .000 4.944 10.309 [Money = 3] 8.749 1.426 37.634 1 .000 5.954 11.544 [Money = 4] 9.889 1.496 43.707 1 .000 6.957 12.820 [Money = 5] 10.866 1.563 48.303 1 .000 7.802 13.930 [Money = 6] 11.398 1.604 50.495 1 .000 8.254 14.542 [Money = 7] 14.077 1.942 52.564 1 .000 10.272 17.883 Age .087 .016 29.724 1 .000 .056 .118 Income .100 .120 .690 1 .406 -.136 .336 Education .288 .189 2.315 1 .128 -.083 .659 Efficient .042 .008 25.226 1 .000 .026 .059 -.002 .009 .038 1 .846 -.019 .016 .640 .277 5.331 1 .021 .097 1.183 [Gender=1] -.229 .364 .395 1 .530 -.943 .485 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] -.251 .403 .390 1 .533 -1.041 .538 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . Administrative EffectAvg Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. 60 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate money; general including interactions Model Fitting Information; general including interactions Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 402.469 Final 324.663 df 77.805 Sig. 12 .000 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; general including interactions Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 515.793 786 1.000 Deviance 324.663 786 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; general including interactions Cox and Snell .492 Nagelkerke .507 McFadden .193 Link function: Logit. 61 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; general including interactions 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound Threshold [Money = 1] 12.623 3.036 17.289 1 .000 6.673 18.573 [Money = 2] 13.845 3.067 20.375 1 .000 7.833 19.857 [Money = 3] 15.013 3.107 23.351 1 .000 8.924 21.103 [Money = 4] 16.225 3.161 26.346 1 .000 10.030 22.421 [Money = 5] 17.317 3.224 28.847 1 .000 10.998 23.636 [Money = 6] 17.921 3.265 30.121 1 .000 11.521 24.321 [Money = 7] 20.708 3.542 34.179 1 .000 13.766 27.651 [Gender=1] -.293 .375 .611 1 .434 -1.029 .442 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] -.432 .427 1.024 1 .311 -1.268 .404 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . Age .070 .017 16.246 1 .000 .036 .104 Income .177 .130 1.836 1 .175 -.079 .432 Education .226 .197 1.316 1 .251 -.160 .612 Efficient .042 .009 24.342 1 .000 .025 .059 Administrative -.003 .009 .116 1 .733 -.021 .015 EffectAvg 2.860 .894 10.240 1 .001 1.108 4.612 FaithAvg -.231 .224 1.063 1 .303 -.670 .208 SpiritAvg .132 .167 .618 1 .432 -.197 .460 SelfAvg 3.274 1.285 6.488 1 .011 .755 5.793 -1.016 .396 6.593 1 .010 -1.791 -.240 Location EffectAvg * SelfAvg Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. 62 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate time; general without interactions Model Fitting Information; willingness to donate time Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 287.312 Final 272.261 df 15.051 Sig. 11 .180 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; willingness to donate time Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 481.001 559 .992 Deviance 272.261 559 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; willingness to donate time Cox and Snell .123 Nagelkerke .134 McFadden .052 Link function: Logit. 63 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; willingness to donate time 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Std. Error Threshold Location Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound [Volunteer = 1] 2.933 1.613 3.307 1 .069 -.228 6.094 [Volunteer = 2] 4.238 1.639 6.683 1 .010 1.025 7.451 [Volunteer = 3] 4.894 1.656 8.740 1 .003 1.650 8.139 [Volunteer = 4] 5.305 1.668 10.115 1 .001 2.036 8.574 [Volunteer = 5] 5.566 1.677 11.009 1 .001 2.278 8.854 Age -.016 .017 .943 1 .331 -.050 .017 Income .029 .137 .045 1 .833 -.240 .298 Education .184 .208 .783 1 .376 -.223 .591 FaithAvg .043 .233 .034 1 .853 -.413 .500 SpiritAvg .387 .176 4.864 1 .027 .043 .731 SelfAvg .027 .310 .008 1 .930 -.581 .636 EffectAvg .632 .312 4.110 1 .043 .021 1.242 Efficient -.003 .008 .108 1 .742 -.018 .013 Administrative -.010 .010 1.012 1 .314 -.031 .010 [Gender=1] -.511 .393 1.690 1 .194 -1.281 .259 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] .275 .440 .391 1 .532 -.587 1.136 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. 64 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate time; individual dispositions Model Fitting Information; individual dispositions Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 319.952 Final 308.480 df 11.473 Sig. 8 .176 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; individual dispositions Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 590.084 562 .199 Deviance 308.480 562 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; individual dispositions Cox and Snell .085 Nagelkerke .093 McFadden .036 Link function: Logit. 65 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; individual dispositions 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Std. Error Threshold Location Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound [Volunteer = 1] 1.248 1.214 1.058 1 .304 -1.130 3.627 [Volunteer = 2] 2.497 1.230 4.123 1 .042 .087 4.907 [Volunteer = 3] 3.269 1.246 6.889 1 .009 .828 5.710 [Volunteer = 4] 3.657 1.258 8.455 1 .004 1.192 6.122 [Volunteer = 5] 3.906 1.268 9.495 1 .002 1.422 6.391 Age -.014 .015 .804 1 .370 -.044 .016 Income .057 .128 .199 1 .655 -.194 .308 Education .037 .188 .039 1 .843 -.331 .406 FaithAvg .070 .214 .108 1 .742 -.349 .490 SpiritAvg .458 .167 7.533 1 .006 .131 .785 SelfAvg -.060 .290 .042 1 .837 -.629 .510 [Gender=1] -.265 .361 .540 1 .462 -.973 .442 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] .503 .404 1.548 1 .213 -.290 1.296 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. 66 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate time; charity perceptions Model Fitting Information; charity perceptions Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 288.509 Final 279.201 df 9.308 Sig. 8 .317 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; charity perceptions Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 506.964 562 .953 Deviance 279.201 562 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; charity perceptions Cox and Snell .077 Nagelkerke .084 McFadden .032 Link function: Logit. 67 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; charity perceptions 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Std. Error Threshold Location Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound [Volunteer = 1] 2.490 1.333 3.488 1 .062 -.123 5.102 [Volunteer = 2] 3.746 1.360 7.592 1 .006 1.081 6.411 [Volunteer = 3] 4.382 1.378 10.116 1 .001 1.682 7.082 [Volunteer = 4] 4.782 1.392 11.800 1 .001 2.053 7.510 [Volunteer = 5] 5.036 1.403 12.883 1 .000 2.286 7.786 Age -.007 .016 .215 1 .643 -.039 .024 Income -.067 .127 .281 1 .596 -.316 .181 Education .316 .201 2.477 1 .116 -.077 .709 EffectAvg .723 .309 5.496 1 .019 .119 1.328 Efficient -.002 .008 .072 1 .789 -.018 .013 Administrative -.013 .010 1.623 1 .203 -.033 .007 [Gender=1] -.472 .381 1.537 1 .215 -1.218 .274 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] .102 .421 .058 1 .809 -.723 .926 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. 68 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Willingness to donate time; general with interaction Model Fitting Information; general with interaction Model -2 Log Likelihood Chi-Square Intercept Only 287.312 Final 271.041 df 16.272 Sig. 12 .179 Link function: Logit. Goodness-of-Fit; general with interaction Chi-Square df Sig. Pearson 487.653 558 .985 Deviance 271.041 558 1.000 Link function: Logit. Pseudo R-Square; general with interaction Cox and Snell .132 Nagelkerke .144 McFadden .057 Link function: Logit. 69 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Parameter Estimates; general with interaction 95% Confidence Interval Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound Threshold [Volunteer = 1] 6.018 3.135 3.686 1 .055 -.126 12.162 [Volunteer = 2] 7.337 3.160 5.391 1 .020 1.143 13.531 [Volunteer = 3] 8.000 3.173 6.358 1 .012 1.781 14.219 [Volunteer = 4] 8.415 3.181 6.997 1 .008 2.180 14.651 [Volunteer = 5] 8.681 3.187 7.417 1 .006 2.434 14.928 [Gender=1] -.527 .394 1.792 1 .181 -1.299 .245 [Gender=2] 0a . . 0 . . . [Village=1] .196 .446 .193 1 .661 -.679 1.070 [Village=2] 0a . . 0 . . . -.022 .018 1.528 1 .216 -.057 .013 Income .045 .139 .106 1 .745 -.227 .317 Education .184 .210 .770 1 .380 -.227 .595 EffectAvg 1.604 .918 3.051 1 .081 -.196 3.404 Efficient -.003 .008 .120 1 .729 -.019 .013 Administrative -.011 .010 1.070 1 .301 -.031 .010 FaithAvg .049 .235 .044 1 .834 -.412 .510 SpiritAvg .398 .177 5.042 1 .025 .051 .746 SelfAvg 1.556 1.351 1.326 1 .249 -1.092 4.204 EffectAvg * -.467 .405 1.328 1 .249 -1.260 .327 Location Age SelfAvg Link function: Logit. a. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant. Interactions checked for donated money (significance levels only) Faith in humanity Spirituality Selfishness Effectiveness 0,123 0,963 0,010 Efficiency 0,882 0,640 0,102 Administrative 0,646 0,488 0,148 Interactions checked for volunteered hours (significance levels only) Faith in humanity Spirituality Effectiveness 0,159 0,171 Efficiency 0,249 0,745 70 Administrative 0,633 0,893 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Selfishness 0,249 0,232 0,792 Questionnaire content The past year, I’ve transfered to a charitable organisation: Nothing € 1 to € 25 € 25 to €50 € 50 to € 100 € 100 to € 250 € 250 to € 500 € 500 to € 1000 More than € 1000 I functioned as a volunteer for a charitable organization: (on average) No time last year Sometimes 1 hour a week 5 hours a week 10 hours a week More than 10 hours p/w Questions measuring ‘faith in humanity’; Strongly disagree Strongly agree People are more inclined to help themselves than they are to help others: Most people can’t be trusted: 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 You can’t count on strangers anymore: 1 2 3 4 5 Most people would take advantage of you instead Of being honest: 1 2 3 4 5 71 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Questions measuring ‘spirituality’ Strongly disagree Spirituality plays a prominent role in my life: 1 My spiritual beliefs help me to know right from wrong: 1 2 2 Strongly agree 3 3 4 4 5 5 My whole approach to life is based on my spiritual beliefs: 1 2 3 4 5 My life’s mission is shaped by a higher power: 1 2 3 4 5 Questions measuring ‘selfishness’ Strongly disagree Fristly, I consider my own welfare, even if others have a hard time because of it: I’m not really concerned about others if they have problems: 1 1 2 2 Strongly agree 3 3 4 4 5 5 I don’t bother if the things I do disturb others: 1 2 3 4 5 I strive towards my desires, even if I realize that my striving causes problems for others: 1 2 3 4 5 Questions measuring ‘effectiveness’ Strongly disagree Strongly agree Charities usually achieve their goals: 1 2 3 4 5 Charities often find the best way to releive suffering: 1 2 3 4 5 Charities understand the needs of their benificiaries: 1 2 3 4 5 Of charities one can expect that they observe the changes in societies needs: 72 1 2 3 4 5 Charitable & Willing October 26, 2009 Questions measuring’ efficiency & administrative tolerance’; How many cents of every donated euro ends up – according to you – at the benificiary. cents How many cents of every donated euro – according to you, can the charity spend on organizational costs? cents Gender: Female Male Age (in years) __________ Years Family income (monthley): Up to 1000€ €1000 to €1500 € 1500 to €2000 € 2000 to €3000 € 3000 to €5000 € 5000 to €10.000 More than €10.000 I live in a; Village (less than 25.000 inhabitants) City (more than 25.000 inhabitants) Education:(finished, mark highest) Elementary school High school MBO HBO University 73