Guilty by Cultural Association: Cross-cultural Differences in Vicarious Responsibility People who transgress can obviously experience negative moral emotions like guilt and shame. So too, however, can people who are socially connected to a transgressor, especially when the transgressor comes from their in-group (Johns, Schmader, &Lickel, 2005; Lickel, Schmader, Curtis, Scarnier, & Ames, 2005). Thus, following a criminal act, a criminal’s friends and relatives suffer negative social outcomes, even when they had no hand in the act. How far these negative reactions extend, however, is an open question. Do a criminal’s connections suffer true emotional trauma, or can they distance and differentiate themselves from their transgressing counterpart?Perceptions and reactions to such events, in which people are psychologically connected but not personally responsible, represent important, under-studied issues. The current research investigates the influence of cultural identity on people’s responses to the failure of a cultural compatriot, i.e., a stranger with whom a focal individual shares a national culture. Specifically, we examine the influence of national culture on people’s experience of vicarious responsibility and moral emotions, following a cultural compatriot’s transgression.Drawing from models of the self and group identification, we predicted that other people’s transgressions would have a stronger impact on individuals from Eastern cultures than those from Western cultures, and that vicarious responsibility would mediate the effects of culture on intended future behaviors. We tested our hypotheses in a Western culture (the U.S.) and an East Asian culture (South Korea), and also with a group of East Asians residing in the U.S. Study 1 tested our hypotheses in the U.S. and South Korea. In Study 2, a field study, we recruited participants fromAmerican and Koreanchurches in the Chicago area, allowing us to hold constant several cultural elements that could threaten the validity of a cross-cultural comparison constant (e.g., climate, the economy, the political system). Finally, Study 3 was a controlled experiment at a single American university where wemanipulated rather than measured cultural connections, providing an opportunity to draw causal conclusions. Collectively, these studiessuggest that culture plays an important role in shaping vicarious responsibility and moral emotions after a cultural compatriot’s failure.This research also shows that feelings of vicarious responsibility influence individuals’behavioral intentions to maintain their moral self-image via charitable action, and possibly via moral cleansing. Theoretically, prior research has documented the effects that culture has on moral attributions and group-based emotions, but the current research is among the first to specifically investigate vicarious responsibility and moral emotions, and to test the behavioral implications of these feelings.Practically, our results may help to transcend or at least explain cultural misunderstandings in the wake of transgressions. From Toyota’s unexpected acceleration issues to the recent stabbing of the American Ambassador to South Korea, transgressions in Eastern nations have seemed to prompt national outpourings of emotion that sometimes strike Westerners as unusual. By the same token, the lack of national outpourings following transgressions in Western nations may strike many Easterners as unusual. By helping to explain the source of these differences, we hope that our results may help to transcend the associated cultural misunderstandings. Key words: culture; guilt, reparative behavior; shame; vicarious moral emotions; vicarious responsibility