A Sociolinguistic Analysis of the Catcall as a Speech Act Hailey Mac Arthur Florida International University ABSTRACT. In this paper, I argue that the catcall is a systematic and rule-governed speech act. The current sociolinguistic literature includes several closely related speech acts like complimenting and greeting that, while overlapping in some ways, in other ways fail to explicate the louction, illocution, and perlocution unique to catcalling. Thus, I necessarily propose six criteria for the identification of catcalls within and across speech communities. Applying these criteria to an empirical investigation of catcalls in the speech community of Miami, Florida, I identify and discuss the interaction of the sociolinguistic variables of gender, power, and social distance implicit in the speech act. An ethnography of speaking methodology is employed, including spontaneous speech samples, to unpack the lexical, phonological, and syntactic-semantic features that condition the speech act of catcalling. KEYWORDS: Catcalling, Pragmatics, Speech Act Mac Arthur | 2 Mac Arthur | 3 INTRODUCTION Despite the recent attention catcalls have received in popular media, there is to date no generalizable definition of the catcall and, therefore, no systematic way of deciding what qualifies as a catcall across and within speech communities. Analyses of the closely related speech acts of compliments and greetings have been well documented in the sociolinguistics literature. While catcalling shares qualities with these speech acts, it is distinct in several ways. This paper focuses on such issues, analyzing catcalls with regard to the features that condition the occurrence of the speech act and distinguish it from compliments and greetings. Various sources have attempted to unpack the meaning of the noun catcall. Merriam-Webster defines a catcall as “a sound or noise that someone (such as an audience member) makes toward a speaker, performer, athlete, etc., that he or she does not like.” Alternatively, Oxford English Dictionary defines a catcall as “a whistle, cry, or suggestive comment intended to express sexual attraction or admiration (but usually regarded as an annoyance), typically made by a man to a female passer-by.” Urban Dictionary captures the social texture of a catcall in its definition: “When a guy gives the wolf whirl whistle or yells at a babydoll for the purpose of getting attention and in hopes of a future hookup. This is usually done out of the window of a car. Typically a Pontiac Firebird, or Camaro.” As Bowman (1993) describes it, catcalling “includes both verbal and nonverbal behavior, such as wolf-whistles, leers, winks, and stranger remarks; the remarks are frequently sexual in nature and comment evaluatively on a woman’s physical appearance or on her presence in public.” My definition of the catcall as a speech act derives directly from ethnographic data, indicating that catcalling incorporates six systematic features that make it a conventionalized speech genre. In this paper, I will argue that the catcall is, in fact, a speech act that entails verbal formulas of a highly conventionalized and rule-governed nature. That is, the catcall is a distinct genre of speech simply because it acts as a system of expected patterns that organize a particular interaction between particular interlocutors in a particular context. Thus, the hearer readily interprets an utterance as a catcall because he recognizes a formula of meaning that is conventionalized across speech communities. Before I can make such a claim, however, I must establish a set of criteria by which to determine whether a given utterance or conversational exchange should qualify as a catcall. The context of understanding what people say during catcalls is nothing more and nothing less than the sociocultural climate that supports and is supported by the encounters in which the catcall occurs. The method by which such encounters need to be studied must then minimally include (1) ethnography and (2) at least a working definition of the phenomenon that is being investigated. Accordingly, data for this empirical case Mac Arthur | 4 study consist of sequences of spontaneous speech samples collected in public settings in Miami, Florida. All spontaneous data are in the form of field notes taken immediately following the speech act. In what follows, I first briefly review the theoretical background for speech acts with comparatively similar formulas, namely compliments and greetings. Then, I introduce six criteria for identifying catcalls across and within speech communities. Using these criteria, I go on to identify catcalls in one community where I collected spontaneous speech data. I will argue these empirical case studies blatantly violate the established expectations of compliments and greetings, and I will show that they perform a different kind of work through the interaction of gender, power, and social distance, thus functioning as a distinct speech act: the catcall. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND Before I can define the catcall as a speech act, however, I must first define speech act, itself. A speech act is an utterance that serves a performative function in language use. Speech acts are commonly taken to include such acts as promising, ordering, greeting, warning, inviting, and congratulating. The current use of the term derives from J.L. Austin’s (1975) development of performative utterances. In How To Do Things With Word’s, Austin identifies three distinct levels of action beyond the act of the utterance itself: his theory of locutionary, illocutionary, and perlouctionary acts. A speech act has three aspects: (1) locution, physical utterance by the speaker, (2) illocution, the intended meaning of the utterance by the speaker, and (3) perlocution, the action that results from the locution. In other words, Austin distinguishes the act of saying something, what one does it saying it, and what one does by saying it. Given my claim that the six criteria introduced below should allow researchers to identify catcalls across languages and communities, it is important to establish whether the same criteria can allow us to exchange words and exchanges that are not catcalls. Good candidates for such a test are compliments and greetings. Analyses of these closely related speech acts have appeared in the sociolinguistics literature and are well documented in their analyses. Catcalls shares qualities with each of these speech acts; however, I will show that the catcall is distinct in several ways. According to Searle (1976), compliments are classified as expressive speech acts. In other words, their propositional content specifies a reaction of the speaker to a situation in which the hearer takes an active or passive role. The majority of authors who have dealt with compliments have underlined that their function is to establish, increase, or consolidate solidarity bonds between interlocutors (Chen 1993; Herbert 1989, 1991; Jaworski 1995; Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk 1989; Manes and Wolfson 1981; Wolfson and Manes 1980; Wolfson 1983). Thus, Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk states, “They are typically Mac Arthur | 5 performed to make the addressee feel good by saying something nice to him/her, in this way possibly satisfying the addressee’s expectations rather than expressing a position judgement for a referential or informative reason” (1989, p. 75). Moreover, the topics of compliments tend to be general and relate to things such as appearance, abilities, skills, possessions, personality or friendship (Herbert 1991; Holmes and Brown 1987). Yet, there is cross-cultural variation as far as topic is concerned and topics-selection depends on the different underlying set of values that comprise the idea of politeness in a particular sociocultural group (Herbert 1991; Manes 1983). Greetings are another significant aspect of the politeness phenomenon that exists universally. As Brown and Levinson (1978) claim, greetings occur in all languages. Like compliments, greetings may serve as the means for opening conversations appropriately and for establishing and maintaining social relationships (Goffman 1971). For example, such greetings as “Hello,” “How are you?,” and “Pleased to meet you” often occur in English. Analogous to regular greetings, Goffman (1971, p. 75) considers passing greetings to be a type of supportive ritual. He explains passing greetings as the following: When two acquaintances pass close by each other on their separate daily rounds in consequence of what is seen as the routine intersecting of their activities, they are likely to exchange ‘passing greetings’, often without otherwise pausing. Often these displays will be relatively muted and fully exhaust the encounter to which they give rise; nonetheless, the spontaneous impulse to perform them is very strong. As before, Searle (1969) and Searle and Vanderveken (1985) proposed to analyze greetings as an example of expressive speech acts aimed at the “courteous indication of recognition of the other party” (Searle and Vanderveken 1985, p. 216). Bach and Harnish (1979) classified greetings as “acknowledgments,” interpreting the act of greeting as an expression of “pleasure at seeing (or meeting)” someone. SIX CRITERIA FOR IDENTIFYING CATCALLS Building upon the examples in the literature mentioned above (and a few more not), it is possible to extract a set of six recurring features to be used as criteria for the identification of catcalls within and across speech communities. The six criteria I will argue for are as follows: 1) shared public speech situation; 2) unacquainted interlocutors; 3) topic of sexual objectification; Mac Arthur | 6 4) emphatic intonation contour; 5) possible predictable semantic-syntactic structure; and 6) asymmetrical turn-taking structure. As will become apparent in the following discussion, some of these features could be conflated into larger categories. For example, features 1 and 2 define the social factors and features 1, 5, and 6 define the spatial and temporal factors of the exchange. Features 2 and 3 (and, in some ways, 6) focus on the inextricably intertwined sociolinguistic variables of gender, social distance, and power. In addition, features 3, 4, and 5 cover the interdependence between the lexicon, phonology, and syntax of English, respectively. Although future research may prove the need to regroup or even to eliminate some of the criteria I am proposing, for the purpose of this paper, I have chosen to retain the six criteria as is to ensure a broader spectrum of potentially relevant cases to examine. Furthermore, there are a slew of linguistic and paralinguistic cues that are integral for the interpretation of intended meaning in any conversational exchange. I should mention that although both verbal and nonverbal aspects of catcalling were taken into consideration in the choice of defining features, I will favor verbal over nonverbal aspects of catcalls. This is simply due to my efforts in this case to draw attention to the importance of specific verbal patterns used in catcalling and is not meant to undermine the importance of gestures and motion in the analysis of communicative interaction. 1. SHARED PUBLIC SPEECH SITUATION Speech takes places in speech situations, which are socially definable. We can specify the spatial and temporal factors that define the context in which the speech act occurs. Moreover, we may group these social factors under three headings, covering (1) public space, (2) shared perceptual and/or auditory field, and (3) beginning-of-discourse occurrence. 1. PUBLIC SPACE Catcalling is also called street harassment or street remarks. Appropriate to its name, street harassment can take place on the streets, in stores, on public transportation, in parks, and at beaches. According to the findings of a 2,000-person, nationally represented survey reported by Stop Street Harassment (2014), the majority of street harassment (67 percent) occurred on the street or sidewalk, either of foot or on a bicycle or skateboard. The next most-common place for street harassment was in a store, restaurant, movie theater, or mall (28 percent). In addition, evidence from Gardner (1995) suggests that women in metropolitan areas are more susceptible to harassment from stranger than women in suburban and rural areas. Mac Arthur | 7 2. SHARED PERCEPTUAL AND/OR AUDITORY FIELD Catcalls involve a face-to-face interaction. They either immediately follow or are constitutive of the speaker’s public acknowledgement of the hearer’s presence in a shared perceptual and/or auditory field. Unlike greetings, it is not necessary that the catcall is initiated after the interlocutors have sighted each other (Duranti 1992; Kendon and Ferber 1973). In fact, there may or may not be any reciprocal acknowledgement at all during a catcall. In some cases, making acknowledgment visually available to the other interlocutor—through the act of leering, for example—may achieve a similar effect to catcalling. In other cases, auditory acknowledgement such as a wolf whistle performs a comparable role. Catcalls as a speech act, however, require a visual acknowledgement followed by a verbal acknowledgement. Thus, catcalling would be the speaker’s response to viewing himself within the hearer’s visual and/or auditory range. Finally, it is important to note that this shared perceptual and/or auditory field need only be shared temporarily. 3. BEGINNING-OF-DISCOURSE OCCURRENCE We can routinely expect catcalls to occur at the beginning of a social encounter, although they may not always be the very first words that are exchanged between interlocutors. This first feature of catcalls is related to their potential function as attention-getting devices and their ability to establish a shared field of communicative interaction, or, at least, the access to it. Catcalls are like greetings in that they have to do with marking a change of access in social relationships. For Goffman, they are a type of interpersonal ritual, or, more specifically, “supportive rituals or tires that serve to support social relationships” (1971, p. 67). Goffman further classifies these acts as access rituals. While farewells mark a transition from increased to decreased access, greetings and catcalls signal the opposite. 2. UNACQUAINTED INTERLOCUTORS We can also define the participants in the speech act and the relations among them. 1. GENDER AND RACE Even though definitions of catcalling are predominately gender-neutral (save for Urban Dictionary), preliminary research on this topic typically focuses on instances in which men initiate the speech act with women. Indeed, sixty-five percent of women (two out of three women in the U.S.) have reported having experienced some form of catcalling. Of these, Hispanic and Black women were more likely than White women to report experiencing catcalls, representing 48 percent, 45 percent, and 36 percent, respectively (Stop Street Harassment 2014). Mac Arthur | 8 2. RELATIONAL DISTANCE There is considerable variation in the type of language used according to who is using it and in exchange with whom; according to whether those speaking are related to each other in some socially institutionalized way—some kind of permanent relationship, say a familial relationship—or, whether they meet each other under some casual and temporary conditions, such as we see in the case of catcalls. Relational distance, then, determines multiple features of the modes of address in language use. As with much speech behavior, the social distance variable is a strong feature of catcalling. Catcalls are scarce with interlocutors who are familiar. That is, catcalling is a speech act that is not typically encountered at the extreme end of the social distance continuum in which interlocutors are intimate or even familiar. In fact, Fairchild and Rudman (2008) refer to catcalls as stranger harassment. 3. TOPIC OF SEXUAL OBJECTIFICATION Sexual objectification is a clear component of catcalls. The common thread running through all catcalls is “the experience of [the intended hearer] being treated as a body (or collection of body parts) valued predominately for its use (or consumption by) others” (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997, p. 174). Such an experience may be accomplished through lexical choice. For example, in (1) and (2) below, the pronouns mami and baby do not refer to the speaker’s actual mother or an actual infant. Instead, these refer to the hypersexualized and eroticized extension of the words. Other personalized pronouns are shown in (3), (5), and (7): chica linda [cute girl], un bombón [sweet thing], and muñeca [doll face], respectively. The use of intensifiers and adjectives bolsters the theme of sexual objectification, as evidenced with good, damn, chica [cute], really, chuparte los dedos [finger-licking], juguete [toy]. Unsurprisingly, the stereotypical male gaze of (8) has expanded to the related sensual imagery of the male appetite in examples (5) and (6), in which the intended hearer of the utterances is reduced to, quite literally, a piece of meat in the latter. Overall, examples (1) through (7) all share in common an underlying evaluative comment on a woman’s physical appearance, accomplished through the use of intensifiers, adjectives, sensual imagery, and personalized pronouns. (1) Hey, mamicta [little mommy]. You looking good. Can I get your number? (2) Damn, baby. How are you doing? (3) ¡Chica linda! [Cute girl!] Those jeans are really working for me. (4) ¡Que curvas! [What curves!] Mac Arthur | 9 (5) Estas para chuparte los dedos, sos un bombón, [You are finger-licking good, you sweet thing.] (6) Tanta carne y yo sin dientes. [All that meat and me with no teeth.] (7) ¿De qué juguetería te sacaron, muñeca? [What toy store did they get you from, doll face?] (8) ¡Quién fuera bizco, para verte dos veces! [I wish I was cross-eyed, so I could see you twice!] 4. EMPHATIC INTONATION CONTOUR AND DURATION Both pitch variations across an utterance (intonation) and length of the utterance (duration) are definable features of catcalling. Specifically, short catcalls like example (4) above will demonstrate an intentional rising intonation contour in each word and a deliberate lengthening of each word. Figure 1 below shows the intonation contour of the utterances “Hey, baby?” followed by “Hey, baby!” in Pratt, in which the former utterance is classified as a question and the latter, a catcall. Both speech samples show rising intonation. This pattern of pitch is hearer-oriented, and the degree of rise is matched to the degree of uncertainty. We can describe this pattern as either (1) high rise or (2) low rise and, accordingly, assign meaning to each. The high rise found in the former utterance is a more marked pattern, which is typical of yes-no questions. The low rise found in the latter utterance is more common and is used in a variety of speech situations, including catcalls. (Yavas 2011). Figure 1: Intonation Contour of “Hey, Baby?” and “Hey, Baby!” Mac Arthur | 10 In the utterance “Hey, Baby?” we observe phrase-final high rising intonation. By contrast, in the utterance “Hey, baby! ” we observe an intonation contour in each word. The way a speaker shapes his utterances depends on his intended meaning. For example, the syllable with the major pitch change is typically the result of emphasis. The speaker is creating distinct meanings in the enunciation of the same words in the same order through distinct emphasis. Figure 2: Waveform and Spectrogram of “Hey, Baby?” and “Hey, Baby!” Figure 2 above shows the waveform and spectrogram of the same speech samples in Pratt. The blue lines represent the intonation contour we observed in Figure 1. In Figure 2, the spectrogram is a spectro-temporal representation of the utterances, where the horizontal axis represents time expressed in seconds. The total duration of the utterance “Hey, baby?” is .56 seconds, while the duration of merely the first syllable in the utterance “Hey, baby!” is .66 seconds, alone nearly 18 percent longer than the entirety of the former utterance. The total duration of the latter utterance is 1.59 seconds. As such, the total duration of the catcall is more than 180 percent longer than the duration of the same phrase in question form. 5. POSSIBLE PREDICTABLE SEMANTIC-SYNTACTIC STRUCTURE Most catcalls appear to be identical in their semantic-syntactic structure to another type of speech act: compliments. Manes and Wolfson have shown in several works (Manes and Wolfson 1981; Wolfson Mac Arthur | 11 1981, 1983; Wolfson and Manes 1980) that most compliments are realized by means of a limited set of highly predictable semantic-syntactic structures. This led the authors to conclude that compliments can be considered formulae whose interpretation would be relatively straightforward and would not involve much difficulty or effort for hearers. As Herbert puts it, “The noncreativity of the compliment act is a striking fact: these are speech formulae. It is tempting to speculate that such noncreativity is directly tied to a need for easily recognizable formulae in status- and solidarity-negotiating gambits in speech. That is, in making a social move of this kind, the use of a formula decreases the likelihood that the move might be misinterpreted or unnoticed by an addressee” (1991). According to Manes and Wolfson, there are three very commonly used semantic-syntactic structures for the realization of compliments (1 through 3), although other six minor patterns can also be appreciated (4 though 9): 1) NP [is/looks] (really) ADJ; 2) I (really) [like/love] NP; 3) PRO is (really) (a) ADJ NP; 4) You V (a) ADJ NP; 5) You V NP (really) ADV; 6) You have (a) (really) ADJ NP; 7) What (a) ADJ NP; 8) ADJ NP; and 9) Isn’t NP ADJ! If we turn back to the examples under criterion 3, we see that: example (1) follows pattern 1; (2), (3) and, to an extent, (6) follow pattern 8; (4) and (7) follow pattern 7; and (5) follows pattern 4. Although the final example, example (8), begins with the structure of pattern 2, it doesn’t follow through. Therefore, just as most compliments fit one of the nine predictable structures elucidated above, so do most—but not all—catcalls. 6. ASSYMETRICAL TURN-TAKING STRUCTURE According to Stop Street Harassment (2014), only about half (53 percent) of all survey respondents reported that at least once they had done something proactive about the catcalling they have experienced or witness. The other half (47 percent) did not. The final criterion of a catcall is the turn-taking structure that does not rely on interlocutor response. That is, the hearer may take an active or may take a passive part in the communicative Mac Arthur | 12 interaction without affecting the type of speech act. Hence, it is the asymmetrical structure that is an integrated feature of the catcall. Indeed, catcalls separate themselves from compliments and greetings by this very asymmetrical structure. The sequential format of the typical turn-taking communicative interaction allows interlocutors to engage in an encounter that exhibits some evidence of mutual acknowledgement and mutual understanding. For example, Herbert (1991) found that compliments from males are generally acknowledged and accepted, especially by female hearers. Yet, the catcall opening turn is often met with a violation of conversational cooperation: silence. Silence, in effect, is a rejection of a turn. Conversations are not “a succession of disconnected remarks,” but cooperative efforts where participants recognize a common purpose, “or at least a mutually accepted direction” (Grice 1975, p. 45). The Grecian approach to cooperation argues that it is a universal rule of all human interaction: “Make your conversational contribution such as is required, at the stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange in which you are engaged” (Grice 1975). Silence or rejection of the two-part turn-taking structure is an example of a violation of Grice’s cooperation principle. Yet, what may appear to be an example of non-cooperation may be, in fact, an intentional flouting in order to generate an implicature. That is, silence on the turn of the hearer will be interpreted as an expression of rejection of the preceding utterance of the speaker. AN EMPIRICAL CASE STUDY Any proposal for criteria needs empirical investigations to support it. In the rest of this paper, I will offer a brief discussion of catcalls collected in Miami, Florida as a way of assessing and refining some of the claims made so far. In addition, I will use this space to discuss the illocution of the speech act and how it is conditioned by the interactions of the sociolinguistic variables of gender, social distance, and power. Data for the study consist of sequences of spontaneous speech collected in public settings. All spontaneous data are in the form of field notes taken immediately following the speech act. It should be understood that what follows is not an exhaustive study of Miami catcalls. Such a study would require a project expressively designed with the goal of collecting all types of catcalls used in Miami speech communities. Although the data discussed here are drawn from a range of interactions, they do contain a considerable number of exchanges that qualify as catcalls according to the above-mentioned criteria. The following exchange occurred between an unacquainted male (A) and female (B) in the shared public space of a Cuban café in Miami Beach, Florida: Mac Arthur | 13 (A): Great legs! Are you an athlete, or is that genetic? (B): I’m not an athlete, so it must be. (A): Those are the best kind. (B): nods (A): Those calves are phenomenal! (B): silence (A): Don’t be so serious. You only live once. This exchange qualifies easily as a catcall, according to the six criteria established above. The exchange occurs immediately following the arrival of (A) to the public location in which (B) was already present (criterion 1). It also exhibits a shared perceptual and auditory field, as defined by the response of (B) to the initial turn of (A). The interlocutors were strangers of separate genders (criterion 2). The opening turn is an evaluation of the physical appearance (criterion 3) in which each word in the two-word utterance displayed an audible intonation contour (criterion 4). The catcall is predictable in its semantic-syntactic structure, subscribing to an established ADJ NP pattern (criterion 5). The exchange is sequentially organized into a simple two-pair structure that does not necessarily require hearer response (criterion 6). When two interlocutors, at least one of which is ostensibly going somewhere, cross into each other’s visual field of perception and are close enough for their voices to be heard (the volume of the speaker’s voice being adjusted proportionally to the physical distance between them), they have met the first criteria for a shared public speech situation: (A): Hey, sexy. Where you going? (B): Fuck off. (A): Hey! (B) silence (A) Baby, come back here! I like ‘em feisty! (B): silence First, we must contextualize this exchange. Speaker (A) is a stationary male propped in the doorway of a convenience shop in Miami Beach, Florida, while speaker (B) is a female moving along the sidewalk in front of the shop (criterion 2). Under this shared public speech situation, the initiator of the exchange addresses the other, who may or may not respond, or respond in kind (criterion 1). Second, we must recognize that the first two turns conform to the turn-taking scheme expected in a question-and-answer interaction with an added caveat: the non-cooperative retort of (B). The first turn by (B) implicates an Mac Arthur | 14 acknowledgment and consequential rejection of the catcall (criterion 6). Although the semantic-syntactic structure does not subscribe to one of the nine most common structural realizations of a catcall (criterion 5)—acting on the semantic-syntactic surface level as more of a greeting than a compliment—we can, nonetheless, qualify the initial utterance as a catcall because of the evaluative lexical choice of the intended hearer’s physical appearance in the term sexy (criterion 3). However, it is the wh-question structure of the catcall itself that points to the underlying sociolinguistic variables of gender and power. As may be gathered by an examination of its social context, the “where are you going?” catcall is more than an expression of greeting. Rather, it is an attempt to sanction the reciprocal recognition of each other’s presence with some specific request of information that may or may not receive a satisfactory response. Although highly predictable and conventional, the “where are you going?” catcall forces the intended hearer to deal with a wide range of issues including the speaker’s rights to have access to information about the hearer’s whereabouts, culture-specific expectations about the ethics of venturing into public space, the force of questioning as a form of social control, and, hence, the possibility of withholding information as a form of resistance to public scrutiny (Keenan 1976). The speech act analysis proposed for greetings, then, cannot be easily extended to catcalls, given that to initiate a “where are you going?” catcall is definitely more than (and most definitely different from) a “courteous indication of recognition” (Searle and Vanderveken 1985, p. 216) or a conventional expression of pleasure at the sight of someone (Bach and Harnish 1979, p. 51-52). Moreover, to ask “where are you going?” is not just a simple request for an account of information. The way in which this particular exchange is played out illustrates a number of important points about the social organization presupposed by the encounter, as well as the social organization achieved by it. First, the situation displays a noticeable gender gap between the male (A) and the female (B). As such, (A) expresses his ability to judge and comment upon the physical appearance of (B), a female with whom he is unacquainted, but with whom he temporarily shares the same public space. Second, the propositional content of the catcall indexes a power disparity between (A) and (B). Despite the conventionality of the exchange between two interlocutors whom are unfamiliar, what is said and how it is said is extremely important. The illocutionary point or goal of the catcall is not just “a courteous indication of recognition, with the presupposition that the speaker has just encountered the hearer,” as demonstrated with greetings (Searle and Vanderveken 1985, p. 215). Although acknowledgement (and approval) is certainly involved, the exchange plays out a set of social relations and cultural expectations: It is only males who get to occupy that verbal space. Only males are permitted to comment upon a woman’s physical appearance simply because she shares the same public space as he. Thus, the catcall is neither a friendly greeting nor a generous compliment between acquaintances if it reinforces heterosexual male power to claim public space. Mac Arthur | 15 In this way, catcalling further works as a way to restrict and control the spaces to which women have access. Catcalls, as expressions of power, are part of a larger problem of gender inequality wherein men sexually objectify women to exert their control over them, and this power play in a speech community plays out in the speech acts of that community. CONCLUSION The preceding sociolinguistic analysis of the speech act we commonly term catcalling indicates that the catcall is systematic and rule-governed in the speech community of Miami, Florida. The analysis of catcalls presented here shows that discourse analysis must be integrated with ethnographic information if we want to provide an adequate pragmatic analysis of speech behaviors within and across speech communities. Whatever catcalls accomplish in their illocution, they do so by virtue of the interlocutors’ ability to interpret a recognizable formula within a particular social context. After proposing such a definition of the catcall consisting of six criteria, I have shown that this speech act is distinct from other expressive speech acts of complimenting and greeting. While catcalls share a semantic-syntactic structure with compliments and overlap in the social variables of greetings, they violate many norms of complimenting and greeting speech behavior. My analysis of two examples of catcalls in Miami, Florida offers an empirical corroboration of the six criteria and proposes some new hypotheses about the work that is done during catcalls that will be of further research. In particular, I have shown that (1) catcalling needs to be explicated as its own speech act and (2) the sociolinguistic variables of gender and power underscore the speech act of catcalls. Overall, catcalling appears to be a frequent experience for women and, therefore, is deserving of future research designed to more fully elaborate the experience of street harassment. Mac Arthur | 16 WORKS REFERENCED Austin, J.L. (1975). How To Do Things With Words (Vol. 1955). Oxford University Press. Bach, K., & Harnish, R. (1979) Linguistic Communication and Speech Acts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Blair, F. (1983). Greetings and Their Function. Pilot Studies in Sociolinguistics: Variation, Use and Attitudes. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics. Bowman, C.G. (1993). Street Harassment and the Informal Ghettoization of Women. Harvard Law Review, 517-580. Brown, P., & Levinson, S. (1987). Politeness: Some Universals in Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University. Catcall [Def. 1]. In Merriam Webster Online, Retrieved November, 30 2014, from http://www.merriamwebster.com/dictionary/catcall. Catcall [Def. 1]. In OED Online, Retrieved November, 30 2014, from http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/28811?rskey=3ASLNI&result=1&isAdvanced=false. Catcall [Def. 1]. In Urban Dictionary, Retrieved November 30, 2014, from http://www.urbandictionary.com/define.php?term=cat%20call. Chen, R. (1993). Responding to Compliments A Contrastive Study of Politeness Strategies Between American English and Chinese Speakers. Journal of Pragmatics, 20(1), 49-75. Cooke, S. (1999). Street Remarks: A Cross Cultural Study. Language Matters: Studies in the Languages of Africa, 30(1), 171-196. Duranti, A. (1992). Language and Bodies in Social Space: Samoan Ceremonial Greetings. American Anthropologist. 94: 657-691. Duranti, A. (1997). Universal And Culture-Specific Properties Of Greetings.Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 7(1), 63-97. Fairchild, K., & Rudman, L. (2008). Everyday Stranger Harassment And Women’s Objectification. Social Justice Research, 21, 338-357. Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectification Theory: Toward Understanding Women’s Lived Experiences and Mental Health Risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173-206. Gardner, C.B. (1984). Passing By: Street Remarks, Address Rights, and the Urban Female. In Baugh, J. & Sherzer, J. (eds) Language In Use: Readings In Sociolinguistics. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of Talk. University of Pennsylvania Press. Goffman, E. (1971). Relations in Public. New York: Harper. Mac Arthur | 17 Herbert, R.K. (1989). The Ethnography of English Compliments and Compliment Responses: A Contrastive Sketch. Contrastive Pragmatics, 3-35. Herbert, R.K. (1991). Sex-Based Differences in Compliment Behaviour. Language in Society 19: 201224. Holmes, J., & Brown, D.F. (1987). Teachers and Students Learning About Compliments. TESOL Quarterly, 21(3), 523-546. Jaworski, A. (1995). “This is Not an Empty Compliment!” Polish Compliments and the Expression of Solidarity. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5(1), 63-94. Keenan, E.O. (1976). The Universality of Conversational Postulates. Language in Society. 5:67-80. Kendon, A. & Ferber, A. (1973). A Description of Some Human Greetings. In R.P. Michael and J.H. Crook (eds.) Comparative Ecology and Behavior of Primates. London: Academic Press. Kissling, E.A. (1991). Street Harassment: The Language of Sexual Terrorism. Discourse and Society, 2, 451-460. Ladegaard, H. (2009). Pragmatic Cooperation revisited: Resistance and Non-Cooperation as a Discursive Strategy in Asymmetrical Discourses. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 649-666. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (1989). Praising and Complimenting. Contrastive Pragmatics, 73-100. Li, W. (2009). Different communication rules between the english and chinese greetings. Asian Culture and History, 1(2), 72-74. Pretorius, E.J. (1990) The Discourse Dynamics of Male-To-Female Street Remarks: Insulting Compliments or Complimentary Insults? English Usage in Southern Africa, 21, 1-17. Searle, J. (1976). A Classification of Illocutionary Acts. Language in Society, 5(01), 1-23. Searle, J., & Vanderveken, D. (1985). Foundations of Illocutionary Logic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Stop Street Harassment. (2014). Unsafe and Harassed in Public Spaces: A National Street Harassment Report. Retrived from http://www.stopstreetharassment.org/our-work/nationalstudy/. Wesselmann, E., & Kelly, J. (2010). Cat-Calls and Culpability: Investigating the Frequency and Functions of Stranger Harassment. Sex Roles, 63, 451-462. Wolfson, N. (1981). Compliments in Cross-Cultural Perspective. TESOL Quarterly 15(2): 117-124. Wolfson, N. (1983). Rules of Speaking in J. Richards and R. Schmidt (eds.) Language and Communication. London: Longman. Mac Arthur | 18 Wolfson, N. & Manes, J. (1980). The Compliment as a Social Strategy. Papers in Linguistics 13: 391410. Yavas, M. (2011). Applied English Phonology. John Wiley & Sons.