Psychopharmacology Electronic Supplementary Material 1 The effects of nicotine dependence and acute abstinence on the processing of drug and non-drug rewards Lawn, W.1, Freeman, T.P1, Hindocha, C.1, Mokrysz, C.1, Das, R.K.1, Morgan, C.J.A. 1,2, & Curran, H.V.1 1 Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. 2 School of Psychology, University of Exeter, Washington Singer Building, Perry Road, Exeter, EX4 4QG, United Kingdom. Statistical Analyses CO data were investigated via a mixed ANOVA with a between-subjects factor of group (dependent and occasional) and a within-subjects factor of smoking-condition (ad libitum and abstinent). The time since last smoked was analysed using non-parametric comparisons as the data were nonnormally distributed. MNWS, craving (from MNWS) and SHAPS scores were investigated via mixed analyses of variance (ANOVA) with a between-subjects factor of group (dependent and occasional) and within-subjects factors of smoking-condition (ad libitum and abstinence) and time (pre-task and post-consumption). We also conducted an exploratory analysis into the time taken to choose each reward-type1. This analysis was conducted in order to determine if certain rewards were chosen more quickly than others and therefore perhaps elicited a response with greater motivation. The time taken to choose each reward was averaged across all trials and a mixed ANOVA with a between-subjects factor of group and within-subjects factors of reward-type (cigarette, music and chocolate), reward-magnitude (large and small) and smoking-condition was carried out. Paper was not included in this analysis as 19 participants never chose paper. Furthermore, we also conducted an exploratory analysis into whether the average number of buttonpresses (BP) for the chosen reward was dependent upon the alternative reward that was foregone. This analysis was conducted in order to determine whether motivation for each reward (as measured by BP) was affected by the reward option that was rejected. It therefore provided information on the relationship between choice and BP data. Separate ANOVAs for cigarette, music and chocolate were carried out with a between-subjects factor of group and a within-subjects factor of alternative-reward (cigarette, chocolate, music and paper). We collapsed across reward-magnitude and smokingcondition for these three ANOVAs. Missing data for both of these exploratory analyses were imputed by separately calculating the mean for the relevant cell in each group. Results State measures Carbon monoxide (CO) and hours since last smoked (Table 1) The dependent group’s CO level was greater in the ad libitum condition compared with the abstinent condition (t18=7.915, p<0.001). The occasional group’s CO level did not differ between conditions. 1 We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for suggesting the two exploratory analyses described here. Dependent smokers had smoked more recently before the experiment during the ad libitum condition compared to during abstinence (W19=3.827, p<0.001) but, as expected, there was no difference for occasional smokers. Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale (MNWS), Craving (from MNWS) and SHAPS (Table 2) Dependent smokers had smoked more recently before the experiment during the ad libitum condition compared to during abstinence (W19=3.827, p<0.001) but, as expected, there was no difference for occasional smokers. Group, smoking-condition and time all significantly affected overall MNWS and the craving item. The dependent group’s MNWS score was greater than the occasional group’s score only during abstinence and at pre-task (t38=3.905, p<0.001). The dependent group’s craving decreased between pre-task and post-consumption during both abstinence (t19=7.192, p<0.001) and, to a lesser extent, during ad libitum smoking (t19=2.667, p=0.011), whereas there were no changes between pre-task and post-consumption for the occasional smokers. SHAPS data were analysed using the original scoring system (Snaith-Hamilton et al., 1995). There were significant interactions between smoking-condition, time and group (F1,38=10.195, p=0.003, ηp2=0.212), smoking-condition and time (F1,38=8.162, p=0.007, ηp2=0.177), time and group (F1,38=6.306, p=0.016, ηp2=0.152) and smoking-condition and time (F1,38=4.347, p=0.044, ηp2=0.1033). Furthermore, there were main effects of smoking-condition (F1,38=7.823, p=0.008, ηp2=0.171) and group (F1,38=5.698, p=0.022, ηp2=0.130). The groups only differed during abstinence at pre-task (t38=3.178, p=0.009) and the dependent group’s score only differed between pre-task and post-consumption during abstinence (t38=4.731, p<0.001). Time to choose each reward (Figure. 1) There was an interaction between group, reward-type and reward-magnitude (F1.664, 63.226, p=0.016, ηp2=0.110) and a main effect of reward-magnitude, with larger rewards chosen faster than smaller rewards (F1, 38=12.317, p=0.001, ηp2=0.245). Exploration of the group X reward-type X rewardmagnitude interaction showed that the dependent group were faster to choose large compared to small rewards for music (t19=2.940, p=0.006) and chocolate (t19=3.265, p=0.002), but not for cigarettes. On the other hand, the occasional group were faster to choose large compared to small rewards for cigarettes (t19=2.591, p=0.014) and music (t19=2.036, p=0.049), but not for chocolate. Average button-presses (BP) for the chosen reward affected by the alternative reward (Figure. 2) For music BP, there was a main effect of alternative-reward (F1, 38=3.687, p=0.030, ηp2=0.088). BP was greater when music was chosen over cigarettes than when music was chosen over paper (t39=2.530, p=0.047). For chocolate BP, there was a main effect of alternative-reward (F1, 38=3.873, p=0.025, ηp2=0.092). BP was greater when chocolate was chosen over cigarettes than when chocolate was chosen over paper (t39=2.867, p=0.020). For cigarette BP, there was an interaction between alternative-reward and group (F1.665, 63.288=5.889, p=0.007, ηp2=0.134) and a main effect of alternative-reward (F1.665, 63.288=5.917, p=0.007, ηp2=0.135). Exploration of the interaction showed that, within the occasional group, BP was greater when cigarettes were chosen over chocolate than when cigarettes were chosen over paper (t19=3.251, p=0.007) and BP was greater when cigarettes were chosen over chocolate than when cigarettes were chosen over music (t19=4.297, p<0.001). Contrastingly, there were no significant differences within the dependent group for cigarette BP as a function of foregone reward. The main effect was due to BP being greater when cigarettes were chosen over chocolate than when cigarettes were chosen over music (t39=4.409, p<0.001), when collapsing across groups. Table 1 Group means (SDs) of CO and time since last smoked. CO = carbon monoxide.*p<0.05; ***p<0.001 within-subjects significance; o p<0.05, ooo p<0.001 overall between-subjects significance. Bold numbers identify which within-subject difference the significance refers to. Between-subject differences are collapsed across smoking-condition and time and so represent overall differences Dependent Occasional Abstinent Ad Libitum Abstinent Ad Libitum CO***, ooo 5.15 (2.13) 14.95 (6.75) 2.80 (1.06) 2.95 (1.50) Last Smoked 14.52 (3.38) 0.50 (0.54) 89.73 (77.20) 83.28 (102.65) (hours)***,ooo Table 2 Group means (SDs) of state measures at times pre-task and post-consumption. MNWS = Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale; SHAPS = SnaithHamilton Pleasure Scale. *p<0.05; ***p<0.001 within-subjects significance; o p<0.05, ooo p<0.001 overall between-subjects significance. Bold numbers identify which within-subject difference the significance refers to. Between-subject differences are collapsed across smoking-condition and time and so represent overall differences Dependent Occasional Abstinent Pre-task Ad Libitum Post- Pre-task consumption Abstinent Post- Pre-task consumption Ad Libitum Post- Pre-task consumption Postconsumption MNWS*** 13.55 (7.75) 3.85 (3.13) 4.55 (4.38) 2.55 (3.0) 5.65 (4.67) 4.55 (3.47) 4.65 (3.28) 3.55 (3.55) Craving (from 2.80 (1.40) 0.70 (0.80) 1.10 (1.165) 0.50 (0.61) 0.75 (0.91) 0.30 (0.47) 0.50 (0.61) 0.40 (0.75) 4.15 (4.08) 1.45 (2.19) 1.60 (2.23) 1.60 (2.14) 0.80 (2.35) 0.85 (1.95) 0.70 (1.59) 0.60 (1.23) MNWS)***, ooo SHAPS***, o Fig. 1 Average time taken (in seconds) to choose each reward type (cigarette small, cigarette large, music small, music large, chocolate small, chocolate large) in the DReaM-Choice task, collapsed across smoking-condition. Error bars represent ± standard error. Time to choose each reward 2 1.8 1.6 Time (s) 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 Small Large Small Dependent Large Occasional Cigarette Music Chocolate Fig. 2 Average number of button-presses (BP) in 7 seconds for each reward: a) music b) chocolate c) cigarettes as a function of the foregone reward (cigarette, music, chocolate, paper). Error bars represent ± standard error. Average number of button-presses a) BP for music 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 Dependent Over Cigarette Occasional Over Chocolate Over Paper Average number of button-presses b) BP for chocolate 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 Dependent Over Cigarette Occasional Over Music Over Paper Average number of button-presses c) BP for cigarettes 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 Dependent Over Music Occasional Over Chocolate Over Paper