Becoming a reader in a digital age; boys’ and girls’ self- perceptions as they start school Paper presented at the British Educational Research Association New Researchers/Student Conference, Wednesday 14th September 2005 (Draft not for circulation) Rachael Levy University of Cambridge Contact rachael.levy@ntlworld.com This research is funded by a studentship from the ESRC 2 Abstract SIG: Early Childhood Education Title: Becoming a reader; girls’ and boys’ self-perceptions as they start school This work builds on an earlier study where six nursery-aged children were questioned about their thoughts on learning to read. These children appeared to believe that print is of substantial importance in text and regard an ability to decode print to be of greater value than other meaning-making strategies. The present study therefore asks whether the multidimensional mediums in which print is situated, influences children’s perceptions of print literacy. Situated within the discourse on gender and reading, this study will investigate young children’s perceptions of reading, and their self-perceptions of becoming a reader at the time of entry into the formal education system. The results of the pilot study will be presented together with the preliminary findings from the first phase of the main study. Despite considerable research suggesting that boys and girls may have different attitudes towards reading, and offering reasons why, this phenomenon has not been explored from the perspectives of very young children themselves. As yet, the extent to which young girls and boys enter the formal education system with perceptions of themselves as readers remains unknown. Furthermore, much previous work on gender and reading has over-simplistically polarised children into categories of ‘underachieving boys’ and ‘successful girls’. This work will therefore avoid such simple binary classifications and accept that gender issues are inextricably bound with other issues of self-identity. This study acknowledges the social and cultural context in which children develop strategies to make meaning from texts and form ideas about themselves as readers. The role of multidimensional texts, particularly related to changes in technology, is especially salient to this project. Recognising that the reading of multimodal texts goes largely unrecognised in the curriculum and its assessment (Bearne, 2004) and that the acquisition of print reading skill is continually given precedence in schools above other forms of reading (Pahl, 2002; Anning, 2003; Marsh, 2003) the pilot study suggested that nursery aged children may be inheriting these values. This longitudinal multiple-case study follows two cohorts of children as they move into and out of the Reception year. The study asks whether children’s self-perceptions change during this transitional phase of early education and whether children respond differently, depending on whether they are in a home or school setting. Not only has research in this field failed to investigate children in their early years, there is also a dearth of research concerned with hearing ‘the voice of the child’ (Nutbrown and Hannon, 2003). Subsequently, research tools in the form of games and activities have been designed and piloted to collect data directly from the children themselves. Whilst maintaining this commitment to hearing the pupil voice, teachers and parents will also be interviewed about the children’s reading behaviour and attitudes. This will provide the study with greater context. Unless we understand young children’s conceptualisations of reading and whether these early self-perceptions are gendered, then current compensatory programmes can be at best remedial. This paper suggests that if boys and girls are to achieve similar levels of success, systematic change is needed from the Foundation stage onwards. 3 Introduction This study, situated within the discourse on gender and reading, investigates the nature of boys’ and girls’ self-perceptions of reading, as they enter the formal education system. For over a decade, a considerable amount of research has attempted to explain why boys and girls may have different attitudes towards reading (Millard, 1997; Barrs and Pidgeon, 1993). Studies in the field have suggested that boys’ reading behaviour and attitudes towards reading may be influenced by a variety of factors, including issues of proficiency judgement (Moss, 1999; 2000). Yet surprisingly, this phenomenon has not been explored from the perspectives of very young children. The extent to which young boys and girls enter the formal education system with perceptions of themselves as readers is therefore unknown. As many researchers have recognised (Scott, 2000; Langston e al, 2004), if we want to receive valid information about children’s views then it is the children themselves we need to ask. Subsequently, this study has been designed to collect data directly from Nursery and Reception-aged children themselves. Clearly, methodological implications for the collection of data from young informants remain a crucial concern within this study. This work also acknowledges that much previous research on gender and reading has over-simplistically polarised children into categories of ‘underachieving boys’ and ‘successful girls’. This study will question the prevalence of such simple binaries, accepting that gender issues are inextricably bound with other issues of self-identity. Drawing from several different fields of literature, this study therefore acknowledges the social and cultural context in which children develop strategies to make meaning from texts and form ideas about themselves as readers. This involves a widening of the definition of ‘reading’, commonly used in the discourse on gender and reading. This is connected to the role of multi-dimensional texts, particularly related to developments in technology. This investigation seeks to understand very young children’s perceptions of themselves as readers – readers in a multimodal society. However, the reading of multimodal texts goes largely unrecognised in the curriculum and its assessment (Bearne, 2004). Moreover, as the acquisition of print reading skill is continually given precedence in schools above other forms of reading (Pahl, 2002; Anning, 2003; Marsh, 2003), it seemed likely that children too may be inheriting this value structure. This was in fact found to be the case in the pilot study for this project, where six nursery-aged children were questioned about their thoughts on learning to read. It was discovered that the children in this study reported a belief that print was of substantial importance in text and regarded an ability to decode print to be of greater value than other meaning-making strategies (Levy 2004). With this in mind, this study now asks how the acquisition of print reading skill is influenced by the presence of multimodal texts, and in particular the growing use of computer technology. Are children’s perceptions of themselves as readers of print, influenced by the multi-dimensional media in which the print occurs? And is there a gender difference in these perceptions? Furthermore the literature consistently highlights the importance of a child’s own home environment in literacy development (Minns, 1997; Tizard and Hughes, 1984; Pahl, 2002). Yet there is also a real concern that children entering the formal education system quickly forsake the literacy skills acquired at home, for those which are more ‘in keeping’ with accepted school practice. To what extent then, do 4 children’s perceptions of themselves as readers change as they move through the first few years of formal schooling? And is there a difference in their attitudes towards, and perceptions of reading depending on whether they are in a home or school setting? Subsequently, there are two broad questions, forming the basis of this investigation. These are… 1. What is the nature of boys’ and girls’ perceptions of themselves as readers, as they enter the formal education system? 2. Are young boys’ and girls’ perceptions of print literacy influenced by the multidimensional media in which the print is situated? In order to answer these two questions, there are several sub-questions. Each subquestion expands on an area of the main research questions as demonstrated in Figure 1.1. a. What are young boys’ and girls’ perceptions of what reading is, and what is ‘a reader’? b. Do boys’ and girls’ perceptions of reading, and self-perceptions of being a reader, change over time and in what ways? c. Do boys’ and girls’ self-perceptions vary depending on whether they are in a home or school setting? d. If they are developed, what factors lead to their formation and influence selfperception? Figure 1.1 Research Questions Question 1. The nature of self-perceptions (as potentially gendered). a. What is ‘reading’ and being ‘a reader’? c. Influence of home/school setting. Question 2. Influence of multi-dimensional media (as potentially gendered). b. Changes over time. d. Factors leading to formation of self-perceptions. 5 These are the questions and issues guiding the formation of this study. This paper presents findings drawn from the pilot which have in turn shaped the main study. Some findings from the first phase of data collection are also presented however it must be recognised that this is an exploratory study, with an on-going process of analysis taking place throughout the year of data collection and beyond. Building on Glaser and Strauss’ (1967) original interpretation of grounded theory analysis, this study accepts Harry et al’s (2005, p5) claim that the analysis is a gradual process involving a movement “from coding to conceptual categories, and thence to theory development”. The results presented here will subsequently inform the next stage of data collection. Research Design This is an interpretive study, following an inductive research methodology. As the study is concerned with the ways in which school and home cultures may influence young children’s perceptions of themselves, it is essential that the interpretivist paradigm framing this study allows for an engagement with cultural criticism. The nature of reading is therefore explored within a social and cultural context. For these reasons, the study has been framed within Denzin’s model of Interpretive Interactionism that recognises how “interacting individuals connect their lived experiences to the cultural representations of those experiences” (Denzin, 1992, p74) The Case Study Considering the research questions, with an emphasis on interpretation, a case study approach was adopted so as to abstract the desired depth of understanding. As raised, one aim of the study is to understand children’s changing perceptions as they move from one year group into another during the first few years of formal education. Two cohorts of children were therefore selected (one following children from Nursery into Reception, and the other from Reception into Year 1). Moreover, as the study is also concerned with understanding gender differences within the cohorts, the sample includes equal numbers of boys and girls. In other words, this study involves a number of individual ‘cases’. This approach has been described as a ‘multiple case study’ (Yin, 2004) which Yin argues can strengthen findings and make “interpretations more robust” (ibid, p xvii). Each child in this study could be regarded as an individual ‘case’ yet as Stake points out, when using more than one case, “there will be important co-ordination between the individual studies” (Stake, 1995,pp 3-4). As this is expected with regard to the individual cases in this study, I prefer to use Stake’s definition of collective case study, to describe the research strategy. The children have all been selected from the same school in central Cambridge. This school was originally selected on the basis of the pilot study. As the pilot study was designed to investigate Nursery children’s perceptions of reading, the first decision was whether to approach a Nursery school1 or a Nursery class within a primary school2. It was decided that a Nursery class within a primary school would be the most appropriate choice as the integration of a nursery into a wider school environment might offer more of an insight into children’s perceptions of the role of schools in encouraging reading development. 1 2 These schools cater for nursery aged children only. Not all primary schools have their own Nursery. 6 A school within the city of Cambridge, which shall be referred to as ‘Oakfield’, was then selected on the basis of its multi-cultural and diverse social catchment. This school has its own Nursery in the same building as the two Reception classes. Situated close to the centre of the city, the school admits children from a wide range of social, cultural and ethnic backgrounds. It is a large, popular school of 414 pupils, of whom a quarter travels from outside the catchment area. The 12 per cent of pupils known to be eligible for free school meals matches the national average. The number of pupils for whom English is a second language is high at 24 percent. The number of children on the register of special educational needs is below average at 17 per cent. In a recent OFSTED inspection, the teaching of pupils aged up to 5 years was assessed as “very good”, whereas teaching was assessed to be “good” in the rest of the school. However, rates of mobility within the school are high, with 10 per cent of pupils moving in and out of the area each year. Whereas this was not an issue for the short duration of the pilot study, it must be recognised that attrition is a potential problem for the longer study. Therefore the numbers of children selected for this investigation are slightly higher than actually required, in order to accommodate potential loss. Data collection began with a period of classroom observation in both the nursery and one of the reception classes. While these observations guided the selection of the children, there were many additional factors to take into consideration during the selection process. For example the sample includes only children who the teachers felt would be happy to participate in the research. Although this sample cannot be used as the basis for generalization, deliberate attempts were made to include children for whom English is not their first language as this reflected the overall school population. For reasons of validity, children who participated in the pilot study were not included, nor were children likely to leave the school during the course of the research. Having satisfied these criteria children were then selected on the basis of age, so that the sample included younger and older children in each year. Ethical considerations Practice according to the BERA Ethical Guidelines (2000) has also influenced the implementation of this project. Firstly, a letter was sent to the parents of each child, explaining the aims and structure of the study and stressing the longitudinal nature of the work. All parents gave consent for their child to participate and also provided written agreement to be interviewed in their home environment. Moreover, the children were also asked to give their consent before data collection commenced, though it was recognised that issues of ‘informed consent’ are especially problematic when working with children of this age. Following the example of Nutbrown and Hannon (2003) the pilot study was used not only as an opportunity to test various tools for data collection, but also to provide children with an opportunity to comment on how they felt when being interviewed and participating in the activities. The children were, in fact, all very enthusiastic about the activities used in the collection of data and tended to want to maintain the sessions for as long as possible. Finally, for reasons of confidentiality all names have been changed in all written reports. This includes the names of children, parents, teachers and the school. 7 The Pilot Study A variety of research tools were tested in the pilot stage. These tools were designed to enable young children to be informants in the research. As highlighted by Kellett and Ding (2004, p167) some researchers have considered children below the age of seven or eight as “not viable interviewees” because of their age, but “many writers and professional associations are now challenging this notion, maintaining that poor data are not necessarily a product of the young age but of inappropriate interview techniques”. Other research with Nursery-aged children has further shown that it is indeed possible to access the ‘voice’ of children of this age, so long as appropriate methods are used to collect data (Cremin and Slater, 2004; Brenna, 1995). For this reason, it seemed imperative that the activities should be carefully piloted and evaluated before being implemented into the main study. Three open-ended and age-appropriate activities were designed and tested in the pilot study (see Table 1), each allowing the children to talk about their thoughts on reading. The first activity was the closest in form to a semi-structured interview. The interview was conducted through the medium of a conversation with a glove puppet – a small yellow chick called Charlie Chick. The children were told that Charlie was born at Easter (the research was conducted in May) and was therefore very young. The children were then told that the chick did not know anything about school, but really wanted to learn some things from the children. In this respect, the children were offered the role of ‘expert’. Whilst the interview was guided by the responses of the child, certain aspects of the discussion ensured that the children were indeed asked to talk about their perceived ideas of reading and to explain what they thought reading actually involved. Table 1 Overview of activities used in pilot study Activity One Name Interview with puppet Activity Two Symbols Game Activity Three Small world play Props Charlie Chick puppet Sheet of letters and numbers Selection of reading material Pack of symbols cards Small world play equipment including dolls, beds, table, chairs, car, street signs Purpose To investigate children’s knowledge and attitudes about reading To investigate the skills children are using to make meaning from symbols. To investigate children’s perceptions of other children’s and adults’ reading behaviour. The second activity was designed to investigate the strategies these children were using to make meaning from a variety of signs and symbols, using a set of duplicate laminated cards. Each pair of cards displayed a sign or symbol commonly found in the local environment (such as car badge signs, supermarket logos, road signs and food labels). Having placed the cards face down on the floor, the children were then asked to turn two cards over to try and make a pair. The extent to which the game 8 was followed was guided by the interest of each child. Yet in every case, the game provided an opportunity to ask the children to talk about the signs and to investigate how they were making meaning from the various symbols. The final activity took the form of role-play, using a selection of small world play equipment. This included a family of dolls and some home furniture. As the activity progressed, a small car and a selection of road signs were also introduced. The activity was designed to further investigate the children’s perceptions of reading, especially within a home context. The activity was loosely structured around 6 subactivities however it was intended that that the children should be able to play with the equipment, inventing their own role-play situations throughout. Results from the Pilot Many findings were drawn from this study, yet for the purposes of this paper only those most salient to the design of the main study will be discussed. At this point, the ways in which the pilot study provided methodological implications for future study becomes inextricably bound with issues of substantive content. For this reason, no attempt has been made to separate the two. Finding It was clear that the young children interviewed in the pilot study were already forming strong ideas about the process of learning to read and themselves as developing readers. Although the sample size was small, there was a consistency in the children’s beliefs that print was of substantial importance in text and that an ability to decode print is of greater value than other meaning-making strategies. Implication for main study Recognising that teachers and children seem to be placing a great deal of emphasis on the place of print, it now seems important to ask whether the multi-dimensional media in which the print is situated influences children’s perceptions of print literacy. Subsequently, the research tools have now been modified to include reference to multi-modal text usage in order to explore these issues. Finding Surprisingly, there was very little evidence in this study to suggest a gender difference in relation to these children’s perceptions of reading. Firstly, whereas many studies with older children have revealed gender differences in children’s reading interests (Millard. 1997, Osmont and Davis, 1987; Hall and Coles, 1997), it was interesting to note that the boys and the girls in this study seemed to have similar interests in the cards used in Activity 2. This included food labels, a car badge sign, supermarket signs, numbers and road signs as well as their choices of texts for the dolls in Activity 3. More importantly though, there was little evidence to suggest gender difference in these children’s attitudes about reading and in their perceptions of themselves as readers. This is an important observation, as Moss (2000) discovered that older boys and girls react differently to the judgements made about their proficiency as readers. This could be related to the fact that many of the children regarded themselves as too young to be able to read, but believed that they would learn to read during the Reception year. It is possible that these children did not feel as if their teachers, parents or peers were judging their reading ability at that point. 9 Implications for main study This suggested a need to include Reception children in the main study. For this reason, the main study will investigate children’s perceptions as they move into and out of the Reception year. Finding The only gender difference reported was in response to the question – do you think it will be ‘easy’ or ‘hard’ to learn to read? In fact all three of the girls stated that they thought it would be ‘easy’ to learn to read, whereas all three of the boys said they thought it would be ‘hard’. Implications for main study This suggests that children’s thoughts about the process of learning to read, in terms of perceived difficulty, is an area requiring specific attention. However, it is also clear that further study must ensure that terms such as ‘easy’ and ‘hard’ are clarified within the research design. For this reason, activities have been designed to investigate children’s perceptions of what is ‘easy’ and what is ‘hard’ in terms of skill acquisition, including the skill of learning to read. Main study As the study is concerned with understanding the potential changes in selfperception as children move into and out of the Reception year, this will be a longitudinal study, centred around three main phases of data collection (presented in Table 2). However, bearing in mind the exploratory nature of this work, these data points may well change as the enfolding results influence the research design. Table 2: Overview of data collection Phase 1 Summer 2005 (May – July) 2 Spring 2006 (January – April) 3 Summer 2006 (May – July) Data Collection Children - Two-part interview (school) Small world play activity (school) Unstructured interview (home) Teachers –Interview Parents – Interview in home Children – Observation (Reading behaviour) (General classroom interaction) Interview (school) Small world play activity (school) Teachers – Interview (new teacher) Children – Interview (school) Small world play activity (school) Unstructured interview (home) Parents - Interview in home In order to answer the research questions, a profile will be developed for each child in the study. With the child at the centre, each case will consist of a collection of different strands of data, including data from a parent and two different teachers. Observation schedules will also be drawn up, where the child’s reading behaviour will also be observed in the context of daily classroom activity. These profiles will allow for comparisons to be drawn within cases as well as between cases. As demonstrated in Table 3, it is anticipated that different strands of data will be drawn 10 together over the course of a calendar year to help build these profiles. As the profiles develop though, this may indicate a need to expand, re-focus or change some of the data points highlighted in these tables, which must currently be viewed as a guide only. Table 3: Inter-case Data Collection Summer 2005 Spring 2006 Child at school Child at school Child at home Teacher 1 Teacher 2 Parent/s at home Summer 2006 Child at school Child at home Parent/s at home The Tools for Data Collection The aim of this section is to describe the tools which will be used for data collection. In order for comparisons to be drawn across the strands of data collected, the tools used in Phases 2 and 3 must closely resemble those used in Phase 1. In other words, the activities will be largely repeated in order to draw conclusions about the children’s changing perceptions. However, this is a flexible design and it is expected that the tools will continue to be extended and modified as the study progresses. Moreover, there is also a risk that the children may become bored with the interviews if some degree of variation does not exist between the phases. Yet it is important that the study demonstrates a consistency of methodological content throughout the phases in order that patterns and themes can be detected. Within the confines of this paper, it is clearly not possible to report on every aspect of the data collected during Phase 1. For this reason I have decided to focus on two data points only (Charlie Chick Interview Part 1 and the Small World Play) As stated, interviews have also taken place with the parents in the children’s homes and with the teachers. Furthermore, in the Charlie Chick Interview Part 2, the children have also worked directly with a computer. While no attempt has been made to report on these aspects of the data in this paper, this data will clearly be very valuable in creating a context for the children’s individual profiles. Activity 1; Charlie Chick Interview As this interview is quite lengthy, it was conducted on two separate occasions, so as to minimise the risk of the children losing interest. As described in the pilot, the whole of the interview was conducted through the medium of a glove puppet (Charlie Chick). The interviews were recorded using a simple tape-recorder. All activities began by allowing the children time to play with the props. The first section of the interview (part one) was used as a general introduction. This allowed the children time to become comfortable with the interview situation. In the second section, Charlie asked some general questions about school and the purposes of school. A main focus of this interview was to explore the children’s understanding of the terms ‘hard’ and ‘easy’ and apply this to the concept of learning to read. Charlie showed the children a collection of photographs. On each picture a child (the same child) was performing a task, as demonstrated in Figure 1. The children were told that in each case, the child is learning to do something. Through the context of the interview, the children’s thoughts on how easy or hard it is to learn to do these things was explored. The children were asked to play a game where the pictures were 11 placed in either an ‘easy’ pile or a ‘hard’ pile depending on how the child assessed the skill. Figure 1 Pictures of ‘skills’ used in Activity 1 Skills shown in picture Riding a bike Reading a book Running Tying shoelaces Reading words on a computer screen Painting a picture Writing words with a pencil Writing words on a computer Activity 2; Small World Play This activity, modified on the basis of the pilot study, took the form of role-play using small world play equipment. This included a family of dolls (mother, father, younger sister, younger brother and an older sister), lounge furniture (including chairs, television, video, and table) and study furniture (including a desk, chair, computer, and printer). This activity was designed to further investigate the children’s perceptions of reading, especially focussing on the home environment. Once again, the children were given the opportunity to play with the equipment before any structure was given to the activity. Then, by means of introduction, each character doll was introduced to the children and their role in the family was discussed. The next section of the activity was concerned with gaining an understanding of the children’s attitudes towards learning to read. During the activity, pictures of a Nursery/ Reception class of children was shown, so as to encourage the children to talk about ‘home’ and ‘school’ situations. The children were also encouraged to talk about their perceptions of what they thought might be involved in ‘learning to read’, when they move into a new class the following year. A proportion of this activity was also designed to further investigate the children’s perceptions of print reading in multimodal forms. As is explicated in the activity plan, this involved placing the dolls in front of the television and the computer and asking the children to comment on the use of such artefacts and the dolls ‘abilities’ to use such equipment. The children’s perceptions of the dolls’ attitudes towards reading print in multimodal forms were woven into the role-play. Preliminary results from Phase 1 of the main study The results presented here from Phase 1 are partial and represent only a section of the overall analysis. Over the course of the study analysis in greater depth will take place both within and across cases as represented in Figure 2. 12 Figure 2: Data Analysis Within case analysis Data Across case analysis Analysis of individual activities and interviews, across cases Analysis of each individual case (create vignettes) Identify themes and patterns within cases Modify tools for next stage of data collection Synthesis. Consolidating themes and patterns within and across cases. Inform next phase of data collection. Identifying themes and patterns across activities/interviews The children’s responses have so far been categorised into 3 broad areas. What the children have said about learning to read (with a focus on children’s definition of ‘reading’) What the children have said about the use of multi-modal texts (with a focus on screen texts) Note – the term ‘multi-modal text’ will be used in this thesis to describe a variety of texts including the use of picture reading. However, the analysis has so far focussed on the screen reading component of multimodality which is thus reported on in this paper. What the children have said about print on screen/ screen reading What the children have said about learning to read A Starting Point Most of the children in this sample demonstrated fairly positive views about reading and learning to read, however this did not mean that they all thought the activity was ‘easy’. For example, when given the ‘skills’ pictures (see Figure 1) and asked to categorise the picture as either an ‘easy’ thing to learn to do, or a ‘hard’ thing to learn to do, only 3 of the 12 children rated learning to read books as ‘easy’ and only 2 of the 12 rated learning to read words on the computer as ‘easy’. While this brief section of quantitative analysis cannot reveal much about these children’s thoughts on reading, it does provide an interesting starting point for the investigation. Firstly it is noteworthy that all of the positive responses were given by boys (see Table 4). Although this is only a small sample, the results do appear to contradict much other work with older children, where girls are consistently reported as having more positive attitudes about reading in comparison with boys (Millard, 1997; Hall and Coles1997; Barrs and Pidgeon, 1993). 13 Table 4 Children’s responses to difficulty in skill acquisition (reading and writing) No. of girls rating skill as ‘easy’ Reading books Reading on computer Writing with pencil Writing on computer No. of girls rating skill as ‘hard’ No. of boys rating skill as ‘easy No. of boys rating skill as ‘hard’ 0 0 6 5 3 2 3 4 4 1 1 4 6 5 0 1 Table 5 Children’s responses to difficulty in skill acquisition (non reading and writing) No. of girls rating skill as ‘easy’ No. of girls rating skill as ‘hard’ No. of boys rating skill as ‘easy No. of boys rating skill as ‘hard’ Tying 1 5 0 6 shoelaces Running 5 1 4 2 Cycling 4 1 3 3 Painting 6 0 5 1 Note – In most cases each row will add up to 12 (each cell being out of 6 boys or 6 girls). However one girl chose to include a ‘middle’ rating for Reading on Computer, Writing on Computer, Writing with Pencil and Cycling. As these results are not represented on the tables, these rows will add up to 11. While the boys and the girls in this study reported very similar perceptions of difficulty for the non-reading and writing activities (see Table 5), there was a much larger discrepancy reported for the reading and writing activities. Not only are the boys in this sample reporting more positively about learning to read, more boys are also claiming that learning to write is ‘easy’ in comparison with the girls. As these results do diverge from those in previous research with older children, this further justifies the need for an in-depth understanding of young children’s perceptions of reading. Moreover, these results can only be analysed using the binary categories of ‘boy’ and girl’. While it is helpful to begin with the knowledge that the boys in this sample do appear to be reporting positively about learning to read, we must now begin the process of understanding what the children in this sample believe reading to be, and how they see themselves readers in the society in which they live. The Analysis From these data gathered so far, Tom (Reception) and Simai (a boy in nursery) are the only two children in the sample to claim that learning to read is ‘easy’ and to claim that they can read. Three girls (two from nursery) claimed that they could read, but still rated learning to read as being ‘hard’. Five other children (four from Reception – two girls and two boys) rated learning to read as being ‘hard’ and also claimed that they were unable to read themselves. It would therefore appear that for most of the children, their perception of difficulty is related to their perceived ability. However some girls are claiming that they can read, but are still rating the activity as ‘hard’. Moreover, whilst these data provides a useful foundation, they are not intended to stand in isolation from the rest of the data. For example, although all of the girls in the reported activity said that learning to read was ‘hard’ (rather than ‘easy) some did provide positive responses in other activities, suggesting that reading was an ‘easy’ activity. For example Imogen, a Reception girl, told Charlie that you learn “things like maths…and…letters” at school, which is “quite easy for me”. When asked which of the activities she was able to do, Imogen confidently replied “I can do all of them”. 14 When asked to comment specifically about reading she replied “that’s quite easy. Because you just look at the pictures and that gives you an idea of what the words say”. This suggests that Imogen feels comfortable with the strategies she is using to make meaning from texts, which includes the use of picture cues. Furthermore, she also described reading as “quite fun” and thought that Charlie would enjoy learning to read because “I enjoyed it when I learned to read”. However, there are also suggestions in Imogen’s interview that she was less happy with school-based reading than home-reading. For example when asked what it means if someone is good at reading she replied “it means you just read – and get on with it, even if you don’t want to read. The teacher says you need to read”. In a second activity she also stated that it was easier to read “at home, because I’ve got my mummy to help me”. The structure of school-based reading in Reception was also highlighted by Malcolm. When asked the question ‘what is reading?’ he answered “when the teacher calls your name and then you have to read and at home your mummy and daddy reads books to us”. Like Imogen, Malcolm seems to be describing reading as a very structured activity, where you perform the task when asked by the teacher. Again, he stated that he preferred reading at home rather than at school, but this seemed to be because he would be read to at home rather than having to do the reading himself. Consistent with the findings in the pilot study, many of the children in both Nursery and Reception appeared to believe that reading was ‘hard’ for them at the moment, but that they would master the skill as they got older. For example Sarita, a girl in Reception who consistently referred to reading as being ‘hard’, was asked to show Charlie what you do with a book. She replied, “You look at the pictures and you act like a big boy or a big girl, properly with the books”. Similarly Huda, a girl in Nursery, showed a great deal of enthusiasm about books but again reported that “big girls…big girls like this” know how to read and that “little babies don’t know how to read”. Whereas Simai, a boy in nursery, agreed that reading is hard “for little children” but declared that he could read books “because now I’m bigger I can read books without my mum now”. However, this data does not investigate differences between these children’s perceptions and their practices. The children’s reading behaviour now needs to be investigated through a context of classroom observation During the small world play activity there were again many comments from boys and girls in Reception and Nursery suggesting that the children had positive images of themselves as readers. For example, when given the family of dolls to play with, most of the children were asked ‘does anyone in this family like reading?’ Interestingly two of the children (a boy in Reception and a girl in nursery) answered “me!” even though the question was not directed at them. Three other Reception children reported that they thought the ‘little children’ in the family would like reading best, while Simai (a nursery boy) suggested that the little brother would want to read the most. Only one child thought that ‘the mummy’ liked reading the most, with the remaining 4 children either answering ‘don’t know’ or suggesting that nobody would want to read. Finally, consistent with findings in the pilot study, many children were again making strong connections between ‘reading’ and ‘decoding words’. For example, when asked what it means if you can’t read, one child answered “that means you don’t know letters” while another stated “you still have to look at these bits”, while pointing to the printed text. Figure 3 highlights some further comments from the children 15 (given during the Charlie Chick interview) suggesting links between print and reading, when asked to describe what reading is. Figure 3 Children’s comments about reading and print Children’s comments ‘you look at letters and read it’ ‘you have to do group reading’ ‘you need words’ ‘it’s words’ ‘you have to tell the name of a book’ ‘reading the writing’ ‘you have to point under the words, so if you’re reading a story to everyone, they’ll know which words you are reading’ ‘hard work and practise’ ‘you have to look at these bits (the print)’ In conclusion these preliminary results suggest that these cohorts of children feel largely positive about learning to read and have positive perceptions about themselves as readers, yet most children regard the activity as being ‘hard’. It is possible that the boys in this study feel more positively than the girls but this requires further investigation. Most of the children seem to be linking their perceptions of difficulty with perceptions of their own abilities in reading. This data now needs to be analysed within a context of data gathered from the teachers and the parents in order to deepen an understanding of these children’s self-perceptions. There are also suggestions that some children may be unhappy with the formalised structure of reading activity taking place in Reception. Finally, consistent with results from the pilot study, many of these children are making strong links between ’reading’ and ‘printed text’, suggesting that they are already viewing decoding strategies as being ‘reading’. The children seem to believe that these skills are hard for younger children, but will be mastered with age. What the children have said about the use of multi-modal texts On the basis of these data gathered so far, it is clear that all of these children are familiar with multimodal texts, with many demonstrating a sophisticated ability to describe the use of computers in particular. However, when asked during the Small World Play activity whether they would prefer to keep the television or the computer, assuming they had to make a choice, most of the children said they would prefer to keep the television. However, it must be acknowledged that not all of the children in the sample have access to computers at home which may have influenced their responses. Within the context of the Small World Play, the children were also asked to talk about ‘mummies’’ and ‘daddies’’ use of computers. As the activity was unstructured, the same questions were not asked of every child, yet similar issues were indeed addressed in each interview. Four of the children reported that daddies ‘do work’ on the computer, but no child was able to elaborate on what that work actually was, except in the case of Simai who described the work as ‘boring’. Daddies were also reported as playing games, looking at the news and watching films on the computer. Mummies, on the other hand, were reported as doing ‘writing’, looking up the weather, putting games on for the children and looking at pictures with the children. One girl suggested that mummies and daddies would both use the computer to find information on holiday homes if the family was about to take a holiday. 16 There were several suggestions that the ‘mummy’ would prefer the television to the computer, but this was never the case for the ‘daddy’. However, there were also several suggestions that the computer would be most popular with the children in the house rather than the adults. This suggests that these children are aware of the use of multimodal texts in their homes. While some evidence exists to suggest that these children may be developing gendered stereotyped ideas about the adults’ use of some texts (the television and the computer in particular) the children seemed less inclined to provide such stereotyped ideas about ‘children’s’ use of the same texts. What the children have said about print on screen/ screen reading So far, these activities have sought to investigate whether these children are aware of print in the context of computer and television use. The second Charlie Chick interview (which has not yet been analysed) explores these issues in far greater detail. When asked whether there are any words on the computer and/or the television, most children replied ‘no’ for both. Tom (Reception) was the only child to answer “yes”. While he could not give an example of words he might see on the television or the computer, he reported that the words might be “for information”. Simai (Nursery) originally stated that there were no words on the computer, however he then went on to describe where letters and numbers should be on the keyboard, with substantial accuracy. Imogen (Reception) also reported that there were no words on the computer or the television, except ‘scores’ at the end of a computer game. When asked if she could read these scores she replied that this was ‘easy’. Conclusion This paper has sought to describe a research study into young children’s perceptions of reading and self-perceptions of becoming a reader at the time of entry into the formal education system. Based on the results of a pilot study, this project has been designed to understand potential differences in boys and girls perceptions of reading within the multimodal system in which they live. Although preliminary, results so far suggest that previous research on gender and reading cannot be applied to children of this age and that there is a necessity to investigate the phenomenon from the perspectives of young children. These results also help to sharpen the overall study by prompting further questions for investigation. For example, the children in this study may feel positively about learning to read, but what exactly do they perceive ‘reading’ to be? Do they connect their own ‘reading’ with being read to, and if so, to what extent? Do they connect concepts of ‘ability’ with concepts of ‘difficulty’? Furthermore, despite their own reported commitment to printed texts, do they value the decoding of pictures as a strategy for finding meaning in texts? And to what extent does schooled-reading practise influence these children’s attitudes towards learning to read and views of themselves as ‘readers’? These results demonstrate that children are using a variety of multimodal texts in their homes and in schools, yet they do not seem to be especially aware of print in multimodal contexts (television and computer screen print in particular). To what extent then are children making meaning from print in multimodal forms? Do these young children regard print as a more integral part of text when it appears in contexts 17 other than in books? In other words, is print in multimodal contexts less threatening to some young children than print in books? These results also indicate that more needs to be understood about the difference between these children’s perceptions and their actual practices. This suggests that a period of close observation must be built into the next phase of data collection in order to understand these connections and/or differences more fully. Finally, these observations will also contribute towards the creation of the children’s profiles. It is now imperative that vignettes are drawn up for each child so that an indepth understanding of the thoughts, ideas and attitudes that form the basis of these children’s perceptions of reading can be gained. In doing so, it is anticipated that this study will provide a basis to our understanding of young children’s self-perceptions of reading in this digital age, thus informing practise from the Foundation Stage and beyond. 18 References Anning, A., (2003) ‘Pathways to the graphicacy club: The crossroad of home and preschool’, Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, Vol 3, No 1, pp 5 - 35 Barrs, M. and Pidgeon, S. (eds) (1993) Reading the Difference: Gender and Reading in the Primary School, London, CLPE Bearne, E., (2004) ‘Multimodal texts; what they are and how children use them’, in Evans, J., (ed) New ways of Reading, New Ways of Writing; Using Popular Culture, New Technologies and Critical Literacy in the Primary Classroom, New York, Heinemann BERA (2000) (British Educational Research Association), Ethical Guidelines, http;/www.bera.ac.uk/guidelines.html (accessed 2/4/05) Brenna, B. (1995) ‘The metacognitive reading strategies of five early readers’, Journal of Research in Reading, Vol 18, No 1 pp 53 - 62 Cremin, H. and Slatter, B., (2004) ‘Is it possible to access the ‘voice’ of pre-school children? Results of a research project in a pre-school setting’, Educational Studies, V 30, No 4, pp 457 - 470 Denzin, N. K., (1992) Symbolic Interactionism and Cultural Studies, Cambridge, Basil Blackwell Glaser, B. G., and Strauss A. L., (1967) The Discovery of Grounded Theory; strategies for qualitative research, Chicago, Aldine Hall, C. and Coles, M. (1999) Children’s Reading Choices, London, Routledge Harry, B., Sturges, K.M., and Klinger, J.K., (2005) ‘ Mapping the Process; An Exemplar of Process and Challenge in Grounded Theory Analysis’, Educational Researcher, Vol 34, No. 2, pp3 - 13 Kellett, M. and Ding, S., (2004) ‘Middle Childhood’, in Fraser, S., Lewis, V., Ding, S., Kellett, M. and Robinson, C., (eds) Doing Research with Children and Young People, London, Sage Langston, A., Abbott, L., Lewis, V. and Kellett, M., (2004) ‘Early Childhood’, in Fraser, S., Lewis, V., Ding, S., Kellett, M. and Robinson, C., (eds) Doing Research with Children and Young People, London, Sage Levy, R., (2004) Nursery children’s perceptions of reading; methodological implications for future study, Thesis presented for the degree of M Phil, Cambridge University Marsh, J., (2003) ‘One-way traffic? Connections between Literacy practices at home and in the Nursery’, British Educational Research Journal, Vol 29, No.3, pp 369 - 382 Millard, E., (1997) Differently Literate, London, Routledge Falmer Minns, H., (1997) Read it to me now!: Learning at home and at school, Buckingham, Open University Press 19 Moss, G., (1999) The fact and fiction research project. Interim findings. Southampton, UK, University of Southampton. Moss, G., (2000) ‘Raising boys’ attainment in reading; some principles for intervention’, Reading, Vol 34, No. 3, pp101 – 106 Nutbrown, C. and Hannon, P., (2003) ‘Children’s Perspectives on Family Literacy: Methodological Issues, findings and Implications for Practice, Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, Vol 3, No 2, pp115 – 145 Osmont, P., and Davis, J. (1987) Look and Listen: an account of girls’ and boys’ achievement in reading and mathematics in the primary school, London, ILEA Pahl, K., (2002) ‘Ephemera, mess and miscellaneous piles: Texts and practices in families’, Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, Vol 2, No 2, pp 145 – 166 Scott, J. (2000) Children as Respondents, In Christensen, P. and James, A. (Eds) Research with Children; Perspectives and Practices, London, Routledge Stake, R. E. (1995) The Art of Case Study, London, SAGE Tizard, B. and Hughes, M., (1984) Young children learning: Talking and Thinking at Home and at School, London, Falmer Yin, R.K., (ed) (2004) The Case Study Anthology, London, SAGE