Regression Toward the Mean The phenomenon of regression toward the mean was first noted by Sir Francis Galton in the nineteenth century. He observed that in any series of random events clustering around an average, an extraordinary event is, by mere chance, most likely to be followed by an ordinary event. Thus, very tall fathers are likely to have slightly shorter sons and very short fathers, somewhat taller sons. Regression toward the mean helps explain why great movies are often followed by poor sequels, why poor presidents often have better successors, and why extremely intelligent women tend to have slightly duller husbands. Paul Schaffner provides an excellent example of regression toward the mean that you can present in class. Participants in his study assumed the role of a teacher attempting to encourage a hypothetical student to arrive promptly for an 8:30 A.M. class. The student’s arrival time, which varied from 8:20 to 8:40 for 15 consecutive days, was recorded on a computer. Each day the teachers could choose to praise, reprimand, or say nothing to the student. As expected, they praised him when he was early and reprimanded him when he was late. Unknown to the teachers, the student’s arrival time was pre-programmed and thus unrelated to the teacher’s response of the previous day. Due to regression alone, the student’s arrival time tended to improve, that is, regress to 8:30, after being punished for being late and to deteriorate (again by regressing to 8:30) after being praised for arriving early. Schaffner found that 70 percent of his teachers concluded that reprimand was more effective than praise in producing prompt attendance by the student. Regression toward the mean operates with regularity in sports, particularly when luck is mixed with skill. While sports commentators recognize its effect, they often offer different explanations. Amos Tversky notes, “Listen to the commentators at the Winter Olympics. If a ski jumper has done well on his last jump, they say, ‘He’s under immense pressure, so he’s unlikely to do as well this time.’ If he did poorly, they say, ‘He’s very loose and can only improve.’” Perhaps the so-called “Sports Illustrated Jinx” can also be understood in terms of regression toward the mean. According to sports folklore, the “Sports Illustrated Jinx” dooms teams or athletes appearing on the cover to lose after they are featured. For example, Earvin “Magic” Johnson of the Los Angeles Lakers graced the cover when his team was leading the NBA championship series. The Lakers then lost the title to the Boston Celtics in seven games. Similarly, the New York Islanders were on the cover going for their fifth straight Stanley Cup. They lost four straight to Edmonton. Tennis players and golfers seem to suffer the most after appearing on the cover. Researchers Tim Leone and Robbie Gluckson found that the performance of these athletes fell off more than 83 percent of the time. The performance of swimmers, skiers, foot-ball rushers, and crew members also dropped off significantly after cover appearances. At the same time, the researchers found that baseball pitchers and teams, as well as basketball players and teams (“Magic” Johnson being one exception), did well more than 70 percent of the time after they were on the cover. Several observers have noted that athletes appear on the cover only after performing unusually well. Regression toward the mean would explain their poorer subsequent performance.