Miles Myers THE PROBLEM OF TEACHER AUTHORITY There are two ways to have influence as a teacher—one is the use of political power and the other is the use of recognized authority. Political power is the organization of constituencies around particular goals—getting the necessary votes to take power within an organization and getting collective bargaining laws which require the resolution of disputes within a political arena. Both political goals have been achieved by classroom teachers over the last twenty years. Classroom teachers control their organizations, both the American Federation of Teachers and the National Education Association, and teachers in most places have collective bargaining or some kind of negotiations allowing them to use power relationships to influence school policies. But at the same time that teachers have improved their ability to influence school policy through power, they have continued to lose professional authority. Professional authority assumes that teaching is an intellectual discipline requiring of practitioners thoughtful study and insight. In other words, if teachers had authority, teachers would be recognized as the experts on how to teach at a given grade level and/or in a given subject area. But this is not the case. In fact, authority for K-12 teaching has been completely conferred on those who do not teach in K-12 classrooms. But what is wrong with that? And if something is wrong with that, what causes the problem and what can be done to correct it? These are the two questions which this article will address. First, what is wrong with K-12 teachers having no professional authority? One problem is the same one Paracelsus found in the medical treatments of the sixteenth century. That is, medicine at that time was practiced by having doctors read books and give written rules to routine workers who were the ones who actually treated patients. Paracelsus argued that on-the-spot diagnosis was a critical step in medical treatment, no two patients or situations being exactly alike, and that doctors should treat patients, not just read books. The issue is the same in teaching. Can the teachers be routine workers who follow the written rules of the experts? The answer largely depends on one’s theory of learning. If one assumes, as does a behaviorist, that learning is an ordered pattern of stimulusresponse-reward, then teacher-proof materials are possible and teachers can be routine workers without professional authority. The major problem in such a learning theory is how to make the stimulus discrete enough so that the subject gets rewarded for exactly the right response. In writing programs, such an approach to teaching takes the form of a series of drills focusing on small (discrete) items, for instance, the floating -s in subjectverb agreement problems. The teacher’s job is routine—know the correct answer and mark papers with appropriate rewards (grades, checks, stars, M&M’s). In the meantime, the experts can experiment with more efficient ways of presenting the stimulus and the reward. Teacher-proof materials are also possible in an imprinting model of learning. This model assumes that there is an innate Language Acquisition Device (LAD) in the head and that if the LAD is triggered by the sound or appearance of language, the child will imprint on the language and begin to speak and then write. Learning in this model is very similar to what ducks do when they imprint on another duck or Konrad Lorenz. In other words, language learning eventually happens if children are surrounded by language. The task of the teacher is to surround the child with whatever language is available. In reading, teaching takes the form of Dan Fader’s approach in Hooked on Books, and in writing, this task is the write-everyday-as-much-as-you-can approach. In the meantime, the experts prepare new and longer writing assignments which have been tested on different groups for imprinting effects. The 5,000 Topics for Classroom Writing is a classic from this tradition. Both the imprinting and behaviorist models have been dominant influences in the teaching of reading and writing over the last fifteen years. But vestiges of an earlier model still linger. In this earlier model, the original-sin approach, students were assumed to be flawed (but improvable), and the purpose of school was to have students suffer a little so they could expiate their sins. Instruction required students to memorize and copy the words of such saints as Emerson and Bacon and to imitate the forms of argument identified by Cicero. In the early days of the original-sin approach, teachers were endowed with a moral authority very much like that conferred on preachers. As schooling became more secularized, however, the teacher’s role became more like that of Emily Post, emphasizing the writing manners of tasteful people. Finally under behaviorism all authority for teachers was stripped away. *** But a new model of learning has been emerging in the last twenty years, and this model requires that teaching authority once again be conferred on those who teach. Says Jerome Bruner, “What has emerged is a theory of mother-infant interaction in language acquisition—called the functioning theory—that sees language mastery as involving the mother as much as it does the child. According to this theory, if the LAD exists, it hovers somewhere in the air between mother and child” (Bruner, “Learning the Mother Tongue,” Human Nature, September 1978). The functional theory sees language as the result of interactions and the mother or teacher must use a caretaker language appropriate to the given child. In the classroom, the teacher must see writing assignments not as strict imitation or S-R-R, but as situations in which writing has a functional role. In this approach the teacher is organizing class projects and varying distances from speaker to audience and speaker to subject, always monitoring the interactions of individual students. In such an approach teachers must have a sense of authority. They cannot follow rules blindly. They must diagnose responses, must reflect on their experiences, and must think of themselves as scholars of their craft. As long as staff development programs treat teachers as if they are sick people who need treatment, teachers will behave like sick people. As long as teachers are treated as if they did not know anything, they will go in their classrooms and behave as if they do not know anything. Knowing nothing may be a workable assumption for teachers in behaviorist and imprinting models of learning, but it is not a workable assumption in a functional model of learning. Therefore, if a functional model of learning is to become a usable approach in classrooms, teaching authority must be taken from non-teachers and conferred on those who teach. This leads to question two: what causes the problem and what can be done to fix it? What causes the problem is primarily the way the role of researchers is defined in Schools of Education and government funding. Schools of Education in a University justify their existence on the grounds that they conduct research on issues of teaching. Yet much of the education research in these schools is related more to general issues of psychology and linguistics than to specific problems of teaching, and many of the researchers in Schools of Education have little or no experience in the public schools and cannot translate research findings into language understandable to teachers. But in both government funding agencies and in Schools of Education the assumption is that educational research will lead to rules for teachers to follow. The National Institute of Educations’s Beginning Teacher Evaluation Study in California had the stated purpose of using “research in the teaching evaluation area... in order to establish baseline entry skill data for beginning teachers” (1972) and researchers at NIE’s 1974 conference on studies in teaching repeated more than once their belief that studies of teaching should be used “to solve such problems as the selection and education of teachers” (N. L. Gage, p. 5, panel two of report). The way research begins as a correlational study and ends as assertions about cause-effect, all for the purpose of postulating rules for teachers, is nicely illustrated in these sentences from California’s Beginning Teacher Evaluation Study: The necessary condition for predicting that two variables are causally related is that one of them occurs prior to the other (p. 36)… Since we have reason to believe that the two variables are causally dependent, and since the path coefficient is significant, we infer a causal relation because the association between the variables cannot be attributed to their association with any other variable (p. 42)... As is well known, a causal inference cannot be made directly from a significant correlation between two variables. The complementary principle is, however, frequently ignored, namely, that when a significant correlation is found a causal relation exists somewhere (p. 50). When researchers into teaching and learning move from research to rule-making for teachers, these researchers have left their area of expertise and crossed over into an area where they are no better than the worst amateur. They have blurred the difference between a correlational finding in a research study and the causal relations implied in the rules given to teachers. The effect of these rules, other than their frequent uselessness and occasional negative effects, is that the teacher is taught not to trust what he/she already knows and can learn through sensible inquiry. Not all teachers succumb. Duckworth (1972) reports: The colleagues I admired most got along very well without any special knowledge of psychology. They trusted their own insights about when and how children were learning, and they were right. Their insights were excellent. Several researchers have called for a review of the relationship between researchers and teachers, Schools of Education and the teaching profession. Courtney Cazden in a reflection on her one year as a full-time public school teacher in 1974 reports: …the teachers all have to take courses at local colleges for salary increments and advanced degrees and rightfully resent being told what to do by professors who’ve never been in a classroom (p. 76)... Of course, research knowledge about language isn’t the only basis for improved action. I think it’s fair to say that there is a general trans-Atlantic contrast at this point. Whereas Americans like me have worked “down,” trying to derive implications for education from theories about language and its development, colleagues in England have worked “up” from instances of the best classroom practice... (1976, p. 81). McLaughlin and Marsh (1978) in summaries of the Rand Corporation’s findings (NIE Grant G74- 0055) have explained why the ”rules” of researchers are often not helpful to teachers: …in teaching as in other clinical settings, the appropriate strategy to resolve problems is unclear. To this point, the state of educational research is such that it is difficult to reach consensus concerning the value of most any set of teaching strategies. For teachers, the learning task is more like problem solving than like mastering “proven” procedures. Consequently, outside experts and tightly structured training are relatively less helpful than they are in technologicallydominant activities such as industry. (McLaughlin and Marsh, 1978, p. 87) Eleanor Duckworth describes how a set of rules from a research study can have negative effects on teachers who have no sense of professional authority: It is just as necessary for teachers as for children to feel confidence in their own ideas. It is important for them as people, and also important if they are really going to feel free to acknowledge the children’s ideas. If teachers feel that their class must do things just as the book says, and that their excellence as a teacher depends upon that, they cannot possibly accept children’s divergence and children’s creations. A teacher’s guide must give enough indications, enough suggestions, so the teacher has ideas to start with and to pursue in some depth. But it must also enable the teacher to feel free to move in directions of her own when other ideas arise. For instance, the teachers’ guides for his program include many examples of things children are likely to do. The risk is that teachers may see these as things that the children in their classes must do—whether or not the children do them becomes a measure of successful or unsuccessful teaching. In Duckworth’s example, the guide is given to teachers as part of a deficit model of staff development. This model clearly establishes that teachers have no professional authority. In general, the deficit model of staff development is characterized by the view of other educators that teachers need staff development because they lack the necessary skills to teach successfully. This characterization has several elements that must be understood if the deficit-model approach to staff development is to be changed. First, the deficit model is a collective view supported by members of diverse role groups such as principals, school district administrators, university professors, state department of education officials, and legislators. This leaves teachers with the belief that everyone is critical of them. Secondly, these outside groups bring to bear administrative regulations, credential requirements, university degree requirements, and state law as a network of reinforcement for their belief. The critical view of other educators is being powerfully communicated to teachers. Thirdly, teachers have typically been excluded from any discussion of their “deficit” or any discussion about how to carry out its removal and, finally, the deficit model has been based on the dogmatic belief of other educators that they know, and can justify, their statements about what constitutes good teaching. Though educational research developed over twentyfive years has not resolved the dilemma of what constitutes good teaching, deficit model outside experts or central office specialists often act as though they know. (McLaughlin and Marsh, 1978, p. 89) The cause of the problem, then, is first the misunderstood relationship between theory and practice and second the financial pressures which force many Schools of Education and educational researchers to claim practical expertise, particularly in teaching. Now the final question: what can be done to correct the problem? The first step is a good theory for a staff development model which gives authority to teachers. We already have a good learning theory in the functional theory or interaction theory of language acquisition. This theory of language development requires a teacher who is an active agent, flexible, and adaptive in the classroom. From philosophy comes a useful theory for staff development. T. F. Green argues that the purpose of teaching is to lead students from what is subjectively reasonable for them to believe to what is objectively reasonable for them to believe. This is, of course, what the summer institutes of the Bay Area Writing Project do. First, teachers are invited to the institutes as Fellows of the University. The title is the first step toward developing a sense of professional authority. Second, teachers are asked to present one idea that works. Teachers come with a set of beliefs that seem plausible (subjectively reasonable), at least as measured by experience, but in the course of the five-week institute the experiences of other teachers and findings of research are focused on these subjectively reasonable beliefs to arrive collectively at some understanding of what seems objectively reasonable. The aim is to transform subjectively reasonable beliefs to objectively reasonable beliefs. During the summer institute, there also develops a common language about writing, which enables teachers to engage in collective inquiry on the issues. The purpose of research and theories is not to form rules which teachers must follow but to provide alternative frameworks for examining what seems objectively reasonable about what one subjectively believes from experience. In time, as in any discipline, a concensus model begins to evolve. This same pattern is used in follow-up programs where only teachers are used as workshop leaders. It is teachers, not researchers, who can find ways to translate research into objectively reasonable insights because it is the teachers who know the culture of the school. School has a culture all its own, and curriculum theorists ignore it at their peril. Grades and points are one example. Students who have internalized the school culture want grades and, if possible, points. For such questions as “Do I get a grade on this? What is it worth?” and “How long should it be?” teachers need ready answers: “Yes, it will be graded by me or one of your peers. This assignment is worth twenty-three points, seven more than yesterday’s.” “And how long? Fold your paper in half. Notice where the crease appears. Write to there in the next ten minutes.” To argue that the questions are absurd and should be ignored or that teachers should give up grades and give freer, more open assignments is to ask many teachers to change the culture of their schools and communities. For many parents who already doubt the concern of teachers for their children, a teacher’s claim “I don’t give grades” is another way of saying, “I don’t much care what the students do.” And for many students, a teacher’s instruction to “write on whatever you want, as long or as short as you want to make it,” creates not liberation, but a sickening sense of existential nausea. Such students do not know what to do until the teacher assigns a topic, rigid and specific: “Everyone will write 423 words on why Oakland High School is the most beautiful spot on God’s green earth.” At that instant nine out of ten will have a topic— some will affirm, some oppose, and the rest will realize that they want to do something else, knowing this only after the teacher has told them what they must do. Or take directions. Curriculum researchers often tell us to give clear and complete directions so that students understand what to do. For example, in a Phi Delta Kappan article, (“News, Notes, and Quotes,” May-June 1977), Arthur L. Costa says, “Research by Kounin (1970) and Dalis (1970) indicates that the clarity of the teacher’s directions affects the student’s behavior. If the directions are confusing, incomplete, or too complex, the students will not be able to focus properly on the learning activity, and the chances for deviance will increase. Learning objectives must be stated precisely...” In the culture of the school, the theory of clear directions is often doomed. No matter how clear, no matter how complete, once the teacher says, “Get started,” the hands go up: “What do we do?” Has the teacher failed to give clear and complete directions? No. The students want the individual attention of the teacher explaining one-to-one. In such a situation, teachers might be better advised to mumble something unintelligible, say clearly, “Get started,” wait a minute for the hands to go up, and start around the room. Or spelling tests. Research may tell us that they don’t improve spelling very much, but for many classrooms spelling tests are like a mantra. The world has a center after all. Here is something we know is English. Here is something with a definite beginning, middle, and end—all within a short time span. The world, alas, has recognizable order. Many junior high teachers know that the routine of a spelling test helps calm students, helps center them, making more complicated lessons possible. Or books. Researchers often treat books as if they were only texts to be read. In schools, books are as much emblems, part of the uniform of school, as they are texts to be read. Many students desire books because they are part of the school uniform, and some students consider it a status symbol to be carrying a book which the students cannot read. An objective rule about readable texts is bound to be destructive to some subjectively reasonable beliefs understood by teachers. The model of the Bay Area Writing Project is, then, not just a way of improving the teaching of writing. It is a staff development model which recognizes both the authority of researchers for research and the authority of teachers for practice, and which attempts to establish a new relationship between Universities and the K-12 teaching profession and a new integration of theory and practice. A bibliography will be sent upon request. Write Miles Myers, Bay Area Writing Project. Miles Myers is Administrative Director of the Bay Area Writing Project, U.C. Berkeley.