CHAPTER 10 Cognitive Abilities OUTLINE I. TESTING FOR INTELLIGENCE Many psychologists agree with Robert Sternberg that three characteristics encompass intelligence: possession of knowledge, ability to efficiently use knowledge to reason, and ability to employ that reasoning adaptively in different environments. A. A Brief History of Intelligence Tests Alfred Binet’s original test included a series of age-graded items that demonstrated differences among children in reasoning, judgment, and problem-solving abilities. Children who answered questions at their age level were considered of “regular” intelligence. Louis Terman developed the Stanford-Binet test, devised a scoring method known as the intelligence quotient (IQ), and created the IQ test, which included questions for adults. Despite cultural biases, IQ tests were given to screen immigrants and to place soldiers in appropriate assignments. David Wechsler developed a new test that was made up of several subtests. Wechsler’s test reduced the extent to which answers depended on a certain culture and had some subtests that had little or no verbal content. B. Intelligence Tests Today The Wechsler test, which is designed to be administered individually, includes the verbal scale and the performance scale. The average result obtained by people at each age level is assigned an intelligence quotient, or IQ score, of 100. Each individual’s score is compared to the average for his or her age level in order to compute an IQ. Therefore, an IQ score is a relative measurement. C. II. Aptitude and Achievement Tests These two types of tests are closely related to intelligence tests. Aptitude tests are designed to gauge a person’s ability to learn or do certain things. Achievement tests measure how much a person already knows in a certain area. MEASURING THE QUALITY OF TESTS Tests have two advantages over other means of evaluation: They are standardized so that different people’s performances can be compared, and they are quantifiable (which allows the calculation of norms). A. Reliability If a test has reliability, a person will receive about the same score when tested on different occasions. There are three methods of checking the reliability of a test: test-retest, alternateform, and split-half reliability. If the correlation coefficient between two scores is high and positive, the test is considered reliable. B. Validity A test that has validity measures what it is designed to measure. There are several measures of validity, including content validity, criterion validity, and predictive validity. III. EVALUATING INTELLIGENCE TESTS A. B. The Reliability and Validity of IQ Tests 1. How Reliable Are Intelligence Tests? IQ tests usually provide consistent results. However, test-retest reliability can be low if the initial testing is done prior to seven years of age. The testing conditions and the person’s motivation when taking the test can also affect results. 2. How Valid Are Intelligence Tests? The validity of IQ tests is difficult to measure, in part because intelligence itself is difficult to define. IQ tests do a reasonably good job of predicting academic and job success, but scores can be distorted by the response to the tester. 3. How Fair Are Intelligence Tests? Controversy about the fairness of IQ tests continues, especially in terms of cultural factors. Although many of the technical problems in IQ tests have been solved, the social consequences of testing should be evaluated. Linkages: Emotionality and the Measurement of Cognitive Abilities People tend to perform at their best when their arousal level is moderate. Too much or too little arousal will result in decreased performance. Those whose arousal inhibits their performance in testing are said to suffer from text anxiety. Concern over negative stereotypes about the mental abilities of the group to which they belong can impair test performance of some members of ethnic minorities. In sum, people who are severely anxious about testing will not perform to the best of their ability. C. IQ Scores as a Measure of Innate Ability The influences of heredity and the environment interact to produce intelligence. Correlational studies with twins suggest that heredity influences the development of IQ. However, the environment also exerts a strong influence on IQ. Previously underprivileged children placed in homes that provide an enriching intellectual environment have shown moderate but consistent increases in IQ. And children placed in enrichment programs show improved health, academic achievements, and intellectual skills. D. Group Differences in IQ Scores An examination of the differences among group means on IQ tests does not provide information about specific individuals in those groups. A person in the low-score group may have an individual score that is much higher than a person from the high-score group. In addition, inherited features may be influenced by the environment. Socioeconomic Differences. A child’s ability is influenced by genetic factors and perhaps by the effects of the parents’ occupation and education on the home environment. Also, higher-income families may encourage a higher level of motivation to succeed. 2. Ethnic Differences. There is variation in average IQ scores between ethnic groups, but more variation within groups. Research indicates that differences in IQ among ethnic groups may be due to differences in socioeconomic environment, parental education, nutrition, health care, and schools. Also, people in some cultures may be more or less motivated during testing, depending on the value their cultures place on education or intelligence. Conditions That Can Raise IQ Scores Enrichment programs such as Head Start can cause at least temporary gains in IQ scores. Spending time in projects such as Head Start may cause a child to be more motivated and have a better attitude toward school. 1. E. F. IQ Scores in the Classroom IQ scores may influence teachers’ expectations about the abilities of their students. In turn, these expectancies may influence students’ performance. But IQ scores can also help educators identify student strengths and weaknesses, and offer a curriculum that will best serve the students. G. Thinking Critically: Are Intelligence Tests Unfairly Biased Against Certain Groups? What am I being asking to believe or accept? Intelligence tests are biased against some minority groups. What evidence is available to support the assertion? Differences in IQ scores may reflect motivation and a person’s trust in the test administrator. Some tests are not “culture fair.” People may interpret the test questions intelligently but differently than the defined “right” answer. Are there alternative ways of interpreting the evidence? Intelligence tests may be biased in assessing general intelligence, but they may still be good predictors of who will do well in school or on the job. What conclusions are most reasonable? Current tests, although not completely culture fair, can be useful as predictors of success in the culture in which they are administered. Probably no culture-free test of intelligence exists, as intelligence is defined by the behaviors that a culture values. Psychologists have turned to focus more energy on how to help people develop the abilities required for success in a particular culture. IV. UNDERSTANDING INTELLIGENCE A. The Psychometric Approach The psychometric approach tries to describe the structure of intelligence by examining the correlations among scores on various tests. Charles Spearman postulated two types of intelligence that account for test scores: general intelligence or g; and special intelligences, or s, which are the specific skills and knowledge needed to answer the questions on a particular test. L. L. Thurstone disagreed. He used factor analysis, a statistical technique, to find several independent primary mental abilities, including numerical ability, reasoning, verbal fluency, spatial visualization, perceptual ability, memory, and verbal comprehension. Later, Raymond B. Cattell argued that g exists and that it consists of fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence. Most psychologists agree that there is g. They just do not agree on what g is. B. The Information-Processing Approach Psychologists using the information-processing approach have tried to understand intelligence by examining the mental processes involved in intelligent behavior. Research suggests that those with greater intellectual ability have more attentional resources available when performing a task. Speed of basic processing plays only a small role in determining intelligent behavior. C. The Triarchic Theory of Intelligence Robert Sternberg proposed the triarchic theory of intelligence: analytic, creative, and practical. Analytic intelligence refers to the sort of intelligence that traditional IQ tests measure. Creative intelligence is what you would use to complete a generative task, such as composing music. Practical intelligence refers to one’s “common sense,” or ability to solve practical problems in life, such as determining what to do if you are stranded in a blizzard. D. Multiple Intelligences In his theory of multiple intelligences, Howard Gardner proposed that certain abilities are relatively independent of one another and that individuals may develop some “intelligences” more highly than others. Gardner suggested eight different intelligences: linguistic, logicalmathematical, spatial, musical, body-kinesthetic, intrapersonal (self-knowledge), interpersonal (social skills), and naturalistic. Other researchers have described emotional intelligence. E. V. Focus on Research Methods: Tracking Cognitive Abilities over the Life Span Age-related changes in mental abilities can be examined through cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. The cross-sequential with resampling design combines cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Results show that crystallized intelligence may continue to grow into old age. Fluid intelligence remains stable in adulthood and then declines in late life. Specifically, problems in working memory, processing speed, problem-solving strategy organization, flexibility, and control of attention appear late in life. DIVERSITY IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES A. Creativity Creativity is often assessed by tests of divergent thinking, which measure the ability to generate many different but plausible responses to a problem. Expertise in the field, a set of creative skills, and intrinsic motivation are necessary for creativity. External rewards can deter creativity. The correlation between IQ scores and creativity is not very high. IQ tests measure convergent thinking, whereas creativity is characterized by divergent thinking. B. Unusual Cognitive Ability 1. Giftedness. Those with extremely high IQs do not necessarily become creative geniuses. They do, however, usually become very successful in this society or culture. 2. Mental Retardation. This label is applied to people whose IQs are 70 or below and who fail at daily living skills. Mental retardation sometimes has very specific causes, such as Down syndrome. In most cases of familial retardation, there is no specific cause. Psychologists believe familial retardation results from an interaction between heredity and the environment. Children with mild retardation differ from other children in three ways: They perform certain mental operations more slowly, they know fewer facts about the world, and they are not very good at using particular mental strategies in learning and problem solving. 3. Learning Disabilities. People with learning disabilities have academic performance that does not measure up to their measured intelligence. People with dyslexia see letters as distorted or jumbled. Dysphasia is difficulty in understanding spoken words. In dysgraphia a person has trouble writing, and in dyscalculia a person has trouble with arithmetic.