ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun How do the risk factors of ecological levels influence the vulnerability of children? Sung, Ming-Jiun* Su, Hsiu-chih Paper presented at the European Conference on Educational Research, University of Ghent, 19-21 September 2007 ABSTRACT This study tested hypotheses from an ecological-developmental model for childhood vulnerability and resilience. In this model, vulnerability is defined as a child’s dysfunctional developmental outcomes which were rated by his/her kindergarten teacher. In addition the child’s teacher checked 33 indices of risk factors. These risk factors were from three types of ecological levels- individual characteristics, parenting processes, and environmental context. Data of 200 at-risk children were analyzed by using LISREL 8 statistical software. Structural equation modeling indicated that the effect of individual characteristics factor was significant and contributed uniquely to children’s vulnerability through parenting processes, but the effect of environmental context factor was not significant. Additional analyses suggested that some factors from environmental context can serve as a protective function when parenting processes are compromised. Further research is needed to examine the continuity and discontinuity of these distal and proximal risk factors that influence the developmental trajectories of these at-risk children. Keywords: risk factor, ecological system, vulnerability * Corresponding author 1 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun Introduction Over the past decades, much has been learned about factors that increase children’s risk of maladjustment; and a variety of risk factors have been identified as strongly tied to many kinds of child psychopathology. Understanding these risk factors can provide a knowledge base for establishing preventive interventions for children and family at-risk (Cowen, Wyman, Work, & Parker, 1990), and contributed to knowledge of developmental processes, both normal and abnormal (Cicchetti & Cohen, 1995; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984). The challenge of understanding the origin precursors and core abnormalities of the vulnerability of at-risk children has led a number of investigators to the “high-risk method”. This method often focused on individuals who had experienced some adversities, such as born to a low SES family. These individuals were found to have a substantially higher risk for low academic achievement (Garmezy, 1978). The merit of this method is that it avoids confounding the consequences of maladjustment or disorders with their causes and precursors. However, the samples of different focused groups of maladjustment or disorders often had many overlapping characteristics. That means a lot of risk factors may co-occur, and these factors may have different impact on different ecological systems to different people. For example, living in a high-crime neighborhood is an index of risk factor. However, this factor too often co-occurs with poverty, minority status, and exposure to community violence, which also has substantial influence on children’s deviant development. It seems that all the risk factors have same damaging effects on children’s development, but it is not always so. Take the same example of living in a high-crime neighborhood; harsh parenting may serve as a protecting effect by keeping children away from deviant peers, and decreasing the events of becoming a victim of violence or witnessing violence occurring to others, although harsh parenting often is detrimental to children’s who are living in a safe community. So the same factor that serves a risk function under some circumstances may not serve same function under different conditions and may have different impacts on children’s developmental outcome. Besides, Bronfenbrenner (1977) suggested that not only the number of the risk factors play a key role on children’s psychological adjustment, but also the types of the risk factors. It means that social classes differ in many characteristics that foster or impede psychological development of children. The ecological model proposed by Bronfenbrenner(1977) emphasized the complexity of development and the large number of environmental influences from a variety of domains such as neurological domain, genetical domain, parenting domain, economical domain, family functioning 2 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun domain, social support domain……etc. The Philadelphia Study (Furstenberg, Cook, Eccles, Elder, & Sameroff, 1999) examined twenty factors in six different ecological domains, ranging from those Microsystems (Bronfenbrenner, 1977) in which child was an active participant, to those systems more distal to the child. These domains and variables included family processes (parental investment, support for autonomy); parent characteristics (education, mental health resourcefulness); family structure (household crowding, marital status); management of community (social resources, informal network); peers (antisocial, prosocial); community (neighborhood SES). In Lengua’s (2002) study of children’s responses to multiple risk, a multiple risk score was summed and consisted of 11 indices to represent demographic, psychosocial and environmental, three types of risk factors. Demographic risk factors included mother’s education level, family income, child’s ethnic or racial minority status, single parent status, density in the home, and adolescent parent status. Psychosocial risk factors included negative life events, maternal depression and family history of problem. Environmental risk factors included the quality of the home environment and the quality of the home environment and the quality of the home environment and the quality of the neighborhood environment. Sameroff and Fiese (2000) argue that the number of risk factors was the prime determinant of developmental outcomes, and the same outcomes were the result of different combination of risk factors. No single factor was consistently related to either poor or good outcomes. So for each child, we need to analyze his unique set of intervention strategies embedded within a developmental model. Rutter (1987) reminded us that if we want to help vulnerable children, we need to pay special attention to the protective processes that bring about changes in life trajectories from risk to adaptation, such as reducing the risk impact on children, reducing the likelihood of negative chain reactions to children, promoting self-esteem and self-efficacy of children, and opening up opportunities for children. In the Kauai Longitudinal Study (Werner & Smith, 1992), several risk factors that exposed to a cohort of children were examined in the beginning. As their longitudinal investigation progressed, those who successfully coped with their negative experiences or adversities attracted more attention from the researchers. Latent-variables path analyses were used to examine the links between protective factors in the individual and outside sources of support in childhood and adolescence that led to successful adult adaptation, and five clusters of protective factors were identified. Cluster 1 included temperamental characteristics of individuals that helped them to elicit positive responses from their caring persons. Cluster 2 included skills and values that led to their effective abilities, such as vocational plan. Cluster 3 included parent’s caregiving styles that fostered self-esteem in the child. Cluster 4 consisted of supportive adults who acted as gatekeepers for the future. Cluster 5 was the opening of opportunities at major life transitions. 3 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun Our primary interest in the present study was to determine the direct and indirect effects of risk factors from different levels of ecological system. We proposed a hypothetical model based on Guralnick’s (1998) early development and risk factor model Thurman’s (1997) ecological congruence model, and Brofenbrenner’s (1979) ecological systems approach. Our model depicted in Figure 1 guided our examination of the unique contribution that three types of ecological factors that influence the children’s developmental outcomes. Figure 1 showed that the children’s developmental outcomes were directly influenced by the factors of individual characteristics, parenting processes, and environmental context. Then the parenting processes factor had indirect effects on the child developmental outcomes through its effects on individual characteristics. The environmental context factor may also have indirect effects on child developmental outcomes through parenting processes and through individual characteristics. Individual risk sex Parenting risk age Child vulnerability Contextual risk Figure 1 Model summarizing the hypothesized pattern of relations among individual characteristics, parenting processes, and the environmental context, and the child developmental outcomes. Method Participants This study utilized a sample of 200 at-risk children. Participants were recruited through the children’s kindergarten. The selected kindergartens can represent a variety of sociodemographic characteristics of central Taiwan area. Information forms were sent to the kindergartens, and the principals were asked to nominated at-risk children in their kindergarten. Ninety-four kindergartens from 29 administrative districts notified the potential respondents to us. Then a risk factor checklist was sent to the teachers of these at-risk children. Two hundred and three mailed back checklists were useful for further analysis. Measures Risk indices measurement We used a checklist consisted of 3 subscales to collect the data of whether a child 4 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun was exposed to any risk that is detrimental to a child’s development. These risk factors were identified because they were either theoretically or empirically associated with developmental outcome. We included factors examined by Sameroff and colleagues (1987, 1993) and added a few other factors identified as risk factors for seven domains of developmental outcomes. Some original criteria used by the Rochester Project could not be examined in this study because we did not collect information on these constructs, such as the number of recent contacts with mental health professionals, whether parental occupations were skilled or unskilled. Risk-index scores were computed as the sum of the numbers of the risk factors in the subscales. These indices are categorized into three levels. The first level is the individual characteristics of a child, including peer reject, peer exclusion, social withdrawal, depression, hyperactivity, impulsivity, attention deficit, aggression, self-care inept, learning difficulty, chronic organic disorder, congenital disease, disability/ impairment diagnoses, developmental delay. Cronbach's alpha based on standardized items for the individual characteristics subscale is .813(9 items). The second level is the parenting process, including education below high school level, ethnic minority, parental psychopathology, parental disability diagnoses, out of / instable job, take care by 24 hours natty, cross-generation parenting, harsh discipline, inconsistent discipline, negative control. Cronbach's alpha based on standardized items for the parenting process subscale is .724(9 items). The third level is the environmental context, including live in a foster family, single parent, homeless, latchkey child, abnormal parenting, low SES, poverty, overcrowded family, domestic violence, alcohol addiction, drug abuse, reconviction / criminal, neighborhood with juvenile, delinquency, few cognitive stimulation / activities, low warmth / aloof. Cronbach's alpha based on standardized items for the environmental context subscale is .529(15 items). Outcome measurement The developmental outcome measurement in this study is focusing on the school readiness of children. We concerns about seven domains of children’s performance, including class and task performance, cognitive performance, language and communication, social functioning, externalizing problem, internalizing problem, positive emotionality. Cronbach's alpha based on standardized items is .814(7 items). Missing Data The range of data point missing on single variables across models was between 1.4% and 31.3%. The data are no balanced, as some individuals will have missing data on some of the responses. An estimation technique is required that and fit a multivariate response model that includes missing data. In order not to introduce bias 5 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun into the sample through listwise deletion of missing data, we used the multiple imputation technique described by Rubin(1987) & Schafer(1996). This has been identified as one of the best techniques for handling missing data (Allison, 2002). The basic idea of multiple imputations is to generate a series of data sets from the joint distribution of all variables. We used Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) estimation (Browne, 2003). This model includes all missing data values as parameters to be estimated. Where an individual had missing observations we estimated a mean and variance for each missing observation based on the overall covariance structure of the variables and the nonmissing values present for the individual. The distribution of every missing value in the data set was estimated. The MCMC method provides samples from these distributions. We replaced missing values in risk and outcome variables with the imputation values. Result Descriptive Analyses Descriptive analyses examined the association among the levels of risk factors, children’s outcome measures, sex and age. Risk-index scores were computed as the sum of the number of risk conditions experienced by each child. The intercorrelation of variables is in Table 1. Several finding are worth mentioning. First, no differences were found between any risk and outcome variables related to age. Second, more significant correlations among risk and outcome variables were found for being a girl than boys. Being a girl was associated with less individual characteristics risk than boys. For the other variables, no significance was found between boys and girls. Third, total risk is more consistently related to outcome variable than the individual characteristics risk, Parenting process risk and environmental context risk. Table 1 Intercorrelations among the study variables Age Age Individual risk .015 Parenting risk Contextual risk Risk sum .102 .117 .133 .060 -.067 .016 .448(***) .407(***) .271(**) .557(***) .150 .815(***) .298(***) Individual risk -.111 Parenting risk -.164 .315(**) Contextual risk .179 .429(***) .378(***) Risk sum -.009 .772(***) .658(***) .842(***) Child vulnerability -.091 .616(*(*) .272(*) .420(***) Child vulnerability .457(***) .584(***) Upper triangle matrix represents boys; lower triangle matrix represents girls. For boys N = 118; for girls N=78 *p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001; Variance in children’s vulnerability explained by individual, parenting and environmental risk A multiple regression analysis was performed in which the individual 6 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun characteristics risk, parenting process risk and environmental context risk were entered simultaneously into the regression equation. Multicollinearity was not detected. The standardized regression coefficients shown in Table 2 indicate that among the levels of risk factors, individual risk and environmental risk made significant and independent contributions to the explained variance in children’s developmental outcomes. The multiple regression analysis confirmed our expectation that individual, parenting and environmental risk would explain significant and unique portions of the variance in children’s vulnerability. The next section, in which the path model was tested, will shed more light to this matter. Table2 Linear regression analysis prediction predicting overall developmental vulnerability from individual risk , parenting risk, and environmental risk domains. Variable Beta T Individual .511 9.738*** Parenting .138 2.338* Contextual .292 4.696*** Note: N = 203; R2 = .714; F = 166.348; p = .000; Durbin-Watson = 1.751 *p<.05; ***p<.001 The overall goodness-of-fit of the path model depicted in Figure 1 was assessed using the LISREL 8 Statistical Software (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1989). Given that single indicators were used to represent all of the variables in the model, a structural equation model in which all variables were directly measured, with no assumed measurement error, was examined. The interrelations among the indicators depicted in Table 1 constituted the input matrix for the analyses The analysis of the initial model did not yield a significant fit, χ2(7df, N=203) =16.167, p= .024; GFI = .974; AGFI = .923; RMSEA = .081; NFI = 879, TLI = .835; IFI = .928; CFI = .923; RMR= .076, AIC = 44.167; NCP = .9.167. The deletion of the path between the age and child vulnerability from the initial model did not fit as well, χ2(4df, N=203) =11.688, p= .020; GFI = .978; AGFI = .917, RMSEA = .097, NFI = .908, TLI = .836, IFI = .938; CFI = .935, RMR= .077, AIC = 33.668; NCP = 7.668. The deletion of the path between the sex and child vulnerability from the initial model did not fit as well, χ2(4df, N=203) =6.787, p= .148; GFI = .987; AGFI = .951, RMSEA = .059, NFI = .944, TLI = .938, IFI = .976; CFI = .975, RMR= .091, AIC = 28.787; NCP = 2.787. The deletion of the path between the individual risk and parenting risk result in a significant reduction, χ2(1df, N=203) =.015, p= .902; GFI = 1.000; AGFI = 1.000, RMSEA = .000, NFI = 1.000, TLI = 1.053, IFI = 1.008; CFI = 1.000, RMR= .006, 7 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun AIC = 18.015; NCP = 0.000, indicated that this model, depicted in Figure 2, fit the data well. Parenting risk .16 .65 .17 Individual risk .18 Child vulnerability Individual risk .25 Contextual risk Figure 1 Final LISREL model of relations among individual characteristics, parenting processes, and the environmental context, and the child developmental outcomes. Result In sum, the analyses of the model empirically confirmed our hypotheses regarding the different level of risk may influence the children’s vulnerability. Several interesting phenomenon is worth of discussing. First, age and sex did not have significant impact on children’s developmental outcome in our data set. Second, parenting risk does not have significant correlation with individual risk. Third, the child vulnerability can be largely account for the individual risk, the parenting risk and contextual risk have not much influence on child vulnerability. So the individual risk may be a strong mediator of parenting risk and contextual risk. In the present study, a set of hypotheses model of the levels of risk factors influencing on children’s developmental vulnerability was tested. Given that the analyses were not based on longitudinal data, it is important to keeping mind the at the path model summarizing the results represents a pattern of covariation among variables and does not allow for causal explanations. Although the direction of the paths shown in the model was chosen on theoretical grounds, the existence of effects in the opposite direction or bidirectional effects cannot be excluded. In general, the pattern of association found in the path analytic procedures provided good support for Guralnick’s (1998) assertion that the vulnerability of young children is multiply determined by influences from three domains, individual risk, parenting risk and the contextual risk were each found to explain a significant and unique portion of the children’s vulnerability. Selecting a viable statistical method to describe the association between risk 8 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun factors and children’s vulnerability is important. Our study’s model confirmed that children who experience risk factors associated with poverty, discrimination, and family instability, and sociocultural risk measure include poverty, low levels of maternal IQ, minority ethnic status, large household size, single parent families, stressful life event and authoritarian child-rearing attitudes are more likely to have children showing delayed cognitive and language development, poor academic performance, conduct problems and emotional disorders. This is consistent with the results of Deater-Dechard et al.’s (1998)study that the number of risk condition in the child’s life was somewhat less predictive of child outcome than the model based on risk variables. Overall developmental level was predicted best by the individual risk variables considered together and was predicted worst by age. The level of prediction of the overall developmental level was indexed by the overall main effect test in our longitudinal analyses and by the squared multiple correlations in this study and in Deater-Dechard et al.’s study of social emotional problems. This is not surprising in that the tally of number of risk conditions experienced retain far less information about the child’s environment than the individual risk variable is able to predict outcomes more precisely when it contains more information about the underlying attribute it is representing. There are several limitations that need to be considered. First, the sample size in this study was only moderate, and the number of risk factors was relatively large. The additional information in the assessments allowed us to address questions about whether the risk index, the summary risk factors, or the entire block of individual risk variables were related to overall level of outcome. Second, the major purpose of this study was to contrast statistical methods, so multiple analyses were conducted for each outcome. Such overanalysis of data likely leads to results that are specific to this sample and do not generalize to the population. Only the consistent findings across approaches are likely to be reliable. Third, children with multiple risk factors or who experience high levels of exposure to risk are more likely to display lower levels of developmental vulnerabilities, this approach did not always provide good prediction of rates of change overtime, and interpretation of regression coefficients was hampered by moderate levels of correlations among predictors. References Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513-531. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979) The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Havard Vniversity Press. Browne, W. (2003). Multivariate normal response models and missing data. In 9 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun MCMC estimation in MLwin (pp. 207-227). London: Institute of Education. Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (1995). Perspectives on developmental psychology. In D. Cichetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (pp. 3-20). New York: Wiley-Interscience. Cowen, E. L., Wyman, P. A. Work, W. C., & Parker, G. R. (1990). The Rochester Child Resilience Project: Overview of summary of first year finding. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 193-212. Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Gates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1998). Multiple risk factors in the development of externalizing behavior problems: Group and individual differences. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 469-493. Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Cook, T., Eccles, G. H., & Sameroff, A. J. (1999). Urban families and adolescent success. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Garmezy, N. (1978). Attentional processes in adult schizophrenia and in children at risk. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 14, 3-34. Guralnick, M. J. (1998). Effectiveness of early intervention for vulnerable children : A developmental perspective. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102, 319-45. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8 User’s guide Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software. Lengua, L. J., (2002).The Contribution of emotionality and Self-regulation to the understanding of children’s response to multiple risk. Child Development, 93(1), 144-161. Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for non-response in surveys. New York: Wiley. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,57, 316-331. Sameroff, A. J., and Fiese, B. H. (2000). Transactional regulation and early intervention. In S. J. Meisels & J. P. Shonkoff (Eds.), Handbok of early childhood intervention (pp. 119-149). New York: Cambridge University Press. Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., Baldwin, A., & Baldwin, C. (1993). Stability of intelligence from preschool to adolescence: The influence of social and family risk factor. Child Development, 64, 80-97. Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., Barocas, R., Zax, M., & Greenspan, S. (1987). Intelligence quotient scores of 4-year-old children: Social-environmental risk factors. Pediatrics, 79, 343-350. Schafer, J. L. (1996). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. LondonL Chapman & Hall. Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17-29. 10 ECER2007/1/21 Sung, Ming-Jiun Thurman, A. K. (1997). Systems, ecologies, and the context of early intervention. In S. K. Thurman, J. R. Cornwell, & S. R. Gottwald (Eds.), Contexts of early intervention: Systems and setting (pp. 3-17). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Werner, E. E.& Smith R.S.(1992).Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to Adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. 11