Page 1 Gender Patterns in School Performance – Comparative Evidence Draft paper for presentation at a seminar organized by the Equal Opportunities Commission, Hong Kong SAR, November 2001 Tony Gallagher, Graduate School of Education, Queen’s University Belfast am.gallagher@qub.ac.uk In the United Kingdom free and compulsory post-primary education was established at the end of the Second World War. Although the UK is a single political unit, different education arrangements operated in England and Wales, Northern Ireland and Scotland. However, despite this, the form of post-primary education was largely the same in each place. That is, primary education operated up to age 11 years at which point pupils could take tests. On the basis of their test results a proportion of the pupils were selected to attend academic grammar schools while the rest went to secondary schools. In the 1950s it was common practice to treat boys and girls as separate populations for the assignment of transfer test grades. The rationale for this was that girls tended to achieve better results on these tests as age 11 years, but it was argued that this higher attainment was largely due to the fact that girls matured at a faster rate than boys. Once all pupils had gone through the point of maturity then, it was argued, boys’ educational attainments ‘caught up’ with that of the girls so that by age 16 years both groups were achieving their ‘normal’ levels of achievement. It was further argued that if allowance was not taken of the differing rates of maturity of boys and girls at age 11 years, then this would unfairly disadvantage the boys in the assignment of transfer grades and restrict their opportunity to attend grammar schools. Page 2 In England, Wales and Scotland the selective arrangements of grammar and secondary schools were largely abandoned in the 1960s and 1970s in favour of comprehensive schools which took in pupils of all abilities. Thus, the issue of gender differences at age 11 years became moot as no selective decision was taken at that age. In Northern Ireland, however, the practice of academic selection continued and, in fact, continues to this day although it has been subject to a major review and significant changes have been proposed. In addition, until the 1980s Northern Ireland also continued the practice of treating boys and girls as separate populations on the same basis that they had differing rates of maturity. In the 1980s the Northern Ireland Council for Educational Research (NICER) initiated a major research programme into the selective system of education1. NICER’s main interest lay in the dynamics of the system as a whole and, in particular, the way in which transfer test performance and later educational performance was mediated by social class. However, the data collected by several of the NICER studies suggested that by age 16 years there was no evidence that boys and girls of the same transfer grade had equal levels of academic performance. In fact, the evidence suggested that the performance of girls was higher than that of boys, thus casting doubt on a central justification for treating them as separate populations for the assignment of transfer grades. Although the government of the day showed no interest in pursuing the implications of this research, the Equal Opportunities Commission in Northern Ireland decided to support as court-case brought by the parents of some girls who had taken the transfer tests2. These parents claimed that the practice of treating boys and girls as separate populations for the assignment of transfer grades discriminated against girls as they had to achieve higher results than boys in order to gain the same transfer grade. The NICER evidence had 1 Gallagher, AM (1988) Transfer Pupils at 16. Belfast: Northern Ireland Council for Educational Research; Sutherland, AE and Gallagher (1987) Pupils in the Border Band. Belfast: Northern Ireland Council for Educational Research; Sutherland, AE and Gallagher, AM (1986) Transfer and the Upper Primary School. Belfast: Northern Ireland Council for Educational Research; Teare, S and Sutherland, AE (1988) At Sixes and Sevens. Belfast: Northern Ireland Council for Educational Research; Wilson, JA (1986) Transfer and the Structure of Secondary Education. Belfast: Northern Ireland Council for Educational Research. 2 Gallagher, AM (1989) The relationship between research and policy: an example from Northern Ireland. The Psychologist: Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 2, 62-63. Page 3 removed the main basis on which the Department of Education had attempted to justify this practice and, perhaps not surprisingly, the Department of Education lost the legal battle. The practice of treating boys and girls as separate populations in the award of transfer grades was deemed to be illegal and the Department of Education was told to cease the practice. From that point on all pupils were treated as a single population for the assignment of transfer grades. In the period before the court case boys had comprised a majority of the pupils given the top grade in the transfer tests. In the years immediately following the court decision the situation changed so that girls formed the majority of pupils given the top grade. However, from the early 1990s, and for reasons unconnected to the gender issue, the type of tests used to assign grades was changed. Up to then the transfer tests were verbalreasoning-type tests, with some items included to assess ability in mathematics and English. This was changed to the use of attainment tests, where each test contained three sections with questions on English, mathematics and science – for the present this remains the basis for the tests. In the years that followed there was no consistent relationship between gender and transfer grade. Thus, in some years girls comprised a higher proportion of the pupils given the top grade, in other years boys comprised the majority of these pupils, while in some years an even pattern was found. However, it is perhaps noteworthy that this inconsistency did not so much spark a debate about the relationship between gender and transfer test performance3, but rather heightened concerns about the reliability of the tests used to assign transfer grades and the appropriateness of a ‘high-stakes’ test for 11 year old children. I will return to this issue later in the paper but for the moment I would like to broaden the discussion by looking at patterns in attainment data in Northern Ireland and England, before going on to illustrate some common themes in attainment data in other countries. Although it does potentially raise a question on what a ‘fair’ distribution of grades by gender ought to look like. 3 Page 4 Figures 1 and 2 shows the pattern of qualifications of school leavers in Northern Ireland from 1963 to 19994. Figure 1 shows the proportion of school leavers with the equivalent of five good GCSE qualifications or better. Figure 2 shows the proportion of school leavers with two or more A Levels, the minimum criterion normally required to gain entry to higher education. Figure 1: leavers with %5+ GCSEs A*-C, 1963 to 1999 70 60 50 40 30 20 Boys 10 Girls 4 1998/99 1995/96 1992/93 1989/90 1986/87 1981/82 1976/77 1973/74 1970/71 1967/68 1964/65 0 The graphs are based on data derived from Statistical Bulletins produced by the Department of Education in Northern Ireland and earlier reports from the Ministry of Education: current and recent versions of these statistical bulletins are available at the Department’s website www.deni.gov Page 5 Figure 2: leavers with 2+ A Levels 1963 to 1999 50 40 30 20 10 Boys 1997/98 1994/95 1991/92 1988/89 1985/86 1979/80 1975/76 1972/73 1969/70 1966/67 1963/64 0 Girls Both figures show a steadily increasing level of qualifications among school leavers over time (the blip on both graphs was caused by the raising of the school leaving age). Both figures also show that a higher proportion of girls than boys achieved both these criteria from the mid-1970s onwards, and that the extent of the achievement gap between girls and boys has steadily widened over the years since. Page 6 Figure 3: Grammar leavers 5+ GCSEs A*-C 100 95 90 85 1998/99 1999/00 1999/00 1997/98 1996/97 1995/96 Girls 1998/99 Boys 1994/95 1993/94 1992/93 1991/92 1990/91 1987/88 1986/87 80 All Figure 4: Secondary leavers 5+ GCSEs A*-C 50 40 30 20 Boys Girls 1997/98 1996/97 1995/96 1994/95 1993/94 1992/93 1991/92 1990/91 1987/88 1986/87 10 All Figures 3 and 4 show the gender patterns for school leavers from grammar and secondary schools separately – these data were only available from 1986 onwards. From figure 4 we can see that the gender gap in favour of girls in secondary schools has widened over time, Page 7 while that for leavers from grammar schools has narrowed. Throughout this period there was a slightly higher proportion of girls among school leavers from grammar schools, but, as the table below shows, this did not increase over the period even though the total number of leavers coming from grammar schools did increase: Boys 1986/87 grammar Girls % Boys % Girls % Boys % Girls All 3,805 4,358 28 33 47 53 100 secondary 9,708 8,927 72 67 52 48 100 all 13,513 13,285 100 100 50 50 100 4,651 5,034 36 41 48 52 100 secondary 8,245 7,149 64 59 54 46 100 all 12,896 12,183 100 100 51 49 100 1999/00 grammar Thus far the data point to an increasing gender gap in favour of girls that seems to be increasing over time. That this is only part of the picture is revealed by the next two figures5. Figure 5 shows data for individual grammar schools with the proportion of boys and girls in Year 12 (16 year olds) who achieved five or more good GCSEs. Figure 6 shows the equivalent data for boys and girls in the Year 12 of secondary schools. The overall pattern revealed by these data is consistent with the earlier data, that is, a higher proportion of girls tend to meet this criterion across all the schools as compared with boys, and this is true for grammar and secondary schools. However, figures 5 and 6 also show a high degree of overlap, that is, while girls generally get higher achievement than boys, this is not so for all girls and for all girls. In other words, there are other factors at play in explaining these patterns of performance as opposed simply to gender issues. 5 The data on these graphs are derived from the School Performance Tables produced by the Department of Education: these are available on the Department’s website at www.deni.gov.uk Page 8 Figure 5: grammar schools, 5+ good GCSEs 100 90 5acboys 5acgirls 80 73 65 57 49 41 33 25 17 9 1 70 Figure 6: secondary schools 5+ good GCSEs 100 80 60 5acboys 5acgirls 40 20 155 141 127 113 99 85 71 57 43 29 15 1 0 Another dimension of the complexity that exists within these data is illustrated by recent information on A Level performance among boys and girls in England6. In most, but not all, subjects the data show a higher percentage of girls achieving success in comparison with boys, but the implications of this is related not only to success rate but also to participation rates. Take, for example, the patterns for some specific subjects: 6 DFES, 2001: National Statistics: First Release SFR 41/2001 Page 9 English Mathematics Physics French Boys aged 17-18 Candidates 22,628 33,543 21,991 4,637 Success rate 93 87 89 93 Girls aged 17-18 Candidates 53,077 20,024 5,988 10,651 Success rate 94 91 92 91 On the basis of the success rates for these individual subjects we can see that girls do better at English, Mathematics and Physics, while boys do better at French. However, quite apart from the fact that the difference in success rates is quite small, this immediate judgment takes no account whatsoever of the differing level of participation in the subjects. Thus, in Physics, for example, although girls do better overall, they comprised only 22 per cent of those who achieved success in Physics. In the same way, although boys did better overall in French, they comprised only 31 per cent of those who succeeded in French. It would seem, therefore, that one could conclude that either boys or girls did better on Physics or French, depending on the basis upon which this judgment is made: if the basis of judgment is overall success rate then girls do better at Physics and boys do better at French. On the other hand, if the basis of judgment is on the overall proportion who succeed then boys do better at Physics and girls do better at French. Data from the United States on gender patterns in education also illustrate the complex nature of the issues involved7. Thus, for example, in the 1970s there were large gaps in the educational performance of boys and girls, but these had largely disappeared by the 1990s: in particular, the proportion of young women attending college after high school graduation was on a par with that of young men, and they were as likely to have completed a bachelor’s degree. The US National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) has assessed the knowledge of 9-, 13- and 17-year olds in maths and science since the early 1970s. For maths there had been a gap in favour of girls for the young Page 10 cohorts in the 1970s, and in favour of boys for the oldest cohort in the 1970s. Over time this difference has reduced. For science, on the other hand, there is evidence of a gender gap in favour of boys at age 13 years and, more particularly, at age 17 years. This science pattern for the older cohorts is mirrored also in TIMSS data which suggests that older boys tend to out-perform girls on science: for example, the third international TIMSS study found that 16 year old boys outperformed girls in 16 of the 38 countries in the study, and that this difference was most apparent among high-performing students. In the US similar patterns are also found in achievement tests linked to the SAT and College Board AP tests where either boys take more of these tests, or a higher proportion score above the success threshold, or both. The NCES reports go on to examine some of the factors that appear to be linked to these differences. Although there have been changes in attitudes towards maths and science (whereas in the past boys at all ages tended to be more positive towards both subjects) there is continuing evidence that positive attitudes are linked to participation in maths and science activities, and that boys are more likely to have these opportunities: …boys were more likely than girls to have talked to a scientist or attended a computer club, or to have used a computer for a significant amount of time during the week, or to have a computer or microscope at home. By 10th grade, boys were more likely than girls to have conducted their own science experiments, to have a computer or calculator at home, or to have used a computer more often during the week. In contrast girls were spending more time on mathematics or science homework. (NCES 1997: 10). The NCES report also shows that there are divergent career expectations and aspirations, even among very young children, with boys much more likely to aspire to be scientists or engineers. In the past the differing performance levels of US boys and girls was linked to differing patterns of participation in advanced maths and science subjects. By the 1990s this difference in participation had largely disappeared, except for Physics which retained a higher level of participation by boys. In addition, although there had been marked increases in the College participation rates of young women, there remained significant 7 NCES (1995) The education progress of women: findings from the Condition of Education 1995. Washington DC: US Department of Education; NCES (1997) Women in Mathematics and Science: findings from the Condition of Education 1997. Washington DC: US Department of Education. Page 11 differences between men and women in the fields of study they took while in College. NCES (1997), for example, show the probable fields of study of College freshmen in 1996 as: Source: NCES 1997 Arts and humanities Biology Business Education Engineering Physical sciences Professional Social sciences Technical Computer sciences Other Undecided Men 9 7 18 6 15 3 10 6 4 4 11 7 Women 11 7 14 14 3 2 20 12 1 1 7 9 Remaining differences in participation rates have implications for future employment prospects. In fact, the NCES report shows how, despite the rapid improvements in educational participation and performance by women over time, they remain behind in terms of employment status and remuneration. Overall, NCES (1997) found that the average 1996 median starting salaries of College graduates was $26,122 for men and $21,990 for women. This difference was found also for College graduates in the natural sciences and maths where the median starting salary for men was $21,833 while that for women was $20,508. These US patterns mirror almost exactly the results of a series of studies carried out in Northern Ireland. In one set of studies the A Level subject choices and performance of all students in a sample of grammar schools were identified in 1985 and followed-up ten years later in 1995. One of the more striking changes over that period was that the proportion of girls taking Chemistry A Level had increased from a third to a half of the total: in contrast, the proportion of girls taking Physics A level had stayed stable over the Page 12 period at a little over a quarter8. Qualitative data collected during focus groups of students suggested that the main reason for this change lay in the career aspirations and expectations of the young women. Thus, the main reason offered by women students for taking Chemistry was that they sought entry to higher education courses in medical and related areas. By the same token, their view was that Physics was needed only for entry to engineering and related areas, and they were generally uninterested in this career route. What was also interesting, however, was that change in participation was related not only to career aspirations, but also appeared to have an impact on gendered perceptions of specific subjects: thus, the young women we talked to expressed no stereotypes or perceived constraints with regard to Chemistry, but did express a view that Physics was a subject that boys were ‘naturally’ better at than girls. Another series of studies had examined patterns of participation in higher education by surveying all young people from Northern Ireland who entered higher education in 1991. Since then follow-up surveys have examined aspects of their higher education experience and the early years of employment. Although there has been an increasing pattern of participation among young women, who now comprise a majority of those entering higher education, there remains significant gender differences in the types of courses taken in university and, more particularly, in the starting salaries after graduation. A further important aspect of this issue is provided by Murphy and Elwood9 (1998) who went beyond patterns of participation to examine patterns of choice and the role played by gender in mediating choice and interests. Their analysis pointed to a number of factors which influenced performance patterns. They found that familiarity with the context of a task could often influence performance in the task itself. This links to the earlier point regarding the greater familiarity of boys with scientific equipment: Murphy and Elwood show how boys could out-perform girls on a scientific task if the participants were left to select the equipment for carrying out the task, but that the differences disappeared when 8 Gallagher, AM, McEwen, A and Knipe, D (1996) Girls and A Level Science 1985 to 1995. Belfast: Equal Opportunities Commission. Page 13 they were asked to use specific equipment in order to carry out the task. Similarly, student confidence can mediate successful completion of a task, and confidence is linked to familiarity with the parameters within which a task is located. Thus, for example, they show how boys and girls can react to the same content differently irrespective of the task involved: Questions that involved content related to health, reproduction, nutrition and domestic situations were generally found to show girls performing at a higher level than boys across the ages. It was also the case that more girls than boys would attempt such questions. Hence the gender gap in performance arose because of the increased confidence of girls combined with the lower response rate of boys. In questions where the content was more overtly ‘masculine’ the converse occurred. Typical ‘masculine’ contents included cars, building sites, submarines, machinery, etc. Talking to students revealed that they had definite views about content areas where they expected to be competent and content areas where they anticipated failure. (Murphy and Elwood, 1998: 102). The more general point here is that task familiarity is closely related to the socialization undergone by young children outside school and the opportunities provided by play. When children are faced with tasks in school with which they are familiar, they tackle these with confidence, but the converse is also true in that they may withdraw when faced with tasks with which they lack experience. For Murphy and Elwood, however, this pattern of behaviour may go unremarked by teachers who themselves share gendered assumptions about appropriate and inappropriate contexts of tasks. On a broader level, Murphy and Elwood suggest that attainment may often be related to perceived relevance rather than competence. Other factors which influenced gendered performance included differences between presentation styles of girls and boys, and the link between this and preferred presentation styles within particular subject areas. Thus, for example, boys tended to be prefer a presentation style that was factual, to-the-point and reflected certainty. By contrast girls preferred a presentation style that was reflective, critical and circumspect, and which Murphy, P and Elwood, J (1998) Gendered experiences, choices and achievement – exploring the links. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 2(2), 95-118. See also Epstein, D, Elwood, J, Hey, V and Maw, J (Eds) (1998) Failing Boys? Issues in gender and achievement. England: Open University Press. 9 Page 14 recognized the uncertainties that lay behind a conclusion. The approach favoured by boys was that also favoured by science and technology teachers, while the approach favoured by girls was that more usually favoured by humanities and arts teachers. Murphy and Elwood also suggest that boys are often more prepared to be risk-takers and to offer answers to questions even when they are not sure they are correct, whereas girls are more likely to remain circumspect when they are unsure. At times, they suggest, teachers may interpret these preferred performance styles as reflecting different dimensions of ability: thus, the higher performance of girls is sometimes attributed to their diligence rather than their ability. All of this reflects the importance of contextual dimensions of teaching, learning and performance, and reminds us that the attributions of meaning in these contexts can vary. One of the dimensions along which meaning can vary is related to gender. Much of this discussion echoes earlier work, but the context within which it was written has changed. Murphy and Elwood were writing at a time in Britain when there had been an extraordinary ‘moral panic’ about gender patterns of performance which had led some to speculate on ‘what had gone wrong for the boys’. A simplistic argument had surfaced that boys’ educational performance had somehow or other collapsed while the performance of girls was suddenly, and by inference inappropriately, ahead. Some suggested that this turnaround had occurred due to changes in the curriculum or examinations, or because of a broader crisis of masculinity, or because of the feminization of the teaching workforce. Contrast this with the context of an earlier study which identified many of the same themes highlighted by the Murphy and Elwood. In this earlier study Hanson10 (1992) was examining the mix of factors that had led to girls consistently achieving lower attainments than boys in mathematics and argued that removing these constraints was not simply a priority on the grounds of equity, but also a priority on the grounds of economic imperative as a contribution to increasing the size of the country’s mathematically and 10 Hanson, K (1992) Teaching mathematics effectively and equitably to females. US: Education development Center. Available on: http//:eric-web.tc.Columbia.edu/monographs/ti17_index.html Page 15 technologically skilled work force. Central to Hanson’s analysis is the role of sex stereotypes in defining appropriate and inappropriate orientations towards disciplines and skills and, more generally, the way gender mediates the attribution of meaning in particular contexts: For those women who attempt to enter into the discourse as equals by adopting a male discourse model, the response is no better. Women are often penalized for attempting to participate in the ‘male domain’. Often the perception of behaviour is confused with actual behaviour, based on sex-role stereotypes. While a male might be called ambitious, assertive, and independent, a woman displaying the same behaviour is often labeled aggressive, pushy, and argumentative. Studies continue to show that when woman and men exhibit the same behaviour, that behaviour is devalued for women. (Hanson, 1992). It was due to longer-standing constraints on the progress of women that the United States enacted Title IX in 1972 to prohibit institutions that receive federal funding from practicing sex discrimination in educational programmes or activities. Other legislation operates to promote gender equity, including the 1974 Women’s Educational Equity Act which provides for financial and technical support to local efforts to remove barriers for women in all areas of education, Title IV of the 1964 Civil Rights Act, and 1976 amendments to the 1963 Vocational Education Act which requires affirmative action towards the elimination of sex bias, stereotyping and discrimination in vocational education. Hanson’s recommendations are largely focused at the role parents, teachers and schools can play in promoting practices which work against the socialization of young people into gender roles and modes of learning which actually act to constrain participation. In other words, the main thrust of this analysis has been to highlight the sociocultural factors which influence educational performance. Indeed, the patterns of change, which have been so evident over recent years, highlight the social aspect of these issues: if biology was the predominant factor then the patterns should have been much more stable. In fact, most analyses of educational performance and gender emphasise the role of social and cultural factors, as opposed to biological factors11. 11 See also articles in Husen, T, Postlethwaite, TN, Clarke, BR and Neave, G (Eds) (1995) The Complete Encyclopedia CD-ROM (The International Encyclopedia of Education and The Encyclopedia of Higher Education). Elsevier, especially: Baker, CD: Analysis of School Texts and Page 16 Just to illustrate aspects of these changes, consider some of the consequences of Title IX in the United States. First, there had a marked increase in the participation and completion rates of young women in college education: in 1973 43 per cent of female high school graduates aged 16-24 were enrolled in college, but this figure had increased to 63 per cent in 1994. Second, whereas in 1972 only 9 per cent of medical degrees went to women, but by 1994 this had increased to 38 per cent; in dentistry the proportion of women with medical degrees increased from 1 to 38 per cent over the same period; and the proportion of women with law degrees increased from 7 to 43 per cent. In addition, whereas 25 per cent of doctoral degrees went to women in 1977, this had increased to 44 per cent in 1993/94. And third, we can see change sin other areas of education life as well, such as participation in athletics. In 1997 some 100,000 women participated in US intercollegiate athletics, a fourfold increase form 1971; in 1972 women comprised 15 per cent of college student athletes, but by 1995 this had increased to 37 per cent; between 1971 and 1995 there has been an eight-fold increase in the participation of young women in high school athletics; and all of this perhaps helps to explain by US women won a record 19 gold medals in the 1996 Summer Olympic Games. This represents an entirely different perspective from which to analyse the recent patterns that show such improvement in the academic performance of women across so many domains. Rather then representing some dramatic collapse among boys, these data should be seen as the culmination of the efforts towards equity which have been pursued over recent decades. It is in this context, for example, that the gender patterns in Northern Ireland identified at the beginning of this paper have been analysed. Thus, it has been pointed out that, far from demonstrating a regressive pattern for boys, the Northern Ireland data show that the measured performance levels of all pupils have increased over time. The gender difference has arisen because the rate of increase among girls has been Gender; Farrell, JP: Social Equality and Educational Expansion in Developing Nations; Keeves, JP and Kotte D: Sex differences and educational outcomes; Martin, CL and Levy, G: Gender Roles; Sadker, D and Sadker, M: Sex Equity: Assumptions and Strategies; Stemmler, M and Petersen, AC: Sex Differences in Behavior and Behavioral Development; Weiner, G: Gender and Racial Differences Among Students in Classrooms. Page 17 higher in comparison with that of boys. This has led me to conclude that a series of factors have influenced these changes12. The rapid rate in increase in the performance of girls seems to me to be best explained by the consequences of the equality environment so assiduously promoted since the 1970s. The ten year study of A Level students in grammar schools showed compelling evidence of the extent to which the career expectations and aspirations of girls influenced the curriculum areas within which they felt able to operate. By contrast, some of the factors which have helped to restrain the academic performance of boys remain unaffected by policy initiatives. Indeed, the perception of despair, which sees education as worthless since economic and social prospects remain so poor, remains a factor for pupils in some of the inner city areas of Belfast13 and may even be exacerbated by the effects of the selective system which. Intentionally or not, labels two-thirds of pupils as failures at the age of 11 years14. The end of traditional engineering and factory work into which boys could obtain apprenticeships through family and friendship networks has played a role here as well. This analysis suggested that some of the factors affecting the performance of boys and girls operate independently. In other words, part of the increase in performance levels of boys and girls is probably best explained by the efforts of the education system in general, and schools and teachers in particular, to promote higher standards. In addition, the performance level of girls has been boosted by the removal of constraints which, in the past, have held them back, as a consequence of the equality policies pursued over recent years. In this context boys may well constitute an increasing proportion of the pupils who still face particular and severe problems within education, but the total number of pupils in this situation is declining. And while some of the measures necessary to tackle this problem will involve targeted measures to re-engage the interest of boys in schooling, some of the problem relates to the conditions of inner-city poverty and disadvantage from which both boys and girls suffer. In other words, the problem is due to 12 Gallagher, AM (1997) Educational achievement and gender: a review of research evidence on the apparent underachievement of boys. Research Report No.6. Bangor: Department of Education for Northern Ireland. 13 Gallagher, AM, Shuttleworth, I and Gray, C (1997) Educational Achievement in Northern Ireland: Patterns and Prospects. Research Monograph 4. Belfast: Northern Ireland Economic Council; Gallagher, AM, Shuttleworth, I and Gray, C (1999) Improving Schools in Northern Ireland. Research Monograph. Belfast: Northern Ireland Economic Council Page 18 a combination of social, economic and educational circumstances, rather than biological circumstances, and the solutions must be found in these contexts as well. In the opening section of this paper it was mentioned that the variable gender patterns in transfer test performance, once combined papers with questions in Mathematics, English and Science were used, have provoked concern more about the reliability of the tests rather than any specific concern about the gender patterns per se. There has always been controversy about the selective system and, in particular, the use of such a ‘high stakes’ instrument for pupils who are so young. In response to this controversy the present government instituted a major research project into the effects of the selective system of secondary education which reported in 200015. The report was highly critical of the consequences of the selective arrangements and the way it exacerbated the mediating role of social background on educational performance. Following the publication of the research the Minister of Education established a Review Body on Post Primary Education to consult on the future of the system and bring forward recommendations for the future. Interestingly, in the research and the subsequent debates, the issue of gender and performance has played a much less substantial role that that of social background and performance. In November 2001 the Review Body16 published its report which recommended the end of academic selection and its replacement by a system of formative assessment, parental choice and the creation of collaborative networks of post primary schools. One element behind these recommendations is that many of the problems of a selective system, particularly one that selects so early, lies in the very fact of institutionalizing a high stakes selective decision in the first place. On that basis, key parts of the problem can be removed simply by removing the need to select in this way. Pupils will still move from primary to post-primary school at age 11 years, but it is recommended that, in future, this should be a low stakes decision, amenable to change later in a young person’s educational career. 14 Gallagher, T and Smith, A (2000) The effects of selective system of secondary education in Northern Ireland: main report. Bangor: Department of Education (available at www.deni.gov.uk ) 15 Gallagher, T and Smith, A (2000) op. cit. Page 19 16 Burns Report (2001) Education for the 21 st Century: Report of the Review Body on Post Primary Education. Bangor: Department of Education (available at www.pprbsni.gov.uk and www.deni.gov.uk ).