DCM: Advanced Topics Klaas Enno Stephan Neural population activity 0.4 0.3 0.2 u2 0.1 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0.6 Translational Neuromodeling Unit (TNU) Institute for Biomedical Engineering, University of Zurich & ETH Zurich 0.4 u1 x3 0.2 0 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 x1 Laboratory for Social & Neural Systems Research (SNS), University of Zurich x2 3 fMRI signal change (%) 2 1 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 4 3 Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London A dt dx m i 1 n ui B (i) j 1 x jD ( j) x Cu 2 1 0 -1 3 2 1 0 Methods & models for fMRI data analysis November 2012 Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Extended DCM for fMRI: nonlinear, two-state, stochastic • Embedding computational models in DCMs • Integrating tractography and DCM • Applications of DCM to clinical questions Dynamic Causal Modeling (DCM) Hemodynamic forward model: neural activityBOLD Electromagnetic forward model: neural activityEEG MEG LFP Neural state equation: dx F ( x , u, ) dt fMRI simple neuronal model complicated forward model EEG/MEG complicated neuronal model simple forward model inputs Generative models & model selection • any DCM = a particular generative model of how the data (may) have been caused • modelling = comparing competing hypotheses about the mechanisms underlying observed data a priori definition of hypothesis set (model space) is crucial determine the most plausible hypothesis (model), given the data • model selection model validation! model validation requires external criteria (external to the measured data) Model comparison and selection Given competing hypotheses on structure & functional mechanisms of a system, which model is the best? Which model represents the best balance between model fit and model complexity? For which model m does p(y|m) become maximal? Pitt & Miyung (2002) TICS Bayesian model selection (BMS) Model evidence: p ( y | , m ) p ( | m ) d accounts for both accuracy and complexity of the model allows for inference about structure (generalisability) of the model p(y|m) p( y | m) Gharamani, 2004 y all possible datasets Various approximations, e.g.: - negative free energy, AIC, BIC McKay 1992, Neural Comput. Penny et al. 2004a, NeuroImage Approximations to the model evidence in DCM Maximizing log model evidence = Maximizing model evidence Logarithm is a monotonic function Log model evidence = balance between fit and complexity log p ( y | m ) accuracy ( m ) complexity ( m ) log p ( y | , m ) complexity ( m ) No. of parameters SPM2 & SPM5 offered 2 approximations: Akaike Information Criterion: Bayesian Information Criterion: No. of data points AIC log p ( y | , m ) p BIC log p ( y | , m ) p log N 2 Penny et al. 2004a, NeuroImage Penny 2012, NeuroImage The (negative) free energy approximation • Under Gaussian assumptions about the posterior (Laplace approximation): log p ( y | m ) log p ( y | , m ) K L q , p | m K L q , p | y , m F log p ( y | m ) K L q , p | y , m log p ( y | , m ) K L q , p | m accuracy com plexity The complexity term in F • In contrast to AIC & BIC, the complexity term of the negative free energy F accounts for parameter interdependencies. KL q ( ), p ( | m ) 1 2 ln C 1 2 ln C | y 1 2 |y T 1 C |y • The complexity term of F is higher – the more independent the prior parameters ( effective DFs) – the more dependent the posterior parameters – the more the posterior mean deviates from the prior mean • NB: Since SPM8, only F is used for model selection ! Bayes factors To compare two models, we could just compare their log evidences. But: the log evidence is just some number – not very intuitive! A more intuitive interpretation of model comparisons is made possible by Bayes factors: positive value, [0;[ B12 p ( y | m1 ) p( y | m2 ) Kass & Raftery classification: Kass & Raftery 1995, J. Am. Stat. Assoc. B12 p(m1|y) Evidence 1 to 3 50-75% weak 3 to 20 75-95% positive 20 to 150 95-99% strong 150 99% Very strong BMS in SPM8: an example attention M1 stim M1 M2 M3 M4 M3 stim PPC V1 attention V1 V5 PPC M2 M2 better than M1 BF 2966 F = 7.995 PPC attention stim V1 V5 M3 better than M2 BF 12 F = 2.450 V5 M4 attention PPC M4 better than M3 BF 23 F = 3.144 stim V1 V5 Fixed effects BMS at group level Group Bayes factor (GBF) for 1...K subjects: GBF ij BF (k ) ij k Average Bayes factor (ABF): A B Fij K BF (k ) ij k Problems: - blind with regard to group heterogeneity - sensitive to outliers Random effects BMS for heterogeneous groups Dirichlet parameters = “occurrences” of models in the population r ~ Dir ( r ; ) Dirichlet distribution of model probabilities r mk ~ p (mk | p ) mk ~ p (mk | p ) mk ~ p (mk | p ) m 1 ~ Mult ( m ;1, r ) y1 ~ p ( y1 | m 1 ) y1 ~ p ( y1 | m 1 ) y2 ~ p ( y2 | m2 ) y1 ~ p ( y1 | m 1 ) Multinomial distribution of model labels m Measured data y Model inversion by Variational Bayes (VB) or MCMC Stephan et al. 2009a, NeuroImage Penny et al. 2010, PLoS Comp. Biol. LD m2 MOG FG LD|LVF MOG FG LD|RVF MOG LD|LVF RVF stim. LD LD LG LVF stim. RVF LD|RVF stim. m2 Subjects MOG FG LD LG LG FG m1 LG LVF stim. m1 Data: Stephan et al. 2003, Science Models: Stephan et al. 2007, J. Neurosci. -35 -30 -25 -20 -15 -10 -5 Log model evidence differences 0 5 p(r >0.5 | y) = 0.997 1 5 4.5 4 p r1 r2 99 . 7 % m2 3.5 m1 p(r 1|y) 3 2.5 2 1.5 r1 84.3% r2 15.7% 0.5 0 0 1 11.8 2 2.2 1 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 r Stephan et al. 2009a, NeuroImage 0.6 1 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 nonlinear Model space partitioning: comparing model families log GBF 80 Summed log evidence (rel. to RBML) FFX linear 60 40 20 p(r >0.5 | y) = 0.986 1 0 5 CBMN CBMN(ε) RBMN RBMN(ε) CBML CBML(ε) RBML RBML(ε) RFX 12 4.5 10 4 m2 m1 alpha 8 3.5 p r1 r2 9 8 .6 % 6 3 p(r 1|y) 4 2 2.5 2 0 CBMN CBMN(ε) RBMN RBMN(ε) CBML CBML(ε) RBML RBML(ε) 1.5 1 16 14 12 m1 m2 4 8 1* k r2 2 6 .5 % r1 7 3 .5 % 0.5 0 0 10 alpha Model space partitioning 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 r k 1 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 1 6 4 8 2* k 2 k 5 0 nonlinear models linear models Stephan et al. 2009, NeuroImage Comparing model families – a second example • data from Leff et al. 2008, J. Neurosci • one driving input, one modulatory input • 26 = 64 possible modulations • 23 – 1 input patterns • 764 = 448 models • integrate out uncertainty about modulatory patterns and ask where auditory input enters Penny et al. 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol. Bayesian Model Averaging (BMA) • abandons dependence of parameter inference on a single model • uses the entire model space considered (or an optimal family of models) • computes average of each parameter, weighted by posterior model probabilities p n | y1 .. N p n | y n , m p m | y1 .. N m NB: p(m|y1..N) can be obtained by either FFX or RFX BMS • represents a particularly useful alternative – when none of the models (or model subspaces) considered clearly outperforms all others – when comparing groups for which the optimal model differs Penny et al. 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol. definition of model space inference on model structure or inference on model parameters? inference on individual models or model space partition? optimal model structure assumed to be identical across subjects? yes FFX BMS comparison of model families using FFX or RFX BMS inference on parameters of an optimal model or parameters of all models? optimal model structure assumed to be identical across subjects? yes no FFX BMS RFX BMS no RFX BMS Stephan et al. 2010, NeuroImage FFX analysis of parameter estimates (e.g. BPA) RFX analysis of parameter estimates (e.g. t-test, ANOVA) BMA Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Extended DCM for fMRI: nonlinear, two-state, stochastic • Embedding computational models in DCMs • Integrating tractography and DCM • Applications of DCM to clinical questions DCM10 in SPM8 • DCM10 was released as part of SPM8 in July 2010 (version 4010). • Introduced many new features, incl. two-state DCMs and stochastic DCMs • This led to various changes in model defaults, e.g. – inputs mean-centred – changes in coupling priors – self-connections estimated separately for each area • For details, see: www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/SPM8_Release_Notes_r4010.pdf • Further changes in version 4290 (released April 2011) to accommodate new developments and give users more choice (e.g., whether or not to meancentre inputs). The evolution of DCM in SPM • DCM is not one specific model, but a framework for Bayesian inversion of dynamic system models • The default implementation in SPM is evolving over time – improvements of numerical routines (e.g., for inversion) – change in priors to cover new variants (e.g., stochastic DCMs, endogenous DCMs etc.) To enable replication of your results, you should ideally state which SPM version (release number) you are using when publishing papers. In the next SPM version, the release number will be stored in the DCM.mat. y y BOLD y activity x2(t) λ hemodynamic model activity x3(t) activity x1(t) neuronal states x integration modulatory input u2(t) driving input u1(t) y t Neural state equation ( j) x ( A u j B ) x Cu A endogenous connectivity t The classical DCM: a deterministic, one-state, bilinear model modulation of connectivity direct inputs B ( j) C x x x u j x x u Factorial structure of model specification in DCM10 • Three dimensions of model specification: – bilinear vs. nonlinear – single-state vs. two-state (per region) – deterministic vs. stochastic • Specification via GUI. bilinear DCM non-linear DCM modulation driving input driving input modulation Two-dimensional Taylor series (around x0=0, u0=0): f f x f ( x , u ) f ( x 0 ,0 ) x u ux ... 2 ... dt x u xu x 2 dx Bilinear state equation: A dt dx (i) u i B x Cu i 1 m f f 2 2 2 Nonlinear state equation: A dt dx m uB i i 1 n (i) x j 1 j D ( j) x Cu Neural population activity 0.4 0.3 0.2 u2 0.1 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0.6 u1 0.4 x3 0.2 0 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 x1 x2 3 fMRI signal change (%) 2 1 0 Nonlinear dynamic causal model (DCM) 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 4 3 A dt dx m uB n (i) i i 1 j 1 ( j) x j D x Cu 2 1 0 -1 3 2 1 Stephan et al. 2008, NeuroImage 0 attention MAP = 1.25 0.10 0.8 0.7 PPC 0.6 0.26 0.5 0.39 1.25 stim 0.26 V1 0.13 0.46 0.50 V5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 -2 motion Stephan et al. 2008, NeuroImage -1 0 1 2 3 4 p ( D V 5 ,V 1 0 | y ) 99 . 1 % PPC 5 Two-state DCM Single-state DCM Two-state DCM input u E x1 E x1 x1 I x1 I x1 x x Cu ij ij exp( Aij uB ij ) x x Cu ij Aij uB ij 11 N 1 1N NN Marreiros et al. 2008, NeuroImage x1 x x N EE 11 IE 11 EE N1 0 11 EI 11 II 1N EE 0 NN EE 0 0 Extrinsic (between-region) coupling NN IE 0 0 EE NN II NN Intrinsic (within-region) coupling x1E I x1 x E xN xI N Estimates of hidden causes and states (Generalised filtering) Stochastic DCM inputs or causes - V2 1 dx dt 0.5 (A v u u jB j ( j) )x Cv (x) 0 -0.5 -1 0 200 (v) 400 600 800 1000 hidden states - neuronal 0.1 excitatory signal 0.05 • all states are represented in generalised coordinates of motion • random state fluctuations w(x) account for endogenous fluctuations, have unknown precision and smoothness two hyperparameters • fluctuations w(v) induce uncertainty about how inputs influence neuronal activity • can be fitted to resting state data 1200 0 -0.05 -0.1 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 hidden states - hemodynamic 1.3 flow volume dHb 1.2 1.1 1 0.9 0.8 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 predicted BOLD signal 2 observed predicted 1 0 -1 -2 Li et al. 2011, NeuroImage -3 0 200 400 600 time (seconds) 800 1000 1200 Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Extended DCM for fMRI: nonlinear, two-state, stochastic • Embedding computational models in DCMs • Integrating tractography and DCM • Applications of DCM to clinical questions Prediction errors drive synaptic plasticity PE(t) x3 R x1 a0 x2 McLaren 1989 t wt w0 P E k k 1 t w t a 0 P E ( ) d 0 synaptic plasticity during learning = f (prediction error) Learning of dynamic audio-visual associations 1 Conditioning Stimulus CS1 Target Stimulus CS2 0.8 or p(face) or CS 0 Response TS 200 400 600 Time (ms) 800 0.6 0.4 0.2 2000 ± 650 0 0 200 400 600 trial den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci. 800 1000 Hierarchical Bayesian learning model prior on volatility volatility p k 1 k vt-1 p v t 1 | v t , k ~ N v t , exp( k ) vt probabilistic association rt rt+1 observed events ut ut+1 Behrens et al. 2007, Nat. Neurosci. p rt 1 | rt , v t ~ Dir rt , exp( v t ) Explaining RTs by different learning models Reaction times 1 True Bayes Vol HMM fixed HMM learn RW 450 0.8 430 p(F) RT (ms) 440 420 0.6 0.4 410 400 390 0.2 0.1 0.3 0.5 0.7 0.9 p(outcome) 0 400 0.7 • Rescorla-Wagner • Hidden Markov models (2 variants) 520 560 600 Bayesian model selection: 0.6 Exceedance prob. • hierarchical Bayesian learner 480 Trial 5 alternative learning models: • categorical probabilities 440 hierarchical Bayesian model performs best 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Categorical model den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci. Bayesian learner HMM (fixed) HMM (learn) RescorlaWagner Stimulus-independent prediction error Putamen Premotor cortex p < 0.05 (cluster-level wholebrain corrected) 0 -0.5 0 -0.5 -1 -1.5 -2 BOLD resp. (a.u.) BOLD resp. (a.u.) p < 0.05 (SVC) -1 -1.5 p(F) p(H) den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci . -2 p(F) p(H) Prediction error (PE) activity in the putamen PE during active sensory learning PE during incidental sensory learning p < 0.05 (SVC) PE during reinforcement learning O'Doherty et al. 2004, Science den Ouden et al. 2009, Cerebral Cortex PE = “teaching signal” for synaptic plasticity during learning Could the putamen be regulating trial-by-trial changes of task-relevant connections? Hierarchical Bayesian learning model Prediction errors control plasticity during adaptive cognition • Influence of visual areas on premotor cortex: PUT – stronger for surprising stimuli – weaker for expected stimuli PMd PPA den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci . p = 0.017 p = 0.010 FFA ongoing pharmacological and genetic studies Hierarchical variational Bayesian learning p 1 k 1 volatility association p x 3 | x 3 , ~ N x 3 , exp( ) k k x3 x3 x k 1 2 x k 1 events in the world sensory stimuli u k 1 k 1 p x 2 | x 2 , x3 k k 2 u k 1 k ~ N x 2 , exp( x 3 ) k 1 k k p x1 | x 2 ~ Bernoulli S x 2 k p u | x1 k x1 x1 k 1 k k k k ~ M oG x1 0, x1 1 k k Mean-field decomposition p u , x , | u k k 1.. k 1 q x1 q x 2 q x 3 q k k k Mathys et al. (2011), Front. Hum. Neurosci. Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Extended DCM for fMRI: nonlinear, two-state, stochastic • Embedding computational models in DCMs • Integrating tractography and DCM • Applications of DCM to clinical questions Diffusion-weighted imaging Parker & Alexander, 2005, Phil. Trans. B 1.6 Integration of tractography and DCM 1.4 1.2 1 R1 R2 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 -2 -1 0 1 2 low probability of anatomical connection small prior variance of effective connectivity parameter 1.6 1.4 1.2 1 R1 R2 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 Stephan, Tittgemeyer et al. 2009, NeuroImage -2 -1 0 1 high probability of anatomical connection large prior variance of effective connectivity parameter 2 Proof of concept study probabilistic tractography FG 34 6.5% FG left FG FG right 24 43.6% 13 15.7% LG left LG LG 12 34.2% LG right anatomical connectivity DCM connectionspecific priors for coupling parameters 6.5% v 0.0384 2 1.8 1 5 .7 % 1.6 v 0 .1 0 7 0 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 34.2% 43.6% v 0.5268 v 0.7746 0.4 Stephan, Tittgemeyer et al. 2009, NeuroImage 0.2 0 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 Connection-specific prior variance as a function of anatomical connection probability ij 0 m 1: a=-32,b=-32 m 2: a=-16,b=-32 m 3: a=-16,b=-28 m 4: a=-12,b=-32 m 5: a=-12,b=-28 m 6: a=-12,b=-24 m 7: a=-12,b=-20 m 8: a=-8,b=-32 m 9: a=-8,b=-28 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 10: a=-8,b=-24 m 11: a=-8,b=-20 m 12: a=-8,b=-16 m 13: a=-8,b=-12 m 14: a=-4,b=-32 m 15: a=-4,b=-28 m 16: a=-4,b=-24 m 17: a=-4,b=-20 m 18: a=-4,b=-16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 exp( ij ) 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 19: a=-4,b=-12 m 20: a=-4,b=-8 m 21: a=-4,b=-4 m 22: a=-4,b=0 m 23: a=-4,b=4 m 24: a=0,b=-32 m 25: a=0,b=-28 m 26: a=0,b=-24 m 27: a=0,b=-20 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 • 64 different mappings by systematic search across hyperparameters and 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 28: a=0,b=-16 m 29: a=0,b=-12 m 30: a=0,b=-8 m 31: a=0,b=-4 m 32: a=0,b=0 m 33: a=0,b=4 m 34: a=0,b=8 m 35: a=0,b=12 m 36: a=0,b=16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 37: a=0,b=20 m 38: a=0,b=24 m 39: a=0,b=28 m 40: a=0,b=32 m 41: a=4,b=-32 m 42: a=4,b=0 m 43: a=4,b=4 m 44: a=4,b=8 m 45: a=4,b=12 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 • yields anatomically informed (intuitive and counterintuitive) and uninformed priors 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 46: a=4,b=16 m 47: a=4,b=20 m 48: a=4,b=24 m 49: a=4,b=28 m 50: a=4,b=32 m 51: a=8,b=12 m 52: a=8,b=16 m 53: a=8,b=20 m 54: a=8,b=24 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 55: a=8,b=28 m 56: a=8,b=32 m 57: a=12,b=20 m 58: a=12,b=24 m 59: a=12,b=28 m 60: a=12,b=32 m 61: a=16,b=28 m 62: a=16,b=32 m 63 & m 64 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 log group Bayes factor 600 400 200 log group Bayes factor 0 0 10 20 30 model 40 50 60 0 10 20 30 model 40 50 60 40 50 60 700 695 690 685 680 post. model prob. 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 Models with anatomically informed priors (of an intuitive form) 0.2 0.1 0 0 10 20 30 model m 1: a=-32,b=-32 m 2: a=-16,b=-32 m 3: a=-16,b=-28 m 4: a=-12,b=-32 m 5: a=-12,b=-28 m 6: a=-12,b=-24 m 7: a=-12,b=-20 m 8: a=-8,b=-32 m 9: a=-8,b=-28 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 10: a=-8,b=-24 m 11: a=-8,b=-20 m 12: a=-8,b=-16 m 13: a=-8,b=-12 m 14: a=-4,b=-32 m 15: a=-4,b=-28 m 16: a=-4,b=-24 m 17: a=-4,b=-20 m 18: a=-4,b=-16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 19: a=-4,b=-12 m 20: a=-4,b=-8 m 21: a=-4,b=-4 m 22: a=-4,b=0 m 23: a=-4,b=4 m 24: a=0,b=-32 m 25: a=0,b=-28 m 26: a=0,b=-24 m 27: a=0,b=-20 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 28: a=0,b=-16 m 29: a=0,b=-12 m 30: a=0,b=-8 m 31: a=0,b=-4 m 32: a=0,b=0 m 33: a=0,b=4 m 34: a=0,b=8 m 35: a=0,b=12 m 36: a=0,b=16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 37: a=0,b=20 m 38: a=0,b=24 m 39: a=0,b=28 m 40: a=0,b=32 m 41: a=4,b=-32 m 42: a=4,b=0 m 43: a=4,b=4 m 44: a=4,b=8 m 45: a=4,b=12 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 46: a=4,b=16 m 47: a=4,b=20 m 48: a=4,b=24 m 49: a=4,b=28 m 50: a=4,b=32 m 51: a=8,b=12 m 52: a=8,b=16 m 53: a=8,b=20 m 54: a=8,b=24 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 55: a=8,b=28 m 56: a=8,b=32 m 57: a=12,b=20 m 58: a=12,b=24 m 59: a=12,b=28 m 60: a=12,b=32 m 61: a=16,b=28 m 62: a=16,b=32 m 63 & m 64 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 Models with anatomically informed priors (of an intuitive form) were clearly superior to anatomically uninformed ones: Bayes Factor >109 1 Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Extended DCM for fMRI: nonlinear, two-state, stochastic • Embedding computational models in DCMs • Integrating tractography and DCM • Applications of DCM to clinical questions Model-based predictions for single patients model structure BMS set of parameter estimates model-based decoding BMS: Parkison‘s disease and treatment Age-matched controls Rowe et al. 2010, NeuroImage PD patients on medication Selection of action modulates connections between PFC and SMA PD patients off medication DA-dependent functional disconnection of the SMA Model-based decoding by generative embedding step 1 — model inversion step 2 — kernel construction A B C measurements from an individual subject A subject-specific inverted generative model B C Brodersen et al. 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol. subject representation in the generative score space step 3 — support vector classification step 4 — interpretation jointly discriminative model parameters A→B A→C B→B B→C separating hyperplane fitted to discriminate between groups Discovering remote or “hidden” brain lesions Discovering remote or “hidden” brain lesions detect “down-stream” network changes altered synaptic coupling among non-lesioned regions Model-based decoding of disease status: mildly aphasic patients (N=11) vs. controls (N=26) Connectional fingerprints from a 6-region DCM of auditory areas during speech perception PT PT HG (A1) HG (A1) MGB MGB S S Brodersen et al. 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol. Model-based decoding of disease status: aphasic patients (N=11) vs. controls (N=26) Classification accuracy PT PT HG (A1) HG (A1) MGB auditory stimuli Brodersen et al. 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol. MGB Generative embedding using DCM Multivariate searchlight classification analysis Generative score space 0.4 0.4 0.3 0.3 0.2 0.2 0.1 0.1 -0.3 0 0 patients -0.35 controls -0.1 -0.5 -0.1 -0.5 0 Voxel (-42,-26,10) mm -0.15 -0.2 -0.25 -10 0 0.5 10 -0.15 -10 -0.4 -0.4 0 0 Voxel (-56,-20,10) mm 0.5 10 -0.2 L.HG L.HG generative embedding Voxel (64,-24,4) mm Voxel-based feature space -0.25 -0.3 -0.35 0.5 -0.4 -0.4 -0.2 -0.2 0 -0.5 L.MGB L.MGB 0 0 -0.5 0.5 0 R.HG L.HG Definition of ROIs Are regions of interest defined anatomically or functionally? anatomically A 1 ROI definition and n model inversions unbiased estimate functionally Functional contrasts Are the functional contrasts defined across all subjects or between groups? across subjects between groups B 1 ROI definition and n model inversions slightly optimistic estimate: voxel selection for training set and test set based on test data D 1 ROI definition and n model inversions highly optimistic estimate: voxel selection for training set and test set based on test data and test labels C Repeat n times: 1 ROI definition and n model inversions unbiased estimate E Repeat n times: 1 ROI definition and 1 model inversion slightly optimistic estimate: voxel selection for training set based on test data and test labels F Repeat n times: 1 ROI definition and n model inversions unbiased estimate Brodersen et al. 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol. Key methods papers: DCM for fMRI and BMS – part 1 • Brodersen KH, Schofield TM, Leff AP, Ong CS, Lomakina EI, Buhmann JM, Stephan KE (2011) Generative embedding for model-based classification of fMRI data. PLoS Computational Biology 7: e1002079. • Daunizeau J, David, O, Stephan KE (2011) Dynamic Causal Modelling: A critical review of the biophysical and statistical foundations. NeuroImage 58: 312-322. • Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W (2003) Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage 19:1273-1302. • Friston K, Stephan KE, Li B, Daunizeau J (2010) Generalised filtering. Mathematical Problems in Engineering 2010: 621670. • Friston KJ, Li B, Daunizeau J, Stephan KE (2011) Network discovery with DCM. NeuroImage 56: 1202–1221. • Friston K, Penny W (2011) Post hoc Bayesian model selection. Neuroimage 56: 2089-2099. • Kasess CH, Stephan KE, Weissenbacher A, Pezawas L, Moser E, Windischberger C (2010) Multi-Subject Analyses with Dynamic Causal Modeling. NeuroImage 49: 3065-3074. • Kiebel SJ, Kloppel S, Weiskopf N, Friston KJ (2007) Dynamic causal modeling: a generative model of slice timing in fMRI. NeuroImage 34:1487-1496. • Li B, Daunizeau J, Stephan KE, Penny WD, Friston KJ (2011). Stochastic DCM and generalised filtering. NeuroImage 58: 442-457 • Marreiros AC, Kiebel SJ, Friston KJ (2008) Dynamic causal modelling for fMRI: a two-state model. NeuroImage 39:269-278. • Penny WD, Stephan KE, Mechelli A, Friston KJ (2004a) Comparing dynamic causal models. NeuroImage 22:1157-1172. • Penny WD, Stephan KE, Mechelli A, Friston KJ (2004b) Modelling functional integration: a comparison of structural equation and dynamic causal models. NeuroImage 23 Suppl 1:S264-274. Key methods papers: DCM for fMRI and BMS – part 2 • Penny WD, Stephan KE, Daunizeau J, Joao M, Friston K, Schofield T, Leff AP (2010) Comparing Families of Dynamic Causal Models. PLoS Computational Biology 6: e1000709. • Penny WD (2012) Comparing dynamic causal models using AIC, BIC and free energy. Neuroimage 59: 319-330. • Stephan KE, Harrison LM, Penny WD, Friston KJ (2004) Biophysical models of fMRI responses. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:629-635. • Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Drysdale PM, Robinson PA, Friston KJ (2007) Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM. NeuroImage 38:387-401. • Stephan KE, Harrison LM, Kiebel SJ, David O, Penny WD, Friston KJ (2007) Dynamic causal models of neural system dynamics: current state and future extensions. J Biosci 32:129-144. • Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Drysdale PM, Robinson PA, Friston KJ (2007) Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM. NeuroImage 38:387-401. • Stephan KE, Kasper L, Harrison LM, Daunizeau J, den Ouden HE, Breakspear M, Friston KJ (2008) Nonlinear dynamic causal models for fMRI. NeuroImage 42:649-662. • Stephan KE, Penny WD, Daunizeau J, Moran RJ, Friston KJ (2009a) Bayesian model selection for group studies. NeuroImage 46:1004-1017. • Stephan KE, Tittgemeyer M, Knösche TR, Moran RJ, Friston KJ (2009b) Tractography-based priors for dynamic causal models. NeuroImage 47: 1628-1638. • Stephan KE, Penny WD, Moran RJ, den Ouden HEM, Daunizeau J, Friston KJ (2010) Ten simple rules for Dynamic Causal Modelling. NeuroImage 49: 3099-3109. Thank you